-
Hi. I'm going to talk
to you today about laughter,
-
and I just want to start
by thinking about the first time
-
I can ever remember noticing laughter.
-
This is when I was a little girl.
I would've been about six.
-
And I came across my parents
doing something unusual,
-
where they were laughing.
-
They were laughing very, very hard.
-
They were lying on the floor laughing.
-
They were screaming with laughter.
-
I did not know what they were
laughing at, but I wanted in.
-
I wanted to be part of that,
-
and I kind of sat around at the edge
going, "Hoo hoo!" (Laughter)
-
Now, incidentally,
what they were laughing at
-
was a song which people used to sing,
-
which was based around
signs in toilets on trains
-
telling you what you could
and could not do
-
in toilets on trains.
-
And the thing you have to remember
about the English is, of course,
-
we do have an immensely
sophisticated sense of humor.
-
(Laughter)
-
At the time, though, I didn't
understand anything of that.
-
I just cared about the laughter,
-
and actually, as a neuroscientist,
I've come to care about it again.
-
And it is a really weird thing to do.
-
What I'm going to do now
is just play some examples
-
of real human beings laughing,
-
and I want you think about the sound
people make and how odd that can be,
-
and in fact how primitive
laughter is as a sound.
-
It's much more like an animal call
than it is like speech.
-
So here we've got some laughter for you.
The first one is pretty joyful.
-
(Audio: Laughing)
-
Sophie Scott: Now this next guy,
I need him to breathe.
-
There's a point in there
where I'm just, like,
-
you've got to get some air in there, mate,
-
because he just sounds
like he's breathing out.
-
(Audio: Laughing)
-
This hasn't been edited; this is him.
-
(Audio: Laughing)
(Laughter)
-
And finally we have --
this is a human female laughing.
-
And laughter can take us to some pretty
odd places in terms of making noises.
-
(Audio: Laughing)
-
She actually says, "Oh my God,
what is that?" in French.
-
We're all kind of with her.
I have no idea.
-
Now, to understand laughter,
you have to look at a part of the body
-
that psychologists and neuroscientists
don't normally spend much time looking at,
-
which is the ribcage,
-
and it doesn't seem terribly exciting,
-
but actually you're all using
your ribcage all the time.
-
What you're all doing
at the moment with your ribcage,
-
and don't stop doing it, is breathing.
-
So you use the intercostal muscles,
the muscles between your ribs,
-
to bring air in and out of your lungs
-
just by expanding
and contracting your ribcage,
-
and if I was to put a strap
around the outside of your chest
-
called a breath belt, and just look
at that movement,
-
you see a rather gentle sinusoidal
movement, so that's breathing.
-
You're all doing it. Don't stop.
-
As soon as you start talking,
-
you start using your breathing
completely differently.
-
So what I'm doing now is you see
something much more like this.
-
In talking, you use very fine
movements of the ribcage
-
to squeeze the air out --
-
and in fact, we're the only animals
that can do this.
-
It's why we can talk at all.
-
Now, both talking and breathing
has a mortal enemy,
-
and that enemy is laughter,
-
because what happens when you laugh
-
is those same muscles
start to contract very regularly,
-
and you get this very marked
sort of zig-zagging,
-
and that's just squeezing
the air out of you.
-
It literally is that basic a way
of making a sound.
-
You could be stamping on somebody,
it's having the same effect.
-
You're just squeezing air out,
-
and each of those contractions --
Ha! -- gives you a sound.
-
And as the contractions run together,
you can get these spasms,
-
and that's when you start getting
these -- (Wheezing) -- things happening.
-
I'm brilliant at this. (Laughter)
-
Now, in terms of the science of laughter,
there isn't very much,
-
but it does turn out that pretty much
everything we think we know
-
about laughter is wrong.
-
So it's not at all unusual, for example,
to hear people to say
-
humans are the only animals that laugh.
-
Nietzsche thought that humans
are the only animals that laugh.
-
In fact, you find laughter
throughout the mammals.
-
It's been well-described
and well-observed in primates,
-
but you also see it in rats,
-
and wherever you find it --
-
humans, primates, rats --
-
you find it associated
with things like tickling.
-
That's the same for humans.
-
You find it associated with play,
and all mammals play.
-
And wherever you find it,
it's associated with interactions.
-
So Robert Provine, who has done
a lot of work on this,
-
has pointed out that you are 30 times
more likely to laugh
-
if you are with somebody else
than if you're on your own,
-
and where you find most laughter
-
is in social interactions
like conversation.
-
So if you ask human beings,
"When do you laugh?"
-
they'll talk about comedy and they'll talk
about humor and they'll talk about jokes.
-
If you look at when they laugh,
they're laughing with their friends.
-
And when we laugh with people, we're
hardly ever actually laughing at jokes.
-
You are laughing to show people
that you understand them,
-
that you agree with them, that you're
part of the same group as them.
-
You're laughing to show
that you like them.
-
You might even love them.
-
You're doing all that at the same time
as talking to them,
-
and the laughter is doing a lot
of that emotional work for you.
-
Something that Robert Provine
has pointed out, as you can see here,
-
and the reason why we were laughing
-
when we heard those
funny laughs at the start,
-
and why I was laughing
when I found my parents laughing,
-
is that it's an enormously
behaviorally contagious effect.
-
You can catch laughter from somebody else,
-
and you are more likely to catch laughter
off somebody else if you know them.
-
So it's still modulated
by this social context.
-
You have to put humor to one side
-
and think about the social
meaning of laughter
-
because that's where its origins lie.
-
Now, something I've got very interested in
is different kinds of laughter,
-
and we have some neurobiological evidence
about how human beings vocalize
-
that suggests there might be
two kinds of laughs that we have.
-
So it seems possible that the neurobiology
for helpless, involuntary laughter,
-
like my parents lying on the floor
screaming about a silly song,
-
might have a different basis to it
than some of that more polite
-
social laughter that you encounter,
which isn't horrible laughter,
-
but it's behavior somebody is doing
as part of their communicative act to you,
-
part of their interaction with you;
they are choosing to do this.
-
In our evolution, we have developed
two different ways of vocalizing.
-
Involuntary vocalizations
are part of an older system
-
than the more voluntary vocalizations
like the speech I'm doing now.
-
So we might imagine that laughter
might actually have two different roots.
-
So I've been looking at this
in more detail.
-
To do this, we've had to make
recordings of people laughing,
-
and we've had to do whatever it takes
to make people laugh,
-
and we got those same people
to produce more posed, social laughter.
-
So imagine your friend told a joke,
-
and you're laughing because
you like your friend,
-
but not really because
the joke's all that.
-
So I'm going to play you
a couple of those.
-
I want you to tell me if you think
this laughter is real laughter,
-
or if you think it's posed.
-
So is this involuntary laughter
or more voluntary laughter?
-
(Audio: Laughing)
-
What does that sound like to you?
-
Audience: Posed.
Sophie Scott: Posed? Posed.
-
How about this one?
-
(Audio laughter)
-
(Audience Laughter)
-
I'm the best.
-
(Laughter) (Applause)
-
Not really.
-
No, that was helpless laughter,
-
and in fact, to record that,
all they had to do was record me
-
watching one of my friends listening
to something I knew she wants to laugh at,
-
and I just started doing this.
-
What you find is that people
are good at telling the difference
-
between real and posed laughter.
-
They seem to be different things to us.
-
Interestingly, you see something
similar with chimpanzees.
-
Chimpanzees laugh differently
if they're being tickled
-
than if they're playing with each other,
-
and we might be seeing
something like that here,
-
involuntary laughter, tickling laughter,
being different from social laughter.
-
They're acoustically very different.
-
The real laughs are longer.
They're higher in pitch.
-
When you start laughing hard,
-
you start squeezing air out
from your lungs
-
under much higher pressures
than you could ever produce voluntarily.
-
For example, I could never
pitch my voice that high to sing.
-
Also, you start to get these sort of
contractions and weird whistling sounds,
-
all of which mean that real laughter
is extremely easy,
-
or feels extremely easy to spot.
-
In contrast, posed laughter,
we might think it sounds a bit fake.
-
Actually, it's not, it's actually
an important social cue.
-
We use it a lot, we're choosing
to laugh in a lot of situations,
-
and it seems to be its own thing.
-
So, for example, you find
nasality in posed laughter,
-
that kind of "ha ha ha ha ha" sound
-
that you never get, you could not do,
if you were laughing involuntarily.
-
So they do seem to be genuinely
these two different sorts of things.
-
We took it into the scanner
to see how brains respond
-
when you hear laughter.
-
And when you do this,
this is a really boring experiment.
-
We just played people
real and posed laughs.
-
We didn't tell them
it was a study on laughter.
-
We put other sounds in there
to distract them,
-
and all they're doing
is listening to sounds.
-
We don't tell them to do anything.
-
Nonetheless, when you hear real laughter
and when you hear posed laughter,
-
the brains are responding
completely differently,
-
significantly differently.
-
What you see in the regions in blue,
which lies in auditory cortex,
-
are the brain areas that respond
more to the real laughs,
-
and what seems to be the case,
-
when you hear somebody
laughing involuntarily,
-
you hear sounds you would never
hear in any other context.
-
It's very unambiguous,
-
and it seems to be associated
with greater auditory processing
-
of these novel sounds.
-
In contrast, when you hear somebody
laughing in a posed way,
-
what you see are the regions in pink,
-
which are occupying brain areas
associated with mentalizing,
-
thinking about what
somebody else is thinking.
-
And I think what that means is,
-
even if you're having your brain scanned,
which is completely boring
-
and not very interesting,
-
when you hear somebody going,
"A ha ha ha ha ha,"
-
you're trying to work out
why they're laughing.
-
Laughter is always meaningful.
-
You are always trying
to understand it in context,
-
even if, as far as you are concerned,
at that point in time,
-
it has not necessarily
anything to do with you,
-
you still want to know
why those people are laughing.
-
Now, we've had the opportunity to look
at how people hear real and posed laughter
-
across the age range.
-
So this is an online experiment
we ran with the Royal Society,
-
and here we just asked people
two questions.
-
First of all, they heard some laughs,
-
and they had to say, how real
or posed do these laughs sound?
-
The real laughs are shown in red
and the posed laughs are shown in blue.
-
What you see is there is a rapid onset.
-
As you get older, you get better
and better at spotting real laughter.
-
So six year olds are at chance,
they can't really hear the difference.
-
By the time you are older, you get better,
-
but interestingly, you do not hit
peak performance in this dataset
-
until you are in your
late 30s and early 40s.
-
You don't understand laughter fully
by the time you hit puberty.
-
You don't understand laughter fully
by the time your brain is matured
-
at the end of your teens.
-
You're learning about laughter
throughout your entire early adult life.
-
If we turn the question around and now say
not what does the laughter sound like
-
in terms of being real
or posed, but we say,
-
"How much does this laughter
make you want to laugh?
-
How contagious is this laughter to you?"
we see a different profile.
-
And here, the younger you are,
-
the more you want to join in
when you hear laughter.
-
Remember me laughing with my parents
when I had no idea what was going on.
-
You really can see this.
-
Now everybody, young and old,
finds the real laughs
-
more contagious than the posed laughs,
-
but as you get older, it all becomes
less contagious to you.
-
Now either we're all just becoming
really grumpy as we get older,
-
or it may mean that as you
understand laughter better,
-
and you are getting better at doing that,
-
you need more than just
hearing people laugh to want to laugh.
-
You need the social stuff there.
-
So we've got a very interesting behavior
-
about which a lot of our
lay assumptions are incorrect,
-
but I'm coming to see that actually
-
there's even more to laughter
than it's an important social emotion
-
we should look at, because it turns out
people are phenomenally nuanced
-
in terms of how we use laughter.
-
There's a really lovely set of studies
coming out from Robert Levenson's lab
-
in California, where he's doing
a longitudinal study with couples.
-
He gets married couples, men and women,
into the lab, and he gives them
-
stressful conversations to have
while he wires them up to a polygraph
-
so he can see them becoming stressed.
-
So you've got the two of them in there,
and he'll say to the husband,
-
"Tell me something that your wife does
that irritates you."
-
And what you see is immediately
-
-- just run that one through your head
briefly, you and your partner --
-
you can imagine everybody gets a bit
more stressed as soon as that starts.
-
You can see physically,
people become more stressed.
-
What he finds is that the couples
who manage that feeling of stress
-
with laughter,
positive emotions like laughter,
-
not only immediately become less stressed,
-
they can see them
physically feeling better,
-
they're dealing with this
unpleasant situation better together,
-
they are also the couples that report
-
high levels of satisfaction
in their relationship
-
and they stay together for longer.
-
So in fact, when you look
at close relationships,
-
laughter is a phenomenally useful index
of how people are regulating
-
their emotions together.
-
We're not just emitting at each other
to show that we like each other,
-
we're making ourselves
feel better together.
-
Now, I don't think this is going
to be limited to romantic relationships.
-
I think this is probably
going to be a characteristic
-
of close emotional relationships
such as you might have with friends,
-
which explains my next clip,
-
which is of a YouTube video of some
young men in the former East Germany
-
on making a video to promote
their heavy metal band,
-
and it's extremely macho,
and the mood is very serious,
-
and I want to notice what happens
in terms of laughter
-
when things go wrong
-
and how quickly that happens,
and how that changes the mood.
-
He's cold. He's about to get wet.
He's got swimming trunks on,
-
got a towel.
-
Ice.
-
What might possibly happen?
-
Video starts.
-
Serious mood.
-
And his friends are already laughing.
They are already laughing, hard.
-
He's not laughing yet.
-
(Laughter)
-
He's starting to go now.
-
And now they're all off.
-
(Laughter)
-
They're on the floor.
-
(Laughter)
-
The thing I really like about that
is it's all very kind of serious
-
until he jumps onto the ice, and
as soon as he doesn't go through the ice,
-
but also there isn't blood
and bone everywhere,
-
his friends start laughing,
-
and imagine that hadn't played him out
with him standing there, saying,
-
"No seriously, Heinrich,
I think this is broken,"
-
we wouldn't enjoy watching that.
That would be stressful.
-
Or if he was running around
with a visibly broken leg laughing,
-
and his friends are going, "Heinrich, I
think we need to go to the hospital now,"
-
that also wouldn't be funny.
-
The fact that the laughter works,
-
it gets him from a painful,
embarrassing, difficult situation,
-
into a funny situation, into what we're
actually enjoying there,
-
and I think that's
a really interesting use,
-
and it's actually happening all the time.
-
For example, I can remember
something like this happening
-
at my father's funeral.
-
We weren't jumping round
on the ice in our underpants.
-
We're not Canadian.
-
But -- (Laughter) (Applause)
-
These events are always difficult, I had
a relative who was being a bit difficult,
-
my mum was not in a good place,
-
and I can remember finding myself
just before the whole thing started
-
telling this story about something
that happened in a 1970s sitcom,
-
and I just thought,
I don't know why I'm doing this,
-
and what I realized I was doing
was I was coming up with something
-
from somewhere I could use
to make her laugh together with me.
-
It was a very basic reaction
to shine some reason we can do this.
-
We can laugh together.
We're going to get through this.
-
We're going to be okay.
-
And in fact, all of us
are doing this all the time.
-
You do it so often,
you don't even notice it.
-
Everybody underestimates
how often they laugh,
-
and you're doing something,
when you laugh with people,
-
that's actually letting you access
a really ancient evolutionary system
-
that mammals have evolved
to make and maintain social bonds,
-
and clearly to regulate emotions,
to make ourselves feel better.
-
It's not something specific to humans.
It's a really ancient behavior
-
which really helps us regulate how we feel
and makes us feel better.
-
In other words, when it comes to laughter,
-
you and me, baby,
ain't nothing but mammals.
-
Thank you.
-
Thank you. (Applause)