< Return to Video

How do our brains process speech? - Gareth Gaskell

  • Not Synced
    The average 20-year-old knows between
    27,000 and 52,000 different words.
  • Not Synced
    By age 60, that number averages between
    35,000 and 56,000.
  • Not Synced
    Spoken out loud, most of these words last
    less than a second.
  • Not Synced
    So with every word, the brain has a quick
    decision to make:
  • Not Synced
    which of those thousands of options
    matches the signal?
  • Not Synced
    About 98% of the time, the brain chooses
    the correct word.
  • Not Synced
    But how? Speech comprehension is different
    from reading comprehension,
  • Not Synced
    but it’s similar to sign language
    comprehension—
  • Not Synced
    though spoken word recognition has
    been studied more than sign language.
  • Not Synced
    The key to our ability to understand
    speech
  • Not Synced
    is the brain’s role as a
    parallel processor,
  • Not Synced
    meaning that it can do multiple different
    things at the same time.
  • Not Synced
    Most theories assume that each word
    we know is represented
  • Not Synced
    by a separate processing unit that has
    just one job:
  • Not Synced
    to assess the likelihood of incoming
    speech matching that particular word.
  • Not Synced
    In the context of the brain, the
    processing unit that represents a word
  • Not Synced
    is likely a pattern of firing activity
    across a group of neurons
  • Not Synced
    in the brain’s cortex.
  • Not Synced
    When we hear the beginning of a word,
  • Not Synced
    several thousand such units
    may become active,
  • Not Synced
    because with just the beginning of a
    word, there are many possible matches.
  • Not Synced
    Then, as the word goes on, more and
    more units register
  • Not Synced
    that some vital piece of information
    is missing and lose activity.
  • Not Synced
    Possibly well before the end of the word,
  • Not Synced
    just one firing pattern remains active,
    corresponding to one word.
  • Not Synced
    This is called the ‘recognition point.’
  • Not Synced
    In the process of honing in on one word,
  • Not Synced
    the active units suppress
    the activity of others,
  • Not Synced
    saving vital milliseconds.
  • Not Synced
    Most people can comprehend up to
    about 8 syllables per second.
  • Not Synced
    Yet, the goal is not only
    to recognize the word,
  • Not Synced
    but also to access its stored meaning.
  • Not Synced
    The brain accesses many possible meanings
    at the same time,
  • Not Synced
    before the word has been fully identified.
  • Not Synced
    We know this from studies which show
    that even upon hearing a word fragment––
  • Not Synced
    like ‘cap’ ––
  • Not Synced
    listeners will start to register multiple
    possible meanings,
  • Not Synced
    like captain or capital,
    before the full word emerges.
  • Not Synced
    This suggests that every time we hear a
    word
  • Not Synced
    there’s a brief explosion of meanings in
    our minds,
  • Not Synced
    and by the recognition point the brain
    has settled on one interpretation.
  • Not Synced
    The recognition process moves more
    rapidly with a sentence
  • Not Synced
    that gives us context than in a random
    string of words.
  • Not Synced
    Context also helps guide us towards the
    intended meaning of words
  • Not Synced
    with multiple interpretations, like ‘bat,’
    or ‘crane,’
  • Not Synced
    or in cases of homophones
    like ‘no’ or ‘know.’
  • Not Synced
    For multilingual people, the language
    they are listening to is another cue,
  • Not Synced
    used to eliminate potential words
    that don’t match the language context.
  • Not Synced
    So, what about adding completely new
    words to this system?
  • Not Synced
    Even as adults, we may come across a
    new word every few days.
  • Not Synced
    But if every word is represented as a
    fine-tuned pattern of activity
  • Not Synced
    distributed over many neurons,
  • Not Synced
    how do we prevent new words from
    overwriting old ones?
  • Not Synced
    We think that to avoid this problem,
  • Not Synced
    new words are initially stored in a part
    of the brain called the hippocampus,
  • Not Synced
    well away from the main store of words
    in the cortex,
  • Not Synced
    so they don’t share neurons
    with others words.
  • Not Synced
    Then, over multiple nights of sleep,
  • Not Synced
    the new words gradually transfer over
    and interweave with old ones.
  • Not Synced
    Researchers think this gradual acquisition
    process
  • Not Synced
    helps avoid disrupting existing words.
Title:
How do our brains process speech? - Gareth Gaskell
Speaker:
Gareth Gaskell
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TED-Ed
Duration:
04:21
lauren mcalpine approved English subtitles for How do our brains process speech?
lauren mcalpine accepted English subtitles for How do our brains process speech?
lauren mcalpine edited English subtitles for How do our brains process speech?
Tara Ahmadinejad edited English subtitles for How do our brains process speech?
Tara Ahmadinejad edited English subtitles for How do our brains process speech?

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions