< Return to Video

What happens in your brain when you pay attention?

  • 0:01 - 0:03
    Paying close attention to something:
  • 0:03 - 0:05
    not that easy, is it?
  • 0:06 - 0:11
    It's because our attention is pulled
    in so many different directions at a time,
  • 0:11 - 0:15
    and it's in fact pretty impressive
    if you can stay focused.
  • 0:16 - 0:20
    Many people think that attention
    is all about what we are focusing on,
  • 0:20 - 0:25
    but it's also about what information
    our brain is trying to filter out.
  • 0:26 - 0:29
    There are two ways
    you direct your attention.
  • 0:30 - 0:31
    First, there's overt attention.
  • 0:32 - 0:36
    In overt attention,
    you move your eyes towards something
  • 0:36 - 0:37
    in order to pay attention to it.
  • 0:38 - 0:40
    Then there's covert attention.
  • 0:40 - 0:44
    In covert attention,
    you pay attention to something,
  • 0:44 - 0:46
    but without moving your eyes.
  • 0:47 - 0:49
    Think of driving for a second.
  • 0:51 - 0:54
    Your overt attention,
    your direction of the eyes,
  • 0:54 - 0:56
    are in front,
  • 0:56 - 0:57
    but that's your covert attention
  • 0:57 - 1:01
    which is constantly scanning
    the surrounding area,
  • 1:02 - 1:03
    where you don't actually look at them.
  • 1:06 - 1:07
    I'm a computational neuroscientist,
  • 1:07 - 1:11
    and I work on cognitive
    brain machine interfaces,
  • 1:11 - 1:14
    or bringing together
    the brain and the computer.
  • 1:15 - 1:16
    I love brain patterns.
  • 1:17 - 1:18
    Brain patterns are important for us
  • 1:18 - 1:22
    because based on them
    we can build models for the computers,
  • 1:22 - 1:23
    and based on these models
  • 1:23 - 1:28
    computers can recognize
    how well our brain functions,
  • 1:28 - 1:29
    and if it doesn't function well,
  • 1:30 - 1:34
    then these computers themselves
    can be used as assistive devices
  • 1:35 - 1:36
    for therapies.
  • 1:36 - 1:38
    But that also means something,
  • 1:39 - 1:42
    because choosing the wrong patterns
  • 1:42 - 1:44
    will give us the wrong models
  • 1:44 - 1:45
    and therefore the wrong therapies.
  • 1:45 - 1:47
    Right?
  • 1:48 - 1:49
    In case of attention,
  • 1:49 - 1:51
    the fact that we can
  • 1:52 - 1:55
    shift our attention not only by our eyes
  • 1:55 - 1:57
    but also by thinking --
  • 1:57 - 2:02
    that makes covert attention
    an interesting model for computers.
  • 2:02 - 2:06
    So I wanted to know
    what are the brainwave patterns
  • 2:06 - 2:09
    when you look overtly
    or when you look covertly.
  • 2:10 - 2:12
    I set up an experiment for that.
  • 2:13 - 2:16
    In this experiment
    there are two flickering squares,
  • 2:16 - 2:19
    one of them flickering
    at a slower rate than the other one.
  • 2:21 - 2:24
    Depending on which of these flickers
    you are paying attention to,
  • 2:24 - 2:28
    certain parts of your brain
    will start resonating in the same rate
  • 2:29 - 2:31
    as that flickering rate.
  • 2:32 - 2:35
    So by analyzing your brain signals,
  • 2:35 - 2:38
    we can track where exactly
    you are watching
  • 2:39 - 2:40
    or you are paying attention to.
  • 2:43 - 2:47
    So to see what happens in your brain
    when you pay overt attention,
  • 2:47 - 2:50
    I asked people to look directly
    in one of the squares
  • 2:51 - 2:52
    and pay attention to it.
  • 2:53 - 2:58
    In this case, not surprisingly,
    we saw that these flickering squares
  • 2:58 - 3:00
    appeared in their brain signals
  • 3:00 - 3:02
    which was coming
    from the back of their head,
  • 3:04 - 3:07
    which is responsible for the processing
    of your visual information.
  • 3:08 - 3:11
    But I was really interested
  • 3:11 - 3:14
    to see what happens in your brain
    when you pay covert attention.
  • 3:14 - 3:18
    So this time I asked people
    to look in the middle of the screen
  • 3:18 - 3:20
    and without moving their eyes,
  • 3:21 - 3:24
    to pay attention
    to either of these squares.
  • 3:25 - 3:27
    When we did that,
  • 3:27 - 3:31
    we saw that both of these flickering rates
    appeared in their brain signals,
  • 3:31 - 3:32
    but interestingly,
  • 3:33 - 3:36
    only one of them,
    which was paid attention to,
  • 3:36 - 3:38
    had stronger signals,
  • 3:38 - 3:40
    so there was something in the brain
  • 3:40 - 3:43
    which was handling this information
  • 3:43 - 3:49
    so that thing in the brain was basically
    the activation of the frontal area.
  • 3:50 - 3:53
    The front part of your brain
    is responsible
  • 3:53 - 3:56
    for higher cognitive functions as a human.
  • 3:57 - 4:02
    The frontal part,
    it seems that it works as a filter
  • 4:03 - 4:07
    trying to let information come in
    only from the right flicker
  • 4:07 - 4:09
    that you are paying attention to
  • 4:09 - 4:13
    and trying to inhibit the information
    coming from the ignored one.
  • 4:15 - 4:21
    The filtering ability of the brain
    is indeed a key for attention,
  • 4:21 - 4:23
    which is missing in some people,
  • 4:24 - 4:26
    for example in people with ADHD.
  • 4:27 - 4:32
    So a person with ADHD
    cannot inhibit these distractors,
  • 4:32 - 4:36
    and that's why they can't focus
    for a long time on a single task.
  • 4:38 - 4:39
    But what if this person
  • 4:39 - 4:43
    could play a specific computer game
  • 4:43 - 4:46
    with his brain connected to the computer,
  • 4:46 - 4:49
    and then train his own brain
  • 4:49 - 4:52
    to inhibit these distractors?
  • 4:54 - 4:56
    Well, ADHD is just one example.
  • 4:57 - 5:00
    We can use this cognitive
    brain machine interfaces
  • 5:00 - 5:03
    for many other cognitive fields.
  • 5:04 - 5:06
    It was just a few years ago
  • 5:06 - 5:11
    that my grandfather had a stroke
    and he lost complete ability to speak.
  • 5:13 - 5:16
    He could understand everybody,
    but there was no way to respond,
  • 5:16 - 5:18
    even not writing
    because he was illiterate.
  • 5:20 - 5:23
    So he passed away in silence.
  • 5:25 - 5:27
    I remember thinking at that time,
  • 5:27 - 5:31
    what if we could have a computer
  • 5:31 - 5:32
    which could speak for him?
  • 5:34 - 5:36
    Now, after years that I am in this field,
  • 5:36 - 5:38
    I can see that this might be possible.
  • 5:40 - 5:43
    Imagine if we can find brainwave patterns
  • 5:43 - 5:47
    when people think
    about images or even letters,
  • 5:48 - 5:51
    like the letter A generates
    a different brainwave pattern
  • 5:51 - 5:52
    than the letter B, and so on.
  • 5:53 - 5:57
    Could a computer one day
    communicate for people who can't speak?
  • 5:58 - 5:59
    What if a computer
  • 6:00 - 6:05
    can help us understand
    the thoughts of a person in a coma?
  • 6:06 - 6:07
    We are not there yet,
  • 6:07 - 6:10
    but pay close attention.
  • 6:10 - 6:12
    We will be there soon.
  • 6:12 - 6:13
    Thank you.
  • 6:13 - 6:17
    (Applause)
Title:
What happens in your brain when you pay attention?
Speaker:
Mehdi Ordikhani-Seyedlar
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
06:32

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions