< Return to Video

How your brain hallucinates your conscious reality

  • 0:01 - 0:02
    Just over a year ago,
  • 0:03 - 0:05
    for the third time in my life,
    I ceased to exist.
  • 0:05 - 0:10
    I was having a small operation,
    and my brain was filling with anesthetic.
  • 0:11 - 0:14
    I remember a sense
    of detachment and falling apart
  • 0:14 - 0:15
    and a coldness.
  • 0:16 - 0:18
    And then I was back,
    drowsy and disoriented,
  • 0:18 - 0:20
    but definitely there.
  • 0:20 - 0:22
    Now, when you wake from a deep sleep,
  • 0:22 - 0:25
    you might feel confused about the time
    or anxious about oversleeping,
  • 0:25 - 0:28
    but there's always a basic sense
    of time having passed,
  • 0:28 - 0:30
    of a continuity between then and now.
  • 0:30 - 0:33
    Coming round from
    anesthesia is very different.
  • 0:33 - 0:35
    I could have been under
    for five minutes, five hours,
  • 0:35 - 0:37
    five years or even 50 years.
  • 0:37 - 0:38
    I simply wasn't there.
  • 0:38 - 0:39
    It was total oblivion.
  • 0:40 - 0:42
    Anesthesia --
    it's a modern kind of magic.
  • 0:42 - 0:45
    It turns people into objects,
  • 0:46 - 0:48
    and then, we hope, back again into people.
  • 0:48 - 0:49
    And in this process
  • 0:49 - 0:52
    is one of the greatest remaining
    mysteries in science and philosophy.
  • 0:52 - 0:54
    How does consciousness happen?
  • 0:54 - 0:56
    Somehow, within each of our brains,
  • 0:56 - 0:59
    the combined activity
    of many billions of neurons,
  • 0:59 - 1:02
    each one a tiny biological machine,
  • 1:02 - 1:04
    is generating a conscious experience.
  • 1:04 - 1:06
    And not just any conscious experience --
  • 1:06 - 1:08
    your conscious experience
    right here and right now.
  • 1:08 - 1:10
    How does this happen?
  • 1:11 - 1:13
    Answering this question is so important
  • 1:13 - 1:15
    because consciousness
    for each of us is all there is.
  • 1:16 - 1:18
    Without it there's no world,
  • 1:18 - 1:20
    there's no self,
  • 1:20 - 1:21
    there's nothing at all.
  • 1:21 - 1:23
    And when we suffer, we suffer consciously
  • 1:23 - 1:25
    whether it's through
    mental illness or pain.
  • 1:26 - 1:29
    And if we can experience
    joy and suffering,
  • 1:29 - 1:31
    what about other animals?
  • 1:31 - 1:33
    Might they be conscious, too?
  • 1:33 - 1:34
    Do they also have a sense of self?
  • 1:34 - 1:38
    And as computers get faster and smarter,
  • 1:38 - 1:40
    maybe there will come a point,
    maybe not too far away,
  • 1:40 - 1:43
    when my iPhone develops
    a sense of its own existence.
  • 1:43 - 1:47
    I actually think the prospects
    for a conscious AI are pretty remote.
  • 1:48 - 1:50
    And I think this because
    my research is telling me
  • 1:50 - 1:53
    that consciousness has less to do
    with pure intelligence
  • 1:53 - 1:58
    and more to do with our nature
    as living and breathing organisms.
  • 1:58 - 2:00
    Consciousness and intelligence
    are very different things.
  • 2:00 - 2:04
    You don't have to be smart to suffer,
    but you probably do have to be alive.
  • 2:05 - 2:07
    In the story I'm going to tell you,
  • 2:07 - 2:09
    our conscious experiences
    of the world around us,
  • 2:09 - 2:11
    and of ourselves within it,
  • 2:11 - 2:13
    are kinds of controlled hallucinations
  • 2:13 - 2:17
    that happen with, through
    and because of our living bodies.
  • 2:18 - 2:21
    Now, you might have heard
    that we know nothing
  • 2:21 - 2:24
    about how the brain and body
    give rise to consciousness.
  • 2:24 - 2:27
    Some people even say it's beyond
    the reach of science altogether.
  • 2:27 - 2:28
    But in fact,
  • 2:28 - 2:32
    the last 25 years have seen an explosion
    of scientific work in this area.
  • 2:33 - 2:35
    If you come to my lab
    at the University of Sussex,
  • 2:35 - 2:38
    you'll find scientists
    from all different disciplines
  • 2:39 - 2:41
    and sometimes even philosophers.
  • 2:42 - 2:46
    All of us together trying to understand
    how consciousness happens
  • 2:46 - 2:48
    and what happens when it goes wrong.
  • 2:48 - 2:50
    And the strategy is very simple.
  • 2:50 - 2:52
    I'd like you to think about consciousness
  • 2:52 - 2:54
    in the way that we've
    come to think about life.
  • 2:54 - 2:57
    At one time, people thought
    the property of being alive
  • 2:57 - 3:00
    could not be explained
    by physics and chemistry --
  • 3:00 - 3:02
    that life had to be
    more than just mechanism.
  • 3:02 - 3:04
    But people no longer think that.
  • 3:04 - 3:06
    As biologists got on with the job
  • 3:06 - 3:09
    of explaining the properties
    of living systems
  • 3:09 - 3:10
    in terms of physics and chemistry --
  • 3:10 - 3:14
    things like metabolism,
    reproduction, homeostasis --
  • 3:14 - 3:17
    the basic mystery of what life is
    started to fade away,
  • 3:17 - 3:20
    and people didn't propose
    any more magical solutions,
  • 3:20 - 3:22
    like a force of life or an élan vital.
  • 3:23 - 3:26
    So as with life, so with consciousness.
  • 3:26 - 3:28
    Once we start explaining its properties
  • 3:28 - 3:32
    in terms of things happening
    inside brains and bodies,
  • 3:32 - 3:35
    the apparently insoluble mystery
    of what consciousness is
  • 3:35 - 3:37
    should start to fade away.
  • 3:37 - 3:39
    At least that's the plan.
  • 3:39 - 3:41
    So let's get started.
  • 3:41 - 3:43
    What are the properties of consciousness?
  • 3:43 - 3:45
    What should a science
    of consciousness try to explain?
  • 3:46 - 3:50
    Well, for today I'd just like to think
    of consciousness in two different ways.
  • 3:50 - 3:52
    There are experiences
    of the world around us,
  • 3:52 - 3:54
    full of sights, sounds and smells,
  • 3:54 - 3:58
    there's multisensory, panoramic,
    3D, fully immersive inner movie.
  • 3:59 - 4:01
    And then there's conscious self.
  • 4:01 - 4:04
    The specific experience
    of being you or being me.
  • 4:04 - 4:06
    The lead character in this inner movie,
  • 4:06 - 4:09
    and probably the aspect of consciousness
    we all cling to most tightly.
  • 4:10 - 4:12
    Let's start with experiences
    of the world around us,
  • 4:12 - 4:16
    and with the important idea
    of the brain as a prediction engine.
  • 4:16 - 4:18
    Imagine being a brain.
  • 4:19 - 4:20
    You're locked inside a bony skull,
  • 4:20 - 4:22
    trying to figure
    what's out there in the world.
  • 4:23 - 4:26
    There's no lights inside the skull.
    There's no sound either.
  • 4:26 - 4:28
    All you've got to go on
    is streams of electrical impulses
  • 4:28 - 4:32
    which are only indirectly related
    to things in the world,
  • 4:32 - 4:33
    whatever they may be.
  • 4:34 - 4:36
    So perception --
    figuring out what's there --
  • 4:36 - 4:39
    has to be a process of informed guesswork
  • 4:39 - 4:42
    in which the brain combines
    these sensory signals
  • 4:42 - 4:46
    with its prior expectations or beliefs
    about the way the world is
  • 4:46 - 4:49
    to form its best guess
    of what caused those signals.
  • 4:49 - 4:52
    The brain doesn't hear sound or see light.
  • 4:52 - 4:56
    What we perceive is its best guess
    of what's out there in the world.
  • 4:57 - 5:00
    Let me give you a couple
    of examples of all this.
  • 5:00 - 5:02
    You might have seen this illusion before,
  • 5:02 - 5:05
    but I'd like you to think
    about it in a new way.
  • 5:05 - 5:07
    If you look at those two patches, A and B,
  • 5:07 - 5:10
    they should look to you to be
    very different shades of gray, right?
  • 5:11 - 5:14
    But they are in fact
    exactly the same shade.
  • 5:14 - 5:15
    And I can illustrate this.
  • 5:15 - 5:18
    If I put up a second version
    of the image here
  • 5:18 - 5:21
    and join the two patches
    with a gray-colored bar,
  • 5:21 - 5:22
    you can see there's no difference.
  • 5:22 - 5:24
    It's exactly the same shade of gray.
  • 5:24 - 5:26
    And if you still don't believe me,
  • 5:26 - 5:28
    I'll bring the bar across
    and join them up.
  • 5:29 - 5:32
    It's a single colored block of gray,
    there's no difference at all.
  • 5:33 - 5:34
    This isn't any kind of magic trick.
  • 5:34 - 5:36
    It's the same shade of gray,
  • 5:36 - 5:38
    but take it away again,
    and it looks different.
  • 5:39 - 5:40
    So what's happening here
  • 5:41 - 5:43
    is that the brain
    is using its prior expectations
  • 5:43 - 5:46
    built deeply into the circuits
    of the visual cortex
  • 5:46 - 5:49
    that a cast shadow dims
    the appearance of a surface,
  • 5:49 - 5:53
    so that we see B as lighter
    than it really is.
  • 5:54 - 5:55
    Here's one more example,
  • 5:55 - 5:58
    which shows just how quickly
    the brain can use new predictions
  • 5:58 - 6:01
    to change what we consciously experience.
  • 6:01 - 6:02
    Have a listen to this.
  • 6:03 - 6:06
    (Distorted voice)
  • 6:07 - 6:09
    Sounded strange, right?
  • 6:09 - 6:11
    Have a listen again
    and see if you can get anything.
  • 6:12 - 6:15
    (Distorted voice)
  • 6:16 - 6:17
    Still strange.
  • 6:17 - 6:18
    Now listen to this.
  • 6:19 - 6:22
    (Recording) Anil Seth: I think Brexit
    is a really terrible idea.
  • 6:22 - 6:23
    (Laughter)
  • 6:23 - 6:25
    Which I do.
  • 6:25 - 6:26
    So you heard some words there, right?
  • 6:26 - 6:30
    Now listen to the first sound again.
    I'm just going to replay it.
  • 6:30 - 6:33
    (Distorted voice)
  • 6:33 - 6:35
    Yeah? So you can now hear words there.
  • 6:35 - 6:36
    Once more for luck.
  • 6:37 - 6:40
    (Distorted voice)
  • 6:41 - 6:43
    OK, so what's going on here?
  • 6:43 - 6:47
    The remarkable thing is the sensory
    information coming into the brain
  • 6:47 - 6:48
    hasn't changed at all.
  • 6:49 - 6:51
    All that's changed
    is your brain's best guess
  • 6:51 - 6:53
    of the causes of that sensory information.
  • 6:53 - 6:56
    And that changes
    what you consciously hear.
  • 6:57 - 7:00
    All this puts the brain
    basis of perception
  • 7:00 - 7:01
    in a bit of a different light.
  • 7:01 - 7:05
    Instead of perception depending largely
    on signals coming into the brain
  • 7:05 - 7:07
    from the outside world,
  • 7:07 - 7:09
    it depends as much, if not more,
  • 7:09 - 7:13
    on perceptual predictions
    flowing in the opposite direction.
  • 7:14 - 7:16
    We don't just passively
    perceive the world,
  • 7:16 - 7:18
    we actively generate it.
  • 7:18 - 7:21
    The world we experience
    comes as much, if not more,
  • 7:21 - 7:22
    from the inside out
  • 7:22 - 7:23
    as from the outside in.
  • 7:24 - 7:26
    Let me give you
    one more example of perception
  • 7:26 - 7:29
    as this active, constructive process.
  • 7:29 - 7:35
    Here we've combined immersive
    virtual reality with image processing
  • 7:35 - 7:38
    to simulate the effects
    of overly strong perceptual predictions
  • 7:38 - 7:39
    on experience.
  • 7:39 - 7:42
    In this panoramic video,
    we've transformed the world --
  • 7:42 - 7:44
    which is in this case Sussex campus --
  • 7:44 - 7:45
    into a psychedelic playground.
  • 7:46 - 7:50
    We've processed the footage using
    an algorithm based on Google's Deep Dream
  • 7:50 - 7:54
    to simulate the effects
    of overly strong perceptual predictions.
  • 7:54 - 7:55
    In this case, to see dogs.
  • 7:55 - 7:58
    And you can see
    this is a very strange thing.
  • 7:58 - 8:00
    When perceptual
    predictions are too strong,
  • 8:00 - 8:01
    as they are here,
  • 8:01 - 8:04
    the result looks very much
    like the kinds of hallucinations
  • 8:04 - 8:06
    people might report in altered states,
  • 8:06 - 8:08
    or perhaps even in psychosis.
  • 8:09 - 8:11
    Now, think about this for a minute.
  • 8:11 - 8:16
    If hallucination is a kind
    of uncontrolled perception,
  • 8:16 - 8:21
    then perception right here and right now
    is also a kind of hallucination,
  • 8:21 - 8:23
    but a controlled hallucination
  • 8:23 - 8:26
    in which the brain's predictions
    are being reined in
  • 8:26 - 8:28
    by sensory information from the world.
  • 8:28 - 8:31
    In fact, we're all
    hallucinating all the time,
  • 8:31 - 8:33
    including right now.
  • 8:33 - 8:35
    It's just that when we agree
    about our hallucinations,
  • 8:35 - 8:37
    we call that reality.
  • 8:37 - 8:41
    (Laughter)
  • 8:41 - 8:45
    Now I'm going to tell you
    that your experience of being a self,
  • 8:45 - 8:46
    the specific experience of being you,
  • 8:46 - 8:50
    is also a controlled hallucination
    generated by the brain.
  • 8:50 - 8:52
    This seems a very strange idea, right?
  • 8:52 - 8:54
    Yes, visual illusions
    might deceive my eyes,
  • 8:54 - 8:58
    but how could I be deceived
    about what it means to be me?
  • 8:58 - 8:59
    For most of us,
  • 8:59 - 9:01
    the experience of being a person
  • 9:01 - 9:03
    is so familiar, so unified
    and so continuous
  • 9:03 - 9:05
    that it's difficult
    not to take it for granted.
  • 9:05 - 9:07
    But we shouldn't take it for granted.
  • 9:07 - 9:10
    There are in fact many different ways
    we experience being a self.
  • 9:10 - 9:13
    There's the experience of having a body
  • 9:13 - 9:14
    and of being a body.
  • 9:14 - 9:16
    There are experiences
    of perceiving the world
  • 9:16 - 9:18
    from a first person point of view.
  • 9:18 - 9:20
    There are experiences
    of intending to do things
  • 9:20 - 9:23
    and of being the cause of things
    that happen in the world.
  • 9:24 - 9:25
    And there are experiences
  • 9:25 - 9:29
    of being a continuous
    and distinctive person over time,
  • 9:29 - 9:32
    built from a rich set
    of memories and social interactions.
  • 9:32 - 9:33
    Many experiments show,
  • 9:34 - 9:36
    and psychiatrists
    and neurologists know very well,
  • 9:36 - 9:39
    that these different ways
    in which we experience being a self
  • 9:39 - 9:40
    can all come apart.
  • 9:41 - 9:43
    What this means is
    the basic background experience
  • 9:43 - 9:48
    of being a unified self is a rather
    fragile construction of the brain.
  • 9:48 - 9:51
    Another experience,
    which just like all others,
  • 9:51 - 9:52
    requires explanation.
  • 9:53 - 9:54
    So let's return to the bodily self.
  • 9:55 - 9:57
    How does the brain generate
    the experience of being a body
  • 9:57 - 9:59
    and of having a body?
  • 9:59 - 10:00
    Well, just the same principles apply.
  • 10:00 - 10:02
    The brain makes its best guess
  • 10:02 - 10:04
    about what is and what is not
    part of its body.
  • 10:05 - 10:09
    And there's a beautiful experiment
    in neuroscience to illustrate this.
  • 10:09 - 10:11
    And unlike most neuroscience experiments,
  • 10:11 - 10:12
    this is one you can do at home.
  • 10:12 - 10:14
    All you need is one of these.
  • 10:14 - 10:15
    (Laughter)
  • 10:15 - 10:17
    And a couple of paintbrushes.
  • 10:19 - 10:20
    In the rubber hand illusion,
  • 10:20 - 10:22
    a person's real hand is hidden from view,
  • 10:22 - 10:25
    and that fake rubber hand
    is placed in front of them.
  • 10:25 - 10:28
    Then both hands are simultaneously
    stroked with a paintbrush
  • 10:28 - 10:31
    while the person stares at the fake hand.
  • 10:31 - 10:33
    Now, for most people, after a while,
  • 10:33 - 10:35
    this leads to the very uncanny sensation
  • 10:35 - 10:38
    that the fake hand
    is in fact part of their body.
  • 10:40 - 10:44
    And the idea is that the congruence
    between seeing touch and feeling touch
  • 10:44 - 10:48
    on an object that looks like hand
    and is roughly where a hand should be,
  • 10:48 - 10:51
    is enough evidence for the brain
    to make its best guess
  • 10:51 - 10:54
    that the fake hand
    is in fact part of the body.
  • 10:54 - 10:57
    (Laughter)
  • 11:03 - 11:05
    So you can measure
    all kinds of clever things.
  • 11:06 - 11:09
    You can measure skin conductance
    and startle responses,
  • 11:09 - 11:10
    but there's no need.
  • 11:10 - 11:13
    It's clear the guy in blue
    has assimilated the fake hand.
  • 11:13 - 11:16
    This means that even experiences
    of what our body is
  • 11:17 - 11:18
    is a kind of best guessing --
  • 11:18 - 11:21
    a kind of controlled
    hallucination by the brain.
  • 11:21 - 11:23
    There's one more thing.
  • 11:24 - 11:28
    We don't just experience our bodies
    as objects in the world from the outside,
  • 11:28 - 11:30
    we also experience them from within.
  • 11:30 - 11:34
    We all experience the sense
    of being a body from the inside.
  • 11:35 - 11:38
    And sensory signals
    coming from the inside of the body
  • 11:38 - 11:42
    are continually telling the brain
    about the state of the internal organs,
  • 11:42 - 11:44
    how the heart is doing,
    what the blood pressure is like,
  • 11:44 - 11:46
    lots of things.
  • 11:46 - 11:48
    This kind of perception,
    which we call interoception,
  • 11:49 - 11:50
    is rather overlooked.
  • 11:50 - 11:52
    But it's critically important
  • 11:52 - 11:55
    because perception and regulation
    of the internal state of the body --
  • 11:55 - 11:57
    well, that's what keeps us alive.
  • 11:57 - 12:00
    Here's another version
    of the rubber hand illusion.
  • 12:00 - 12:01
    This is from our lab at Sussex.
  • 12:01 - 12:05
    And here, people see
    a virtual reality version of their hand,
  • 12:05 - 12:06
    which flashes red and back
  • 12:06 - 12:09
    either in time or out of time
    with their heartbeat.
  • 12:09 - 12:12
    And when it's flashing
    in time with their heartbeat,
  • 12:12 - 12:15
    people have a stronger sense
    that it's in fact part of their body.
  • 12:16 - 12:19
    So experiences of having a body
    are deeply grounded
  • 12:19 - 12:22
    in perceiving our bodies from within.
  • 12:24 - 12:26
    There's one last thing
    I want to draw your attention to,
  • 12:26 - 12:30
    which is that experiences of the body
    from the inside are very different
  • 12:30 - 12:32
    from experiences of the world around us.
  • 12:32 - 12:35
    When I look around me,
    the world seems full of objects --
  • 12:35 - 12:37
    tables, chairs, rubber hands,
  • 12:37 - 12:39
    people, you lot --
  • 12:39 - 12:41
    even my own body in the world,
  • 12:41 - 12:43
    I can perceive it
    as an object from the outside.
  • 12:43 - 12:45
    But my experiences
    of the body from within,
  • 12:45 - 12:47
    they're not like that at all.
  • 12:47 - 12:49
    I don't perceive my kidneys here,
  • 12:49 - 12:50
    my liver here,
  • 12:50 - 12:52
    my spleen ...
  • 12:52 - 12:53
    I don't know where my spleen is,
  • 12:53 - 12:55
    but it's somewhere.
  • 12:55 - 12:57
    I don't perceive my insides as objects.
  • 12:57 - 13:00
    In fact, I don't experience them
    much at all unless they go wrong.
  • 13:01 - 13:03
    And this is important, I think.
  • 13:04 - 13:06
    Perception of the internal
    state of the body
  • 13:06 - 13:08
    isn't about figuring out what's there,
  • 13:08 - 13:10
    it's about control and regulation --
  • 13:10 - 13:14
    keeping the physiological variables
    within the tight bounds
  • 13:14 - 13:16
    that are compatible with survival.
  • 13:17 - 13:19
    When the brain uses predictions
    to figure out what's there,
  • 13:20 - 13:23
    we perceive objects
    as the causes of sensations.
  • 13:23 - 13:26
    When the brain uses predictions
    to control and regulate things,
  • 13:26 - 13:29
    we experience how well
    or how badly that control is going.
  • 13:30 - 13:33
    So our most basic experiences
    of being a self,
  • 13:33 - 13:35
    of being an embodied organism,
  • 13:35 - 13:39
    are deeply grounded in the biological
    mechanisms that keep us alive.
  • 13:41 - 13:43
    And when we follow this idea
    all the way through,
  • 13:43 - 13:47
    we can start to see
    that all of our conscious experiences,
  • 13:47 - 13:52
    since they all depend on the same
    mechanisms of predictive perception,
  • 13:52 - 13:55
    all stem from this basic
    drive to stay alive.
  • 13:55 - 13:58
    We experience the world and ourselves
  • 13:58 - 14:01
    with, through and because of
    our living bodies.
  • 14:02 - 14:04
    Let me bring things together step-by-step.
  • 14:05 - 14:06
    What we consciously see depends
  • 14:06 - 14:08
    on the brain's best guess
    of what's out there.
  • 14:08 - 14:11
    Our experienced world
    comes from the inside out,
  • 14:11 - 14:12
    not just the outside in.
  • 14:13 - 14:16
    The rubber hand illusion shows
    that this applies to our experiences
  • 14:16 - 14:18
    of what is and what is not our body.
  • 14:19 - 14:22
    And these self-related predictions
    depend critically on sensory signals
  • 14:22 - 14:24
    coming from deep inside the body.
  • 14:24 - 14:26
    And finally,
  • 14:26 - 14:30
    experiences of being an embodied self
    are more about control and regulation
  • 14:30 - 14:32
    than figuring out what's there.
  • 14:33 - 14:36
    So our experiences of the world
    around us and ourselves within it --
  • 14:36 - 14:38
    well, they're kinds
    of controlled hallucinations
  • 14:38 - 14:41
    that have been shaped
    over millions of years of evolution
  • 14:41 - 14:44
    to keep us alive in worlds
    full of danger and opportunity.
  • 14:44 - 14:47
    We predict ourselves into existence.
  • 14:49 - 14:51
    Now, I leave you with three
    implications of all this.
  • 14:52 - 14:54
    First, just as we can
    misperceive the world,
  • 14:54 - 14:56
    we can misperceive ourselves
  • 14:56 - 14:58
    when the mechanisms
    of prediction go wrong.
  • 14:58 - 15:02
    Understanding this opens many new
    opportunities in psychiatry and neurology,
  • 15:02 - 15:05
    because we can finally
    get at the mechanisms
  • 15:05 - 15:07
    rather than just treating the symptoms
  • 15:07 - 15:09
    in conditions like
    depression and schizophrenia.
  • 15:10 - 15:11
    Second:
  • 15:11 - 15:15
    what it means to be me
    cannot be reduced to or uploaded to
  • 15:15 - 15:17
    a software program running on a robot,
  • 15:17 - 15:19
    however smart or sophisticated.
  • 15:19 - 15:22
    We are biological, flesh-and-blood animals
  • 15:22 - 15:25
    whose conscious experiences
    are shaped at all levels
  • 15:25 - 15:28
    by the biological mechanisms
    that keep us alive.
  • 15:28 - 15:32
    Just making computers smarter
    is not going to make them sentient.
  • 15:33 - 15:34
    Finally,
  • 15:34 - 15:36
    our own individual inner universe,
  • 15:36 - 15:38
    our way of being conscious,
  • 15:38 - 15:41
    is just one possible
    way of being conscious.
  • 15:42 - 15:44
    And even human consciousness generally --
  • 15:44 - 15:48
    it's just a tiny region in a vast space
    of possible consciousnesses.
  • 15:48 - 15:51
    Our individual self and worlds
    are unique to each of us,
  • 15:52 - 15:55
    but they're all grounded
    in biological mechanisms
  • 15:55 - 15:57
    shared with many other living creatures.
  • 15:58 - 16:01
    Now, these are fundamental changes
  • 16:02 - 16:04
    in how we understand ourselves,
  • 16:04 - 16:06
    but I think they should be celebrated,
  • 16:06 - 16:08
    because as so often in science,
    from Copernicus --
  • 16:08 - 16:10
    we're not at the center of the universe --
  • 16:10 - 16:12
    to Darwin --
  • 16:12 - 16:14
    we're related to all other creatures --
  • 16:14 - 16:15
    to the present day.
  • 16:16 - 16:19
    With a greater sense of understanding
  • 16:19 - 16:21
    comes a greater sense of wonder,
  • 16:22 - 16:23
    and a greater realization
  • 16:23 - 16:28
    that we are part of
    and not apart from the rest of nature.
  • 16:29 - 16:30
    And ...
  • 16:31 - 16:33
    when the end of consciousness comes,
  • 16:33 - 16:36
    there's nothing to be afraid of.
  • 16:37 - 16:38
    Nothing at all.
  • 16:38 - 16:40
    Thank you.
  • 16:40 - 16:48
    (Applause)
Title:
How your brain hallucinates your conscious reality
Speaker:
Anil Seth
Description:

Right now, billions of neurons in your brain are working together to generate a conscious experience -- and not just any conscious experience, your experience of the world around you and of yourself within it. How does this happen? According to neuroscientist Anil Seth, we're all hallucinating all the time; when we agree about our hallucinations, we call it "reality." Join Seth for a delightfully disorienting talk that may leave you questioning the very nature of your existence.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
17:00

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions