1 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 The average 20-year-old knows between 27,000 and 52,000 different words. 2 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 By age 60, that number averages between 35,000 and 56,000. 3 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 Spoken out loud, most of these words last less than a second. 4 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 So with every word, the brain has a quick decision to make: 5 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 which of those thousands of options matches the signal? 6 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 About 98% of the time, the brain chooses the correct word. 7 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 But how? Speech comprehension is different from reading comprehension, 8 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 but it’s similar to sign language comprehension— 9 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 though spoken word recognition has been studied more than sign language. 10 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 The key to our ability to understand speech 11 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 is the brain’s role as a parallel processor, 12 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 meaning that it can do multiple different things at the same time. 13 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 Most theories assume that each word we know is represented 14 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 by a separate processing unit that has just one job: 15 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 to assess the likelihood of incoming speech matching that particular word. 16 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 In the context of the brain, the processing unit that represents a word 17 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 is likely a pattern of firing activity across a group of neurons 18 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 in the brain’s cortex. 19 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 When we hear the beginning of a word, 20 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 several thousand such units may become active, 21 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 because with just the beginning of a word, there are many possible matches. 22 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 Then, as the word goes on, more and more units register 23 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 that some vital piece of information is missing and lose activity. 24 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 Possibly well before the end of the word, 25 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 just one firing pattern remains active, corresponding to one word. 26 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 This is called the ‘recognition point.’ 27 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 In the process of honing in on one word, 28 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 the active units suppress the activity of others, 29 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 saving vital milliseconds. 30 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 Most people can comprehend up to about 8 syllables per second. 31 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 Yet, the goal is not only to recognize the word, 32 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 but also to access its stored meaning. 33 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 The brain accesses many possible meanings at the same time, 34 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 before the word has been fully identified. 35 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 We know this from studies which show that even upon hearing a word fragment–– 36 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 like ‘cap’ –– 37 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 listeners will start to register multiple possible meanings, 38 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 like captain or capital, before the full word emerges. 39 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 This suggests that every time we hear a word 40 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 there’s a brief explosion of meanings in our minds, 41 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 and by the recognition point the brain has settled on one interpretation. 42 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 The recognition process moves more rapidly with a sentence 43 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 that gives us context than in a random string of words. 44 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 Context also helps guide us towards the intended meaning of words 45 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 with multiple interpretations, like ‘bat,’ or ‘crane,’ 46 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 or in cases of homophones like ‘no’ or ‘know.’ 47 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 For multilingual people, the language they are listening to is another cue, 48 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 used to eliminate potential words that don’t match the language context. 49 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 So, what about adding completely new words to this system? 50 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 Even as adults, we may come across a new word every few days. 51 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 But if every word is represented as a fine-tuned pattern of activity 52 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 distributed over many neurons, 53 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 how do we prevent new words from overwriting old ones? 54 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 We think that to avoid this problem, 55 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 new words are initially stored in a part of the brain called the hippocampus, 56 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 well away from the main store of words in the cortex, 57 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 so they don’t share neurons with others words. 58 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 Then, over multiple nights of sleep, 59 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 the new words gradually transfer over and interweave with old ones. 60 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 Researchers think this gradual acquisition process 61 99:59:59,999 --> 99:59:59,999 helps avoid disrupting existing words.