9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 The average 20-year-old knows between[br]27,000 and 52,000 different words. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 By age 60, that number averages between[br]35,000 and 56,000. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 Spoken out loud, most of these words last[br]less than a second. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 So with every word, the brain has a quick[br]decision to make: 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 which of those thousands of options [br]matches the signal? 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 About 98% of the time, the brain chooses[br]the correct word. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 But how? Speech comprehension is different[br]from reading comprehension, 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 but it’s similar to sign language [br]comprehension— 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 though spoken word recognition has [br]been studied more than sign language. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 The key to our ability to understand [br]speech 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 is the brain’s role as a [br]parallel processor, 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 meaning that it can do multiple different [br]things at the same time. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 Most theories assume that each word[br]we know is represented 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 by a separate processing unit that has [br]just one job: 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 to assess the likelihood of incoming [br]speech matching that particular word. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 In the context of the brain, the [br]processing unit that represents a word 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 is likely a pattern of firing activity [br]across a group of neurons 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 in the brain’s cortex. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 When we hear the beginning of a word, 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 several thousand such units [br]may become active, 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 because with just the beginning of a [br]word, there are many possible matches. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 Then, as the word goes on, more and[br]more units register 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 that some vital piece of information [br]is missing and lose activity. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 Possibly well before the end of the word, 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 just one firing pattern remains active, [br]corresponding to one word. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 This is called the ‘recognition point.’ 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 In the process of honing in on one word, 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 the active units suppress [br]the activity of others, 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 saving vital milliseconds. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 Most people can comprehend up to [br]about 8 syllables per second. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 Yet, the goal is not only [br]to recognize the word, 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 but also to access its stored meaning. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 The brain accesses many possible meanings[br]at the same time, 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 before the word has been fully identified. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 We know this from studies which show [br]that even upon hearing a word fragment–– 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 like ‘cap’ –– 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 listeners will start to register multiple [br]possible meanings, 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 like captain or capital,[br]before the full word emerges. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 This suggests that every time we hear a [br]word 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 there’s a brief explosion of meanings in [br]our minds, 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 and by the recognition point the brain [br]has settled on one interpretation. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 The recognition process moves more [br]rapidly with a sentence 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 that gives us context than in a random [br]string of words. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 Context also helps guide us towards the[br]intended meaning of words 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 with multiple interpretations, like ‘bat,’[br]or ‘crane,’ 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 or in cases of homophones[br]like ‘no’ or ‘know.’ 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 For multilingual people, the language[br]they are listening to is another cue, 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 used to eliminate potential words[br]that don’t match the language context. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 So, what about adding completely new [br]words to this system? 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 Even as adults, we may come across a [br]new word every few days. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 But if every word is represented as a [br]fine-tuned pattern of activity 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 distributed over many neurons, 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 how do we prevent new words from [br]overwriting old ones? 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 We think that to avoid this problem, 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 new words are initially stored in a part [br]of the brain called the hippocampus, 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 well away from the main store of words[br]in the cortex, 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 so they don’t share neurons[br]with others words. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 Then, over multiple nights of sleep, 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 the new words gradually transfer over[br]and interweave with old ones. 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 Researchers think this gradual acquisition[br]process 9:59:59.000,9:59:59.000 helps avoid disrupting existing words.