Your words may predict your future mental health
-
0:01 - 0:06We have historical records that allow us
to know how the ancient Greeks dressed, -
0:06 - 0:07how they lived,
-
0:07 - 0:09how they fought ...
-
0:09 - 0:11but how did they think?
-
0:11 - 0:16One natural idea is that the deepest
aspects of human thought -- -
0:16 - 0:18our ability to imagine,
-
0:18 - 0:19to be conscious,
-
0:19 - 0:20to dream --
-
0:20 - 0:22have always been the same.
-
0:23 - 0:24Another possibility
-
0:24 - 0:28is that the social transformations
that have shaped our culture -
0:28 - 0:32may have also changed
the structural columns of human thought. -
0:33 - 0:35We may all have different
opinions about this. -
0:35 - 0:38Actually, it's a long-standing
philosophical debate. -
0:39 - 0:41But is this question
even amenable to science? -
0:43 - 0:45Here I'd like to propose
-
0:45 - 0:50that in the same way we can reconstruct
how the ancient Greek cities looked -
0:50 - 0:53just based on a few bricks,
-
0:53 - 0:57that the writings of a culture
are the archaeological records, -
0:57 - 0:59the fossils, of human thought.
-
1:00 - 1:01And in fact,
-
1:01 - 1:03doing some form of psychological analysis
-
1:03 - 1:07of some of the most ancient
books of human culture, -
1:07 - 1:13Julian Jaynes came up in the '70s
with a very wild and radical hypothesis: -
1:13 - 1:15that only 3,000 years ago,
-
1:15 - 1:20humans were what today
we would call schizophrenics. -
1:22 - 1:23And he made this claim
-
1:23 - 1:27based on the fact that the first
humans described in these books -
1:27 - 1:29behaved consistently,
-
1:29 - 1:32in different traditions
and in different places of the world, -
1:32 - 1:35as if they were hearing and obeying voices
-
1:35 - 1:38that they perceived
as coming from the Gods, -
1:38 - 1:39or from the muses ...
-
1:40 - 1:43what today we would call hallucinations.
-
1:44 - 1:47And only then, as time went on,
-
1:47 - 1:50they began to recognize
that they were the creators, -
1:50 - 1:53the owners of these inner voices.
-
1:53 - 1:56And with this, they gained introspection:
-
1:56 - 1:59the ability to think
about their own thoughts. -
2:00 - 2:03So Jaynes's theory is that consciousness,
-
2:03 - 2:06at least in the way we perceive it today,
-
2:06 - 2:10where we feel that we are the pilots
of our own existence -- -
2:10 - 2:13is a quite recent cultural development.
-
2:13 - 2:15And this theory is quite spectacular,
-
2:15 - 2:17but it has an obvious problem
-
2:17 - 2:21which is that it's built on just a few
and very specific examples. -
2:21 - 2:23So the question is whether the theory
-
2:23 - 2:28that introspection built up in human
history only about 3,000 years ago -
2:28 - 2:31can be examined in a quantitative
and objective manner. -
2:32 - 2:35And the problem of how
to go about this is quite obvious. -
2:35 - 2:39It's not like Plato woke up one day
and then he wrote, -
2:39 - 2:40"Hello, I'm Plato,
-
2:40 - 2:43and as of today, I have
a fully introspective consciousness." -
2:43 - 2:46(Laughter)
-
2:46 - 2:49And this tells us actually
what is the essence of the problem. -
2:49 - 2:54We need to find the emergence
of a concept that's never said. -
2:54 - 2:59The word introspection
does not appear a single time -
2:59 - 3:01in the books we want to analyze.
-
3:02 - 3:06So our way to solve this
is to build the space of words. -
3:07 - 3:10This is a huge space
that contains all words -
3:10 - 3:13in such a way that the distance
between any two of them -
3:13 - 3:16is indicative of how
closely related they are. -
3:16 - 3:18So for instance,
-
3:18 - 3:21you want the words "dog" and "cat"
to be very close together, -
3:21 - 3:24but the words "grapefruit" and "logarithm"
to be very far away. -
3:25 - 3:29And this has to be true
for any two words within the space. -
3:30 - 3:33And there are different ways
that we can construct the space of words. -
3:33 - 3:35One is just asking the experts,
-
3:35 - 3:37a bit like we do with dictionaries.
-
3:37 - 3:38Another possibility
-
3:38 - 3:42is following the simple assumption
that when two words are related, -
3:42 - 3:44they tend to appear in the same sentences,
-
3:44 - 3:46in the same paragraphs,
-
3:46 - 3:48in the same documents,
-
3:48 - 3:51more often than would be expected
just by pure chance. -
3:52 - 3:54And this simple hypothesis,
-
3:54 - 3:56this simple method,
-
3:56 - 3:57with some computational tricks
-
3:57 - 3:59that have to do with the fact
-
3:59 - 4:02that this is a very complex
and high-dimensional space, -
4:02 - 4:03turns out to be quite effective.
-
4:04 - 4:07And just to give you a flavor
of how well this works, -
4:07 - 4:11this is the result we get when
we analyze this for some familiar words. -
4:12 - 4:13And you can see first
-
4:13 - 4:16that words automatically organize
into semantic neighborhoods. -
4:16 - 4:18So you get the fruits, the body parts,
-
4:18 - 4:21the computer parts,
the scientific terms and so on. -
4:21 - 4:25The algorithm also identifies
that we organize concepts in a hierarchy. -
4:26 - 4:27So for instance,
-
4:27 - 4:31you can see that the scientific terms
break down into two subcategories -
4:31 - 4:33of the astronomic and the physics terms.
-
4:33 - 4:36And then there are very fine things.
-
4:36 - 4:38For instance, the word astronomy,
-
4:38 - 4:39which seems a bit bizarre where it is,
-
4:39 - 4:41is actually exactly where it should be,
-
4:41 - 4:43between what it is,
-
4:43 - 4:44an actual science,
-
4:44 - 4:46and between what it describes,
-
4:46 - 4:47the astronomical terms.
-
4:48 - 4:50And we could go on and on with this.
-
4:50 - 4:52Actually, if you stare
at this for a while, -
4:52 - 4:54and you just build random trajectories,
-
4:54 - 4:57you will see that it actually feels
a bit like doing poetry. -
4:58 - 5:00And this is because, in a way,
-
5:00 - 5:03walking in this space
is like walking in the mind. -
5:04 - 5:06And the last thing
-
5:06 - 5:10is that this algorithm also identifies
what are our intuitions, -
5:10 - 5:14of which words should lead
in the neighborhood of introspection. -
5:14 - 5:15So for instance,
-
5:15 - 5:19words such as "self," "guilt,"
"reason," "emotion," -
5:19 - 5:21are very close to "introspection,"
-
5:21 - 5:22but other words,
-
5:22 - 5:24such as "red," "football,"
"candle," "banana," -
5:24 - 5:26are just very far away.
-
5:26 - 5:29And so once we've built the space,
-
5:29 - 5:32the question of the history
of introspection, -
5:32 - 5:34or of the history of any concept
-
5:34 - 5:39which before could seem abstract
and somehow vague, -
5:39 - 5:40becomes concrete --
-
5:40 - 5:43becomes amenable to quantitative science.
-
5:44 - 5:47All that we have to do is take the books,
-
5:47 - 5:48we digitize them,
-
5:48 - 5:51and we take this stream
of words as a trajectory -
5:51 - 5:53and project them into the space,
-
5:53 - 5:57and then we ask whether this trajectory
spends significant time -
5:57 - 6:00circling closely to the concept
of introspection. -
6:01 - 6:02And with this,
-
6:02 - 6:04we could analyze
the history of introspection -
6:04 - 6:06in the ancient Greek tradition,
-
6:06 - 6:09for which we have the best
available written record. -
6:10 - 6:12So what we did is we took all the books --
-
6:12 - 6:14we just ordered them by time --
-
6:14 - 6:16for each book we take the words
-
6:16 - 6:18and we project them to the space,
-
6:18 - 6:21and then we ask for each word
how close it is to introspection, -
6:21 - 6:22and we just average that.
-
6:23 - 6:26And then we ask whether,
as time goes on and on, -
6:26 - 6:29these books get closer,
and closer and closer -
6:29 - 6:31to the concept of introspection.
-
6:31 - 6:35And this is exactly what happens
in the ancient Greek tradition. -
6:36 - 6:39So you can see that for the oldest books
in the Homeric tradition, -
6:39 - 6:42there is a small increase with books
getting closer to introspection. -
6:42 - 6:44But about four centuries before Christ,
-
6:45 - 6:49this starts ramping up very rapidly
to an almost five-fold increase -
6:49 - 6:52of books getting closer,
and closer and closer -
6:52 - 6:53to the concept of introspection.
-
6:54 - 6:57And one of the nice things about this
-
6:57 - 6:58is that now we can ask
-
6:58 - 7:02whether this is also true
in a different, independent tradition. -
7:03 - 7:06So we just ran this same analysis
on the Judeo-Christian tradition, -
7:06 - 7:09and we got virtually the same pattern.
-
7:10 - 7:14Again, you see a small increase
for the oldest books in the Old Testament, -
7:14 - 7:16and then it increases much more rapidly
-
7:16 - 7:18in the new books of the New Testament.
-
7:18 - 7:20And then we get the peak of introspection
-
7:20 - 7:22in "The Confessions of Saint Augustine,"
-
7:22 - 7:24about four centuries after Christ.
-
7:25 - 7:27And this was very important,
-
7:27 - 7:30because Saint Augustine
had been recognized by scholars, -
7:30 - 7:32philologists, historians,
-
7:32 - 7:35as one of the founders of introspection.
-
7:35 - 7:38Actually, some believe him to be
the father of modern psychology. -
7:39 - 7:41So our algorithm,
-
7:41 - 7:44which has the virtue
of being quantitative, -
7:44 - 7:45of being objective,
-
7:45 - 7:47and of course of being extremely fast --
-
7:47 - 7:49it just runs in a fraction of a second --
-
7:49 - 7:53can capture some of the most
important conclusions -
7:53 - 7:55of this long tradition of investigation.
-
7:56 - 8:00And this is in a way
one of the beauties of science, -
8:00 - 8:03which is that now this idea
can be translated -
8:03 - 8:06and generalized to a whole lot
of different domains. -
8:07 - 8:12So in the same way that we asked
about the past of human consciousness, -
8:12 - 8:15maybe the most challenging question
we can pose to ourselves -
8:15 - 8:19is whether this can tell us something
about the future of our own consciousness. -
8:20 - 8:21To put it more precisely,
-
8:21 - 8:23whether the words we say today
-
8:23 - 8:29can tell us something
of where our minds will be in a few days, -
8:29 - 8:30in a few months
-
8:30 - 8:31or a few years from now.
-
8:32 - 8:35And in the same way many of us
are now wearing sensors -
8:35 - 8:36that detect our heart rate,
-
8:36 - 8:38our respiration,
-
8:38 - 8:39our genes,
-
8:39 - 8:43on the hopes that this may
help us prevent diseases, -
8:43 - 8:47we can ask whether monitoring
and analyzing the words we speak, -
8:47 - 8:49we tweet, we email, we write,
-
8:49 - 8:54can tell us ahead of time whether
something may go wrong with our minds. -
8:55 - 8:57And with Guillermo Cecchi,
-
8:57 - 9:00who has been my brother in this adventure,
-
9:00 - 9:01we took on this task.
-
9:02 - 9:08And we did so by analyzing
the recorded speech of 34 young people -
9:08 - 9:11who were at a high risk
of developing schizophrenia. -
9:11 - 9:14And so what we did is,
we measured speech at day one, -
9:14 - 9:18and then we asked whether the properties
of the speech could predict, -
9:18 - 9:20within a window of almost three years,
-
9:20 - 9:22the future development of psychosis.
-
9:23 - 9:26But despite our hopes,
-
9:26 - 9:29we got failure after failure.
-
9:30 - 9:34There was just not enough
information in semantics -
9:34 - 9:36to predict the future
organization of the mind. -
9:37 - 9:38It was good enough
-
9:38 - 9:43to distinguish between a group
of schizophrenics and a control group, -
9:43 - 9:45a bit like we had done
for the ancient texts, -
9:45 - 9:48but not to predict the future
onset of psychosis. -
9:49 - 9:51But then we realized
-
9:51 - 9:55that maybe the most important thing
was not so much what they were saying, -
9:55 - 9:57but how they were saying it.
-
9:58 - 9:59More specifically,
-
9:59 - 10:02it was not in which semantic
neighborhoods the words were, -
10:02 - 10:04but how far and fast they jumped
-
10:04 - 10:07from one semantic neighborhood
to the other one. -
10:07 - 10:09And so we came up with this measure,
-
10:09 - 10:11which we termed semantic coherence,
-
10:11 - 10:16which essentially measures the persistence
of speech within one semantic topic, -
10:16 - 10:18within one semantic category.
-
10:19 - 10:23And it turned out to be
that for this group of 34 people, -
10:23 - 10:27the algorithm based on semantic
coherence could predict, -
10:27 - 10:30with 100 percent accuracy,
-
10:30 - 10:32who developed psychosis and who will not.
-
10:33 - 10:36And this was something
that could not be achieved -- -
10:36 - 10:37not even close --
-
10:37 - 10:41with all the other
existing clinical measures. -
10:43 - 10:46And I remember vividly,
while I was working on this, -
10:46 - 10:48I was sitting at my computer
-
10:48 - 10:51and I saw a bunch of tweets by Polo --
-
10:51 - 10:54Polo had been my first student
back in Buenos Aires, -
10:54 - 10:56and at the time
he was living in New York. -
10:56 - 10:59And there was something in this tweets --
-
10:59 - 11:02I could not tell exactly what
because nothing was said explicitly -- -
11:02 - 11:04but I got this strong hunch,
-
11:04 - 11:07this strong intuition,
that something was going wrong. -
11:08 - 11:11So I picked up the phone,
and I called Polo, -
11:11 - 11:13and in fact he was not feeling well.
-
11:13 - 11:15And this simple fact,
-
11:15 - 11:18that reading in between the lines,
-
11:18 - 11:22I could sense,
through words, his feelings, -
11:22 - 11:25was a simple, but very
effective way to help. -
11:26 - 11:28What I tell you today
-
11:28 - 11:30is that we're getting
close to understanding -
11:30 - 11:34how we can convert this intuition
that we all have, -
11:34 - 11:36that we all share,
-
11:36 - 11:37into an algorithm.
-
11:38 - 11:40And in doing so,
-
11:40 - 11:44we may be seeing in the future
a very different form of mental health, -
11:44 - 11:50based on objective, quantitative
and automated analysis -
11:50 - 11:52of the words we write,
-
11:52 - 11:53of the words we say.
-
11:53 - 11:54Gracias.
-
11:54 - 12:01(Applause)
- Title:
- Your words may predict your future mental health
- Speaker:
- Mariano Sigman
- Description:
-
Can the way you speak and write today predict your future mental state, even the onset of psychosis? In this fascinating talk, neuroscientist Mariano Sigman reflects on ancient Greece and the origins of introspection to investigate how our words hint at our inner lives and details a word-mapping algorithm that could predict the development of schizophrenia. "We may be seeing in the future a very different form of mental health," Sigman says, "based on objective, quantitative and automated analysis of the words we write, of the words we say."
- Video Language:
- English
- Team:
closed TED
- Project:
- TEDTalks
- Duration:
- 12:14
![]() |
Brian Greene edited English subtitles for Your words may predict your future mental health | |
![]() |
Brian Greene edited English subtitles for Your words may predict your future mental health | |
![]() |
Brian Greene edited English subtitles for Your words may predict your future mental health | |
![]() |
Brian Greene approved English subtitles for Your words may predict your future mental health | |
![]() |
Brian Greene edited English subtitles for Your words may predict your future mental health | |
![]() |
Brian Greene edited English subtitles for Your words may predict your future mental health | |
![]() |
Brian Greene edited English subtitles for Your words may predict your future mental health | |
![]() |
Brian Greene edited English subtitles for Your words may predict your future mental health |