< Return to Video

Why we laugh

  • 0:01 - 0:04
    Hi. I'm going to talk to you
    today about laughter,
  • 0:04 - 0:08
    and I just want to start by thinking about
    the first time I can ever remember
  • 0:08 - 0:09
    noticing laughter.
  • 0:09 - 0:12
    This is when I was a little girl.
    I would've been about six,
  • 0:12 - 0:15
    and I came across my parents
    doing something unusual
  • 0:15 - 0:17
    where they were laughing.
  • 0:17 - 0:19
    They were laughing very, very hard.
  • 0:19 - 0:21
    They were lying on the floor laughing.
  • 0:21 - 0:23
    They were screaming with laughter.
  • 0:23 - 0:25
    I did not know what they were laughing at,
  • 0:25 - 0:27
    but I wanted in.
  • 0:27 - 0:29
    I wanted to be part of that,
  • 0:29 - 0:31
    and I kind of sat around
    at the edge going , "Hoo hoo!"
  • 0:31 - 0:35
    Now, incidentally,
    what they were laughing at
  • 0:35 - 0:38
    was a song which people used to sing
  • 0:38 - 0:41
    which was based around
    signs in toilets on trains
  • 0:41 - 0:44
    telling you what you could
    and could not do
  • 0:44 - 0:46
    in toilets on trains.
  • 0:46 - 0:48
    And the thing you have to remember
    about the English is, of course,
  • 0:48 - 0:51
    we do have an immensely
    sophisticated sense of humor.
  • 0:51 - 0:53
    (Laughter)
  • 0:53 - 0:55
    At the time, though, I didn't
    understand anything of that.
  • 0:55 - 0:57
    I just cared about the laughter,
  • 0:57 - 1:01
    and actually, as a neuroscientist,
    I've come to care about it again.
  • 1:01 - 1:03
    And it is a really weird thing to do.
  • 1:03 - 1:05
    What I'm going to do now
    is just play some examples
  • 1:05 - 1:07
    of real human beings laughing,
  • 1:07 - 1:11
    and I want you think about the sound
    people make and how odd that can be,
  • 1:11 - 1:13
    and in fact how primitive
    laughter is as a sound.
  • 1:13 - 1:16
    It's much more like an animal call
    than it is like speech.
  • 1:16 - 1:19
    So here we've got some laughter for you.
    The first one is pretty joyful.
  • 1:19 - 1:25
    (Video laughter)
  • 1:25 - 1:30
    (Laughter)
  • 1:36 - 1:39
    Now this next guy, I need him to breathe.
  • 1:39 - 1:40
    There's a point in there
    where I'm just, like,
  • 1:40 - 1:42
    you've got to get some air in there, mate,
  • 1:42 - 1:44
    because he just sounds
    like he's breathing out.
  • 1:44 - 1:53
    (Video laughter)
  • 1:53 - 1:56
    This hasn't been edited, this is him.
  • 1:56 - 2:03
    (Video laughter)
    (Laughter)
  • 2:03 - 2:06
    And finally we have this,
    the human female laughing.
  • 2:06 - 2:10
    And laughter can take us to some pretty
    odd places in terms of making noises.
  • 2:10 - 2:17
    (Video laughter)
  • 2:17 - 2:24
    (Laughter)
  • 2:24 - 2:33
    She actually says, "Oh my God,
    what is that?" in French.
  • 2:33 - 2:37
    We're all kind of with her.
    I have no idea.
  • 2:37 - 2:39
    Now, to understand laughter,
    you have to look at a part of the body
  • 2:39 - 2:43
    that psychologists and neuroscientists
    don't normally spend very much time
  • 2:43 - 2:44
    looking at, which is the ribcage,
  • 2:44 - 2:46
    and it doesn't seem terribly exciting,
  • 2:46 - 2:48
    but actually you're all
    using your ribcage all the time.
  • 2:48 - 2:50
    What you're all doing
    at the moment with your ribcage,
  • 2:50 - 2:52
    and don't stop doing it, is breathing.
  • 2:52 - 2:56
    So you use the [??] muscles,
    the muscles between your ribs,
  • 2:56 - 2:58
    to bring air in and out of your lungs
  • 2:58 - 3:00
    just by expanding
    and contracting your ribcage,
  • 3:00 - 3:02
    and if I was to put a strap
    around the outside of your chest,
  • 3:02 - 3:04
    it's called a breath belt,
    and just look at that movement,
  • 3:04 - 3:08
    you see a sort of rather gentle,
    sinusoidal movement, so that's breathing.
  • 3:08 - 3:10
    You're all doing it. Don't stop.
  • 3:10 - 3:12
    As soon as you start talking,
  • 3:12 - 3:14
    you start using your breathing
    completely differently.
  • 3:14 - 3:17
    So what I'm doing now is you see
    something much more like this.
  • 3:17 - 3:20
    In talking, you use a very fine
    movement of the ribcage
  • 3:20 - 3:22
    to squeeze the air out,
  • 3:22 - 3:23
    and in fact, we're the only animals
    that can do this.
  • 3:23 - 3:25
    It's why we can talk at all.
  • 3:25 - 3:27
    Now, both talking and breathing
  • 3:27 - 3:29
    has a mortal enemy,
  • 3:29 - 3:31
    and that enemy is laughter,
  • 3:31 - 3:34
    because what happens when you laugh
    is those same muscles
  • 3:34 - 3:37
    start to contract very regularly,
  • 3:37 - 3:37
    and you get this very marked
    sort of zig-zagging,
  • 3:39 - 3:40
    and that's just squeezing
    the air out of you.
  • 3:42 - 3:44
    It literally is that basic a way
    of making a sound.
  • 3:44 - 3:47
    You could be stamping on somebody.
    It's having the same effect.
  • 3:47 - 3:48
    You're just squeezing the air out.
  • 3:48 - 3:51
    Each of those contractions
    -- ha! -- gives you a sound.
  • 3:51 - 3:54
    And now if the contractions run together,
    you can get these spasms,
  • 3:54 - 3:57
    and that's when you start getting
    these "haaaaa" things happening.
  • 3:57 - 4:02
    I'm brilliant at this. (Laughter)
  • 4:02 - 4:06
    Now, in terms of the science of laughter,
    there isn't very much,
  • 4:06 - 4:09
    but it does turn out that pretty much
    everything we think we know
  • 4:09 - 4:11
    about laughter is wrong.
  • 4:11 - 4:14
    So it's not at all unusual, for example,
    to hear people to say,
  • 4:14 - 4:16
    "Humans are the only animals that laugh."
  • 4:16 - 4:18
    Nietzsche thought that humans
    are the only animals that laugh.
  • 4:18 - 4:20
    In fact, you find laughter
    throughout the mammals.
  • 4:20 - 4:23
    It's been well-described
    and well-observed in primates,
  • 4:23 - 4:25
    but you also see it in rats,
  • 4:25 - 4:27
    and wherever you find it
  • 4:27 - 4:29
    -- humans, primates, rats --
  • 4:29 - 4:32
    you find it associated
    with things like tickling.
  • 4:32 - 4:35
    That's the same for humans.
  • 4:35 - 4:40
    You find it associated with play,
    and all mammals play.
  • 4:40 - 4:44
    And wherever you find it,
    it's associated with interactions.
  • 4:44 - 4:47
    So Robert Provine, who has done
    a lot of work on this, has pointed out
  • 4:47 - 4:51
    that you are 30 times more likely to laugh
  • 4:51 - 4:54
    if you are with somebody else
    than if you're on your own,
  • 4:54 - 4:56
    and where you find most laughter
  • 4:56 - 4:59
    is in social interactions
    like conversation.
  • 4:59 - 5:02
    So if you ask human beings,
    "When do you laugh?"
  • 5:02 - 5:05
    they'll talk about comedy and they'll talk
    about humor and they'll talk about jokes.
  • 5:05 - 5:09
    If you look at when they laugh,
    they're laughing with their friends.
  • 5:09 - 5:11
    And when we laugh with people,
    we're hardly ever actually
  • 5:11 - 5:13
    laughing at jokes.
  • 5:13 - 5:15
    You are laughing to show people
    you understand them,
  • 5:15 - 5:18
    that you agree with them, that you're
    part of the same group as them.
  • 5:18 - 5:19
    You're laughing to show
    that you like them.
  • 5:19 - 5:20
    You might even love them.
  • 5:20 - 5:21
    You're doing all that at the same time
    as talking to them, and in fact,
  • Not Synced
    the laughter is doing a lot
    of that emotional work for you,
  • Not Synced
    something that Robert Provine
    has pointed out, as you can see here,
  • Not Synced
    and in fact, the reason
    why we were laughing
  • Not Synced
    when we heard those
    funny laughs at the start,
  • Not Synced
    and why I was laughing
    when I found my parents laughing,
  • Not Synced
    is that it's an enormously
    behaviorally contagious effect.
  • Not Synced
    You can catch laughter from somebody else,
    and you are more likely to catch laughter
  • Not Synced
    off somebody else if you know them.
  • Not Synced
    So it's still modulated
    by this social context.
  • Not Synced
    You have to put humor to one side,
  • Not Synced
    and think about
    the social meaning of laughter,
  • Not Synced
    because that's where its origins lie.
  • Not Synced
    Now, something I've got very interested in
    is different kinds of laughter,
  • Not Synced
    and we have some neurobiological evidence
    about how human beings vocalize
  • Not Synced
    that suggests there might be
    two kinds of laughs that we have.
  • Not Synced
    So it seems possible that the neurobiology
    for helpless, involuntary laughter,
  • Not Synced
    like my parents lying on the floor
    screaming about a silly song,
  • Not Synced
    might have a different basis to it
    than some of that more polite
  • Not Synced
    social laughter that you encounter,
    which isn't horrible laughter,
  • Not Synced
    but it's behavior somebody is doing
    as part of their communicative act to you,
  • Not Synced
    part of their interaction with you.
    They are choosing to do this.
  • Not Synced
    In our evolution, we have developed
    two different ways of vocalizing.
  • Not Synced
    involuntary vocalizations
    are part of an older system
  • Not Synced
    than the more voluntary vocalizations
    like the speech I'm doing now.
  • Not Synced
    So we might imagine that laughter
    might actually have two different roots.
  • Not Synced
    So I've been looking at this
    in more detail.
  • Not Synced
    To do this, we've had to make
    recordings of people laughing,
  • Not Synced
    and we've just had to do whatever it takes
    to make people laugh,
  • Not Synced
    and we got those same people
    to produce more posed, social laughter.
  • Not Synced
    So imagine your friend told a joke, and
    you're laughing 'cos you like your friend,
  • Not Synced
    but not really because
    the joke's all that.
  • Not Synced
    So I'm going to play you
    a couple of those.
  • Not Synced
    I want you to tell me if you think
    this laughter is real laughter,
  • Not Synced
    or if you think it's posed.
  • Not Synced
    So is this involuntary laughter
    or more voluntary laughter?
  • Not Synced
    (Audio laughter)
  • Not Synced
    What does that sound like to you?
  • Not Synced
    Posed? Posed.
  • Not Synced
    How about this one?
  • Not Synced
    (Audio laughter)
  • Not Synced
    (Laughter)
  • Not Synced
    I'm the best.
  • Not Synced
    (Laughter) (Applause)
  • Not Synced
    Not really.
  • Not Synced
    No, that was helpless laughter,
  • Not Synced
    and in fact, to record that,
    all they had to do was record me
  • Not Synced
    watching one of my friends listening
    to something I knew she wants to laugh at,
  • Not Synced
    and I just started doing this.
  • Not Synced
    Now, what you find is that people
    are good at telling the difference
  • Not Synced
    between real and posed laughter.
  • Not Synced
    They seem to be different things to us.
  • Not Synced
    Interestingly, you see something
    quite similar with chimpanzees.
  • Not Synced
    Chimpanzees laugh differently
    if they're being tickled
  • Not Synced
    than if they're playing with each other,
  • Not Synced
    and we might be seeing
    something like that here,
  • Not Synced
    these sort of involuntary laughter,
    tickling laughter,
  • Not Synced
    being different from social laughter.
  • Not Synced
    They're acoustically very different.
  • Not Synced
    The real laughs are longer.
    They're higher in pitch.
  • Not Synced
    When you start laughing hard,
  • Not Synced
    you start squeezing air out
    from your lungs
  • Not Synced
    under much higher pressures
    than you could ever produce voluntarily.
  • Not Synced
    So for example, I could never
    pitch my voice that high to sing.
  • Not Synced
    And also, as I stay, you start to get
    these sort of contractions
  • Not Synced
    and weird whistling sounds,
  • Not Synced
    all of which mean that real laughter
    is extremely easy,
  • Not Synced
    or feels extremely easy to spot.
  • Not Synced
    In contrast, posed laughter,
    we might think it sounds a bit fake.
  • Not Synced
    Actually, it's not. It's actually
    an important social cue.
  • Not Synced
    We use it a lot, we're choosing
    to laugh in a lot of situations,
  • Not Synced
    and it seems to be its own thing.
  • Not Synced
    So, for example, you find
    nasality in posed laughter,
  • Not Synced
    that kind of "ha ha ha ha ha" sound
  • Not Synced
    that you never get, you could not do,
    if you were laughing involuntarily.
  • Not Synced
    So they do seem to be genuinely
    these two different sorts of things.
  • Not Synced
    We took it into the scanner
    to see how brains respond
  • Not Synced
    when you hear laughter.
  • Not Synced
    And when you do this,
    this is a really boring experiment.
  • Not Synced
    We just played people
    real and posed laughs.
  • Not Synced
    We didn't tell them
    it was a study on laughter.
  • Not Synced
    We put other sounds in there
    so to distract them,
  • Not Synced
    and all they're doing
    is listening to sounds.
  • Not Synced
    We don't tell them to do anything.
  • Not Synced
    Nonetheless, when you hear real laughter
    and when you hear posed laughter,
  • Not Synced
    the brains are responding
    completely differently,
  • Not Synced
    significantly differently.
  • Not Synced
    What you see in the regions in blue,
    which lies in auditory cortex,
  • Not Synced
    are the brain areas that respond
    more to the real laughs,
  • Not Synced
    and what seems to be the case,
  • Not Synced
    when you hear somebody
    laughing involuntarily,
  • Not Synced
    you hear sounds you would never
    hear in any other context.
  • Not Synced
    It's very unambiguous,
  • Not Synced
    and it seems to be associated
    with greater auditory processing
  • Not Synced
    of these novel sounds.
  • Not Synced
    In contrast, when you hear somebody
    laughing in a posed way,
  • Not Synced
    what you see are the regions in pink,
  • Not Synced
    which are occupying brain areas
    associated with mentalizing,
  • Not Synced
    thinking about what
    somebody else is thinking.
  • Not Synced
    And I think what that means is,
  • Not Synced
    even if you're having your brain scanned,
    which is completely boring
  • Not Synced
    and not very interesting,
  • Not Synced
    when you hear somebody going,
    "A ha ha ha ha ha,"
  • Not Synced
    you're trying to work out
    why they're laughing.
  • Not Synced
    Laughter is always meaningful.
  • Not Synced
    You are always trying
    to understand it in context,
  • Not Synced
    even if, as far as you are concerned,
    at that point in time,
  • Not Synced
    it has not necessarily
    anything to do with you,
  • Not Synced
    you still want to know
    why those people are laughing.
  • Not Synced
    Now, we've had the opportunity to look at
    how people hear real and posed laughter
  • Not Synced
    across the age range.
  • Not Synced
    So this is an online experiment
    we ran with the Royal Society,
  • Not Synced
    and here we just
    asked people two questions.
  • Not Synced
    First of all, they heard some laughs,
    and they had to say
  • Not Synced
    how real or posed do these laughs sound?
  • Not Synced
    The real laughs are shown in red,
    and the posed laughs are shown in blue.
  • Not Synced
    What you see is there is a rapid onset.
  • Not Synced
    As you get older, you get better
    and better and better
  • Not Synced
    at spotting real laughter.
  • Not Synced
    So six year olds are at chance.
    They can't really hear the difference.
  • Not Synced
    By the time you are older, you get better,
  • Not Synced
    but interestingly, you do not hit
    peak performance in this dataset
  • Not Synced
    until you are in your
    late 30s and early 40s.
  • Not Synced
    You don't understand laughter fully
    by the time you hit puberty.
  • Not Synced
    You don't understand laughter fully
    by the time your brain is matured
  • Not Synced
    at the end of your teens.
  • Not Synced
    You're learning about laughter
    throughout your entire early adult life.
  • Not Synced
    If we turn the question around and now say
    not what does the laughter sound like
  • Not Synced
    in terms of being real
    or posed, but we say,
  • Not Synced
    "How much does this laughter
    make you want to laugh?
  • Not Synced
    How contagious is this laughter to you?"
    we see a different profile.
  • Not Synced
    And here, the younger you are,
  • Not Synced
    the more you want to join in
    when you hear laughter.
  • Not Synced
    Remember me laughing with my parents
    when I had no idea what was going on.
  • Not Synced
    You really can see this.
  • Not Synced
    Now everybody, young and old,
    finds the real laughs
  • Not Synced
    more contagious than the posed laughs,
  • Not Synced
    but as you get older,
  • Not Synced
    it all becomes less contagious to you.
  • Not Synced
    Now either we're all just becoming
    really grumpy as we get older,
  • Not Synced
    or it may mean that as you
    understand laughter better,
  • Not Synced
    and you are getting better at doing that,
  • Not Synced
    you need more than just
    hearing people laugh to want to laugh.
  • Not Synced
    You need the social stuff there.
  • Not Synced
    So we've got a very interesting behavior
  • Not Synced
    about which a lot of our
    lay assumptions are incorrect,
  • Not Synced
    but I'm coming to see that actually
  • Not Synced
    there's even more to laughter
    than it's an important social emotion
  • Not Synced
    we should look at, because it turns out
    people are phenomenally nuanced
  • Not Synced
    in terms of how we use laughter.
  • Not Synced
    There's a really lovely set of studies
    coming out from Robert Levenson's lab
  • Not Synced
    in California, where he's doing
    a longitudinal study with couples.
  • Not Synced
    He gets married couples, men and women,
    into the lab, and he gives them
  • Not Synced
    stressful conversations to have
    while he wires them up to a polygraph
  • Not Synced
    so he can see them becoming stressed.
  • Not Synced
    So you've got the two of them in there,
    and he'll say to the husband,
  • Not Synced
    "Tell me something that your wife does
    that irritates you."
  • Not Synced
    And what you see is immediately
  • Not Synced
    -- just run that one through your head
    briefly, you and your partner --
  • Not Synced
    you can imagine everybody gets a bit
    more stressed as soon as that starts.
  • Not Synced
    You can see physically,
    people become more stressed.
  • Not Synced
    What he finds is that the couples
    who manage that feeling of stress
  • Not Synced
    with laughter,
    positive emotions like laughter,
  • Not Synced
    not only immediately become less stressed,
  • Not Synced
    they can see them
    physically feeling better,
  • Not Synced
    they're dealing with this
    unpleasant situation better together,
  • Not Synced
    they are also the couples that report
  • Not Synced
    high levels of satisfaction
    in their relationship
  • Not Synced
    and they stay together for longer.
  • Not Synced
    So in fact, when you look
    at close relationships,
  • Not Synced
    laughter is a phenomenally useful index
    of how people are regulating
  • Not Synced
    their emotions together.
  • Not Synced
    We're not just emitting at each other
    to show that we like each other,
  • Not Synced
    we're making ourselves
    feel better together.
  • Not Synced
    Now, I don't think this is going
    to be limited to romantic relationships.
  • Not Synced
    I think this is probably
    going to be a characteristic
  • Not Synced
    of close emotional relationships
    such as you might have with friends,
  • Not Synced
    which explains my next clip,
  • Not Synced
    which is of a YouTube video of some
    young men in the former East Germany
  • Not Synced
    on making a video to promote
    their heavy metal band,
  • Not Synced
    and it's extremely macho,
    and the mood is very serious,
  • Not Synced
    and I want to notice what happens
    in terms of laughter
  • Not Synced
    when things go wrong
  • Not Synced
    and how quickly that happens,
  • Not Synced
    and how that changes the mood.
  • Not Synced
    He's cold. He's about to get wet.
    He's got swimming trunks on,
  • Not Synced
    got a towel.
  • Not Synced
    Ice.
  • Not Synced
    What might possibly happen?
  • Not Synced
    Video starts.
  • Not Synced
    Serious mood.
  • Not Synced
    And his friends are already laughing.
    They are already laughing, hard.
  • Not Synced
    He's not laughing yet.
  • Not Synced
    (Laughter)
  • Not Synced
    He's starting to go now.
  • Not Synced
    And now they're all off.
  • Not Synced
    (Laughter)
  • Not Synced
    They're on the floor.
  • Not Synced
    (Laughter)
  • Not Synced
    The thing I really like about that
    is it's all very kind of serious
  • Not Synced
    until he jumps onto the ice, and
    as soon as he doesn't go through the ice,
  • Not Synced
    but also there isn't blood
    and bone everywhere,
  • Not Synced
    his friends start laughing,
  • Not Synced
    and imagine that hadn't played him out
    with him standing there, saying,
  • Not Synced
    "No seriously, Heinrich,
    I think this is broken,"
  • Not Synced
    we wouldn't enjoy watching that.
    That would be stressful.
  • Not Synced
    Or if he was running around
    with a visibly broken leg laughing,
  • Not Synced
    and his friends are going, "Heinrich, I
    think we need to go to the hospital now,"
  • Not Synced
    again, that also wouldn't be funny.
  • Not Synced
    The fact that the laughter works,
  • Not Synced
    it gets him from a painful,
    embarrassing, difficult situation
  • Not Synced
    into a funny situation,
  • Not Synced
    into what we're actually enjoying there,
  • Not Synced
    and I think that's
    a really interesting use,
  • Not Synced
    and it's actually happening all the time.
  • Not Synced
    So for example, I can remember now
    something very much like this happening
  • Not Synced
    at my father's funeral.
  • Not Synced
    We weren't jumping round
    on the ice in our underpants.
  • Not Synced
    We're not Canadian.
  • Not Synced
    But -- (Laughter) (Applause)
  • Not Synced
    These events are always difficult. I had
    a relative who was being a bit difficult.
  • Not Synced
    My mum was not in a good place,
  • Not Synced
    and I can remember finding myself
    just before the whole thing started
  • Not Synced
    telling this story about something
    that happened in a 1970s sitcom,
  • Not Synced
    and I just though,
    I don't know why I'm doing this,
  • Not Synced
    and what I realized I was doing
    was I was coming up with something
  • Not Synced
    from somewhere I could use
    to make her laugh together with me.
  • Not Synced
    It was a very basic reaction
    to shine some reason we can do this.
  • Not Synced
    We can laugh together.
    We're going to get through this.
  • Not Synced
    We're going to be okay.
  • Not Synced
    And in fact, all of us
    are doing this all the time.
  • Not Synced
    You do it so often,
    you don't even notice it.
  • Not Synced
    Everybody underestimates
    how often they laugh,
  • Not Synced
    and you're doing something,
    when you laugh with people,
  • Not Synced
    that's actually letting you access
    a really ancient evolutionary system
  • Not Synced
    that mammals have evolved
    to make and maintain social bonds,
  • Not Synced
    and clearly to regulate emotions,
    to make ourselves feel better.
  • Not Synced
    It's not something specific to humans.
    It's a really ancient behavior
  • Not Synced
    which really helps us regulate how we feel
    and makes us feel better.
  • Not Synced
    In other words, when it comes to laughter,
  • Not Synced
    you and me, baby,
    ain't nothing but mammals.
  • Not Synced
    Thank you.
  • Not Synced
    Thank you. (Applause)
Title:
Why we laugh
Speaker:
Sophie Scott
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
17:04
Morton Bast edited English subtitles for Why we laugh
Morton Bast edited English subtitles for Why we laugh
Morton Bast edited English subtitles for Why we laugh
Morton Bast edited English subtitles for Why we laugh
Morton Bast approved English subtitles for Why we laugh
Morton Bast edited English subtitles for Why we laugh
Morton Bast edited English subtitles for Why we laugh
Morton Bast edited English subtitles for Why we laugh
Show all

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions