< Return to Video

The Secret Life of Pronouns: James Pennebaker at TEDxAustin

  • 0:10 - 0:14
    So some of the smallest
    most insignificant work
  • 0:14 - 0:18
    is everyday, cos reflect
    a lot about who we are.
  • 0:18 - 0:20
    And I say this not
    as a linguist,
  • 0:20 - 0:24
    or a computer scientist
    but as a social psychologist.
  • 0:24 - 0:26
    And today I'd like
    to tell you a story
  • 0:26 - 0:28
    that summarizes
    a lot of the research
  • 0:28 - 0:31
    that my colleagues,
    my students and I have done,
  • 0:31 - 0:35
    that have helped me
    to come to this realization.
  • 0:35 - 0:38
    Now several years ago I was studying
    the nature of traumatic experience
  • 0:38 - 0:41
    and how it is related
    to physical health
  • 0:41 - 0:45
    and kept finding
    that just completely perplexed me.
  • 0:45 - 0:49
    Basically when people have
    a major traumatic experience in their life,
  • 0:49 - 0:52
    they are much more likely
    to get sick after that event,
  • 0:52 - 0:54
    if they keep the events secret,
  • 0:54 - 1:02
    than if they actually
    talk to other people.
  • 1:02 - 1:07
    So, this really bugged me.
    So keeping a secret it seems
  • 1:07 - 1:09
    is somehow toxic.
  • 1:09 - 1:12
    So this led me
    to run some experiments
  • 1:12 - 1:14
    where we brought people
    in the laboratory
  • 1:14 - 1:17
    and we asked them to write about
  • 1:17 - 1:21
    the most traumatic experiences they've had,
    especially if they'd kept them secret.
  • 1:21 - 1:24
    And these were big traumas,
    these were things like rape.
  • 1:24 - 1:28
    They were like major public humiliations
    or failure.
  • 1:28 - 1:31
    And the results that we got
    from this this study were stunning.
  • 1:31 - 1:36
    We discovered that having people write
    as little as fifteen minutes a day,
  • 1:36 - 1:38
    for 3 or 4 consecutive days,
  • 1:38 - 1:41
    brought about meaningful changes
    in people's physical health
  • 1:41 - 1:44
    and even their immune function.
  • 1:44 - 1:51
    Translating up, saying experiences
    into words makes a difference, but why?
  • 1:51 - 1:55
    Since then there have been hundreds
    of studies done by labs all over the world
  • 1:55 - 2:00
    trying to answer this and they haven't
    come up with a single explanation.
  • 2:00 - 2:05
    My own approach was to actually look
    at the essays that these people wrote,
  • 2:05 - 2:07
    and try to figure out,
  • 2:07 - 2:09
    was there something about the essays
    that could predict
  • 2:09 - 2:12
    who would benefit
    from writing versus who wouldn't?
  • 2:12 - 2:14
    I tried and I couldn't figure it out.
  • 2:14 - 2:17
    So I got a number of psychologists
    and other experts
  • 2:17 - 2:20
    to read and write hundreds of these essays,
  • 2:20 - 2:23
    and they couldn't see a pattern either;
  • 2:23 - 2:25
    I needed to try some other strategies.
  • 2:25 - 2:28
    So, with the help of
    one of my graduate students,
  • 2:28 - 2:30
    Martha Francis,
    we wrote a computer program.
  • 2:30 - 2:34
    And the idea of this computer program
    was to go into any given text
  • 2:34 - 2:36
    and calculate the percentage
    of words in their texts
  • 2:36 - 2:40
    that were positive emotion words,
    negative emotion words
  • 2:40 - 2:47
    or words related to topics such as death
    or sex or violence or religion or family.
  • 2:47 - 2:49
    And as long as we were writing
    the computer program,
  • 2:49 - 2:52
    I thought well let's go ahead and
    throw in some parts of speech,
  • 2:52 - 2:57
    pronouns, prepositions.
    Why? Because it was easy, who cares?
  • 2:57 - 3:01
    So, I go back, start to analyze
    these traumatic essays,
  • 3:01 - 3:06
    and quickly discover that the content
    of what people were writing about
  • 3:06 - 3:09
    didn't matter in terms of
    if they improved in their health or not,
  • 3:09 - 3:15
    instead, it was these junk words, pronouns,
    articles, prepositions and so forth
  • 3:15 - 3:19
    that did matter.
    Now think about this.
  • 3:19 - 3:23
    Here people are writing about
    deeply disturbing issues
  • 3:23 - 3:29
    and the actual topics that dealt with
    tragedies, devastation, horrible things,
  • 3:29 - 3:32
    the topics themselves
    and the words associated
  • 3:32 - 3:34
    with those topics made no difference.
  • 3:34 - 3:39
    Instead these little words
    like "I" and "the" and "and" did matter.
  • 3:39 - 3:42
    I'd been looking for the obvious,
    but in fact
  • 3:42 - 3:45
    I'd been paying attention
    to what people were saying,
  • 3:45 - 3:48
    but not how they were saying it.
  • 3:48 - 3:52
    So how do I go about
    analyzing what verus how?
  • 3:52 - 3:54
    Well, it turns out that they're
    different kinds of classes of words
  • 3:54 - 3:58
    that look at this distinction,
    and one of them is
  • 3:58 - 4:00
    if you're looking at
    what people are writing about,
  • 4:00 - 4:05
    you look at what are called content words.
    These are nouns and regular verbs
  • 4:05 - 4:08
    and adjectives and some adverbs.
    These are the stuff of thought,
  • 4:08 - 4:12
    these were the stuff of communication.
    We were trying to talk to somebody.
  • 4:12 - 4:17
    Google and search terms
    are all based on these content words.
  • 4:17 - 4:19
    The other group of words
    are a class of words
  • 4:19 - 4:22
    that are generally called
    function words.
  • 4:22 - 4:27
    And function words are made up
    of the most boring words you can imagine.
  • 4:27 - 4:31
    They're made up of pronouns:
    "I", "me", "he", "she";
  • 4:31 - 4:38
    prepositions: "to", "of", "for";
    auxillary verbs: "am", "is", "have" â
  • 4:38 - 4:42
    I'll have to wake you up if I keep talking
    about these function words.
  • 4:42 - 4:47
    But it turns out these
    function words are really interesting,
  • 4:47 - 4:52
    because, first of all, there's only about
    five hundred function words in English,
  • 4:52 - 4:57
    so they account for far less than 1%
    of all the words we know, we hear, we read.
  • 4:57 - 5:04
    Nevertheless, they reflect 55% to 60% of
    all the words that we are surrounded with,
  • 5:04 - 5:07
    they're everywhere,
    but we don't pay attention to them.
  • 5:07 - 5:10
    In English and in other languages,
    they're the shortest words there are,
  • 5:10 - 5:13
    and when they're spoken
    or when you're reading,
  • 5:13 - 5:17
    they zip into your brain
    at the speed of less than 0.2 seconds,
  • 5:17 - 5:21
    meaning that they're processed
    essentially non-consciously.
  • 5:21 - 5:23
    But there's something
    even more interesting about them,
  • 5:23 - 5:27
    they are social,
    they are profoundly social.
  • 5:27 - 5:29
    Let me give me an example,
    let's say you're walking along,
  • 5:29 - 5:32
    you see a note on the ground,
    you pick it up and it says,
  • 5:32 - 5:36
    "I am placing it on the table."
  • 5:36 - 5:38
    Well, that kinda makes sense,
    kinda doesn't.
  • 5:38 - 5:42
    "I'm placing it on the table" â there's
    2 content words, "placing" and "table";
  • 5:42 - 5:44
    all the rest are function words:
  • 5:44 - 5:48
    "I", "am", "it", "on", "the".
  • 5:48 - 5:51
    Now the reason this doesn't
    make sense to most of us is
  • 5:51 - 5:53
    who was "I"?
    No idea.
  • 5:53 - 5:57
    "Am" implies present tense.
    When was it written?
  • 5:57 - 5:59
    "It"? Pfft, no idea what "it" is.
  • 5:59 - 6:03
    "On the table", "the table"
    means it's a table
  • 6:03 - 6:05
    that the author knew about
  • 6:05 - 6:08
    and the intended recipient
    of this note knew about,
  • 6:08 - 6:14
    but nobody else did. And, in fact,
    this note only has meaning to the author
  • 6:14 - 6:16
    and the recipient of the note
  • 6:16 - 6:19
    at a particular time,
    in a particular location.
  • 6:19 - 6:24
    And, in fact, if I took that note
    to this author 6 months later and say,
  • 6:24 - 6:27
    "What's this all about?" there's
    a good chance that the author will say,
  • 6:27 - 6:30
    "No idea."
  • 6:30 - 6:33
    Function words are social,
  • 6:33 - 6:36
    they tell us about the author,
    they tell us about the relationship
  • 6:36 - 6:38
    between the author and the recipient
  • 6:38 - 6:42
    and the relationship between
    the author and the topic itself.
  • 6:42 - 6:46
    And this is the heart of what
    I want to talk to you about today.
  • 6:46 - 6:50
    By analyzing function words
    we start to get a sense
  • 6:50 - 6:52
    of who people are,
    what their relationships are,
  • 6:52 - 6:57
    how they think about theirselves
    and how they connect with others.
  • 6:57 - 6:59
    Yeah, there're a lot of function words,
  • 6:59 - 7:03
    and honestly I could talk
    for several hours about function words,
  • 7:03 - 7:07
    but I'm going to spare you that
    and just focus on a couple today
  • 7:07 - 7:11
    to just give you a flavor
    of why they're so interesting.
  • 7:11 - 7:14
    Let's start off with pronouns, and
    let's start off with third-person pronouns
  • 7:14 - 7:17
    like "he", "she", "they".
  • 7:17 - 7:19
    Now it turns out some people
    out there in the world
  • 7:19 - 7:23
    use these third-person pronouns
    at high rates
  • 7:23 - 7:25
    and other people at low rates.
  • 7:25 - 7:28
    What kind of person would use them?
  • 7:28 - 7:31
    Well, you have to think about pronouns
    and all function words
  • 7:31 - 7:36
    in terms of
    where are people paying attention.
  • 7:36 - 7:38
    If you are using
    these third-person pronouns,
  • 7:38 - 7:41
    by definition you're paying attention
    to other people.
  • 7:41 - 7:44
    You care about other people,
    you're thinking about other people,
  • 7:44 - 7:46
    and people who use these at high rates
  • 7:46 - 7:48
    are much more socially engaged.
  • 7:48 - 7:50
    We can analyze emails,
    tweets and so forth
  • 7:50 - 7:54
    and get a sense of someone's
    social engagement just by looking at this.
  • 7:54 - 7:59
    How about first-person singular pronouns,
    "I", "me" and "my"?
  • 7:59 - 8:02
    OK, using the attentional arguments
  • 8:02 - 8:05
    someone who's attending
    to their thoughts, feelings,
  • 8:05 - 8:08
    behaviors, to themselves in some way
    would use these words more.
  • 8:08 - 8:12
    What kind of person
    do you think uses "I" words the most?
  • 8:12 - 8:16
    I hope, you're sitting there,
    you're thinking, "Well, somebody who's
  • 8:16 - 8:18
    self-centered, self-important,
    narcissistic,
  • 8:18 - 8:24
    hungry for power and high in status."
  • 8:24 - 8:27
    You would be completely wrong.
  • 8:27 - 8:34
    In fact the person who is highest
    in status uses "I" words the least.
  • 8:34 - 8:35
    Let me rephrase that,
  • 8:35 - 8:39
    the higher anybody is in status,
    the less they use "I" words;
  • 8:39 - 8:44
    the lower someone is in status,
    the higher they use a "I" words.
  • 8:44 - 8:48
    Now, I discovered this
    by analyzing emails,
  • 8:48 - 8:53
    instant messages, natural conversations,
    business groups and so forth.
  • 8:53 - 8:57
    And the affects were huge.
    I looked at these results and I thought,
  • 8:57 - 9:03
    "Wow, this must be true for other people
    but it can't possibly be true for me."
  • 9:03 - 9:09
    You know I love everybody equally.
    So I go in and analyze my own emails.
  • 9:09 - 9:11
    I'm the same as everybody else,
  • 9:11 - 9:15
    I look at the email that I get
    from an undergraduate student,
  • 9:15 - 9:19
    "Dear Dr Pennebaker, I would like to know
    if I could possibly meet with you
  • 9:19 - 9:21
    because I think I need to change my grade."
  • 9:21 - 9:25
    And I write back, "Dear Student,
    Thank you so much for your email.
  • 9:25 - 9:30
    Unfortunately, the way
    the grade systems work, blah, blah, blah."
  • 9:30 - 9:32
    I look at my email to the dean.
  • 9:32 - 9:35
    "Dear Dean, I'm Jamie Pennebaker
    and I would like to ask you
  • 9:35 - 9:37
    if I could do this and if I could do that
    and I could do this."
  • 9:37 - 9:40
    And the dean writes back,
    "Dear Jamie,
  • 9:40 - 9:44
    Thank you so much for your email..."
    and so forth.
  • 9:44 - 9:49
    Now everybody is being completely polite,
    nobody's putting anybody down.
  • 9:49 - 9:52
    This is the language of power in status;
  • 9:52 - 9:55
    it tells us where people
    are paying attention.
  • 9:55 - 9:57
    A high status person is
    looking out at the world,
  • 9:57 - 10:01
    the low status person tends
    to be looking more inwardly.
  • 10:01 - 10:02
    What about others states?
  • 10:02 - 10:07
    Let's move beyond status,
    let's look at emotional states.
  • 10:07 - 10:11
    You would think that someone would be
    paying more attention to themselves
  • 10:11 - 10:16
    if they're in pain. It could be
    physical pain or emotional pain.
  • 10:16 - 10:19
    In fact, if we look at people
    who are depressed,
  • 10:19 - 10:22
    we've done many studies on this,
    we know that people who are depressed
  • 10:22 - 10:26
    pay attention to themselves more
    and they used the word "I" more.
  • 10:26 - 10:28
    In fact one of our very first studies
    looked at the poetry
  • 10:28 - 10:31
    of suicidal and non-suicidal poets.
  • 10:31 - 10:35
    Now we did this research where
    we went through, analyzed their poetry,
  • 10:35 - 10:37
    and initially I thought,
    where the big difference
  • 10:37 - 10:40
    in the degree to which
    they use negative and emotional words.
  • 10:40 - 10:42
    Not true.
  • 10:42 - 10:47
    Suicidal and non-suicidal poets all use
    negative emotion words at high rate.
  • 10:47 - 10:52
    I think it's part of the job description.
  • 10:52 - 10:55
    The big difference was
    their use of the word "I",
  • 10:55 - 10:58
    suicidal poets use the word "I" more.
  • 10:58 - 11:03
    Consider this poem, this is by Sylvia Plath
    who later committed suicide.
  • 11:03 - 11:08
    Listen to the way that she uses
    the word "I" and first-person singular.
  • 11:08 - 11:14
    I'm taking some lines from her poem
    "Mad Girl's Love Song".
  • 11:14 - 11:17
    I shut my eyes and
    all the world drops dead;
  • 11:17 - 11:21
    I lift my lid[s] and all is born again.
  • 11:21 - 11:24
    (I think I made you up inside my head.)
  • 11:24 - 11:28
    I fancied you'd return the way you said,
    But I grow old and I forget your name.
  • 11:28 - 11:33
    (I think I made you up inside my head.)
  • 11:33 - 11:36
    You can almost see Plath
  • 11:36 - 11:39
    embracing her sorrow,
    her misery and so forth
  • 11:39 - 11:44
    and you can compare her writing
    with other poets, non-suicidal poets
  • 11:44 - 11:46
    who write about lost love.
  • 11:46 - 11:50
    When they do, you can almost see them
    holding it off from a distance,
  • 11:50 - 11:55
    so they're looking at it from
    a more distance, third-person perspective.
  • 11:55 - 12:00
    Now there's a really interesting,
    important theory within psychology
  • 12:00 - 12:03
    about depression.
    And people who are depressed
  • 12:03 - 12:07
    are thought to be people who are
    very high in self-awareness or self-focus.
  • 12:07 - 12:12
    And part of this is
    they also tend to be extremely honest.
  • 12:12 - 12:20
    In fact there are many studies showing
    that they have this deficit
  • 12:20 - 12:23
    and they're not able to have
    positive illusions about ourselves.
  • 12:23 - 12:26
    Those of us who aren't depressed
    get by every day
  • 12:26 - 12:29
    by holding these insane illusions
    about the life.
  • 12:29 - 12:32
    But these people are brutally honest.
  • 12:32 - 12:36
    Now this made me wonder,
    throw away depression for just a second.
  • 12:36 - 12:40
    Could we turn this entire thing upside down
    and find out if depressed people
  • 12:40 - 12:45
    or if we could use a computer program
    as a linguistic lie-detector.
  • 12:45 - 12:48
    I mean for anybody.
    So in fact we did some studies,
  • 12:48 - 12:51
    where we brought people in the lab,
    we induced them to lie or tell the truth,
  • 12:51 - 12:54
    we analyze court transcripts of people
    who were all found guilty,
  • 12:54 - 12:57
    the half of whom were later exonerated,
  • 12:57 - 13:00
    and the affects
    were really quite impressive.
  • 13:00 - 13:02
    We did a pretty good job at telling
  • 13:02 - 13:04
    if someone who was telling
    the truth versus lying,
  • 13:04 - 13:07
    and one of the best words
    was the use of the word "I".
  • 13:07 - 13:12
    People who tell the truth use the word
    "I" more, owning what they're saying.
  • 13:12 - 13:18
    Liars are tending to hold off,
    distancing themselves.
  • 13:18 - 13:24
    Now, lie-detection and depression, status
    are all some things that we can look at,
  • 13:24 - 13:28
    But one of the things that I'm most
    interested in now is looking at groups,
  • 13:28 - 13:30
    looking at the relationship
    between two people.
  • 13:30 - 13:32
    Can you tell how two people
    are getting along
  • 13:32 - 13:37
    by analyzing the way that they're
    using function words with each other?
  • 13:37 - 13:40
    And the answer is yes. We look
    at the percentage of each class of words
  • 13:40 - 13:45
    and we come up with the metric
    that we call language style matching.
  • 13:45 - 13:48
    And the more that two people are matching
    in their function word use,
  • 13:48 - 13:50
    the more they're on the same page,
  • 13:50 - 13:53
    the more they're talking
    about something in the same way.
  • 13:53 - 13:57
    Now one place we started to look at this
    was with speed dating.
  • 13:57 - 14:05
    Now, I should tell you I love speed dating,
    I would never do it in a million years,
  • 14:05 - 14:09
    but I encourage all of you
    to go do speed dating and when you do,
  • 14:09 - 14:15
    invite a researcher along because
    there is no paradigm that is better.
  • 14:15 - 14:17
    We've been involved
    in speed-dating projects
  • 14:17 - 14:22
    where people come in and in
    their four minute date, we tape-record it,
  • 14:22 - 14:27
    they know we are,
    and then we transcribe the way they talk.
  • 14:27 - 14:29
    The more they match
    in their language,
  • 14:29 - 14:31
    the more likely they are
    to go out on a date.
  • 14:31 - 14:33
    We can predict
    who will go on a date
  • 14:33 - 14:38
    at rates slightly better than
    the people themselves can.
  • 14:38 - 14:40
    We've done studies
    with young dating couples.
  • 14:40 - 14:47
    To be in our study they had to give us
    10 days of their instant messages or IM's.
  • 14:47 - 14:52
    And then what we do is we analyze
    their IM's with this style matching
  • 14:52 - 14:54
    and we do much, much better
    than they do
  • 14:54 - 14:58
    at predicting if they'll be
    together 3 months later.
  • 14:58 - 15:01
    The fact is, is these words
    are telling us how individuals
  • 15:01 - 15:04
    and pairs of people are connecting.
  • 15:04 - 15:07
    What about groups. Now this is an area
    that we're now working at.
  • 15:07 - 15:10
    We're looking at working groups,
    some are groups that we've worked with,
  • 15:10 - 15:14
    people from the business school, we look
    at people in the get-to-know-you groups,
  • 15:14 - 15:17
    we do educational groups.
    And what we're finding is
  • 15:17 - 15:21
    by looking at a group of say 5 or 6 people,
    we can now get a sense
  • 15:21 - 15:26
    of how productive the group will be,
    and also how cohesive the group wil be,
  • 15:26 - 15:28
    simply by looking at the style matching.
  • 15:28 - 15:31
    Now here's where things are
    beginning to get interesting:
  • 15:31 - 15:36
    by tracking a group that's interacting
    and say they're all interacting online,
  • 15:36 - 15:40
    we can have a computer monitoring
    how the group is behaving.
  • 15:40 - 15:46
    Imagine for example you are in this group
    and a computer coach comes to your group
  • 15:46 - 15:50
    every now and then,
    and a message comes and says,
  • 15:50 - 15:53
    "You guys are not paying attention
    to one another,
  • 15:53 - 15:57
    you need to be more attentive to what
    the other people are saying," or
  • 15:57 - 16:00
    "You guys for the last few minutes
    have strayed off topic,
  • 16:00 - 16:04
    try to get back on topic,"
    or that loud mouth in the group,
  • 16:04 - 16:08
    the computer comes in and says,
    "John, for the last 5 minutes
  • 16:08 - 16:11
    you said 50% of the words,
    why don't you stand back
  • 16:11 - 16:14
    and courage others to talk?"
  • 16:14 - 16:17
    Well, we have now created
    a program that does this,
  • 16:17 - 16:21
    and we've now tested it out
    with hundreds of groups
  • 16:21 - 16:25
    and we are getting very promising results.
  • 16:25 - 16:30
    Now you can start to see why I'm so excited
    about this world of function words,
  • 16:30 - 16:34
    that we're now taking this
    in all these directions,
  • 16:34 - 16:36
    that I never would've thought about.
  • 16:36 - 16:40
    We've been looking at it in terms
    of looking at historical records.
  • 16:40 - 16:44
    Can you tell if a particular explorer
    committed suicide or was murdered?
  • 16:44 - 16:46
    We've done a project on that.
  • 16:46 - 16:48
    Can you look at a company and get a sense
  • 16:48 - 16:51
    of how their internal communications
    are working?
  • 16:51 - 16:55
    How well they are connecting with the people
    in their company or with their clients?
  • 16:55 - 17:00
    We look at corporate earnings reports
    or the quarterly phone calls
  • 17:00 - 17:03
    to get a sense of the internal
    group dynamics of the company.
  • 17:03 - 17:06
    We've worked with the government
    to try to get a sense of terroist groups
  • 17:06 - 17:10
    and if they are likely to behave badly.
  • 17:10 - 17:15
    We've helped people sort out
    their love lives.
  • 17:15 - 17:20
    You can start to see that by harnessing
    the power of these function words
  • 17:20 - 17:26
    We can get a sense of individuals
    and groups and how people are connecting.
  • 17:26 - 17:32
    Now what I would urge you to do,
    I'd like you to go home tonight
  • 17:32 - 17:35
    and I want you to start looking
    at your emails,
  • 17:35 - 17:38
    your tweets, your IM's or whatever,
  • 17:38 - 17:41
    and in doing that what I hope
    you start to see,
  • 17:41 - 17:45
    first of all you learn a little bit more
    about your relationships with others,
  • 17:45 - 17:50
    but more than anything I hope
    you learned a little bit about yourself.
  • 17:50 - 17:51
    Thank you very much.
  • 17:51 - 17:56
    (Applause)
Title:
The Secret Life of Pronouns: James Pennebaker at TEDxAustin
Description:

I, You, Me, We, Us -- small words with the tremendous ability to illuminate who we are and how we're feeling.

Chair of the Department of Psychology at one of the largest universities in the country, Jamie delves into our use of language and how it can reflect -- and reshape -- our understanding of ourselves, our interactions with others and our underlying feelings of strength and empowerment.

In the spirit of ideas worth spreading, TEDx is a program of local, self-organized events that bring people together to share a TED-like experience. At a TEDx event, TEDTalks video and live speakers combine to spark deep discussion and connection in a small group. These local, self-organized events are branded TEDx, where x = independently organized TED event. The TED Conference provides general guidance for the TEDx program, but individual TEDx events are self-organized.* (*Subject to certain rules and regulations)

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDxTalks
Duration:
17:59

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions