How early life experience is written into DNA
-
0:01 - 0:03So it all came to life
-
0:03 - 0:04in a dark bar in Madrid.
-
0:05 - 0:09I encountered my colleague
from McGill, Michael Meaney. -
0:09 - 0:12And we were drinking a few beers,
-
0:12 - 0:14and like scientists do,
-
0:14 - 0:15he told me about his work.
-
0:16 - 0:23And he told me that he is interested
in how mother rats lick their pups -
0:23 - 0:25after they were born.
-
0:26 - 0:28And I was sitting there and saying,
-
0:28 - 0:31"This is where my tax
dollars are wasted -- -
0:31 - 0:32(Laughter)
-
0:32 - 0:35on this kind of soft science."
-
0:36 - 0:38And he started telling me
-
0:38 - 0:42that the rats, like humans,
-
0:42 - 0:44lick their pups in very different ways.
-
0:44 - 0:47Some mothers do a lot of that,
-
0:47 - 0:49some mothers do very little,
-
0:49 - 0:51and most are in between.
-
0:52 - 0:54But what's interesting about it
-
0:54 - 0:59is when he follows these pups
when they become adults -- -
0:59 - 1:03like, years in human life,
long after their mother died. -
1:03 - 1:05They are completely different animals.
-
1:05 - 1:09The animals that were licked
and groomed heavily, -
1:09 - 1:11the high-licking and grooming,
-
1:12 - 1:13are not stressed.
-
1:14 - 1:16They have different sexual behavior.
-
1:16 - 1:19They have a different way of living
-
1:19 - 1:25than those that were not treated
as intensively by their mothers. -
1:26 - 1:29So then I was thinking to myself:
-
1:29 - 1:30Is this magic?
-
1:31 - 1:32How does this work?
-
1:32 - 1:35As geneticists would like you to think,
-
1:35 - 1:39perhaps the mother had
the "bad mother" gene -
1:39 - 1:43that caused her pups to be stressful,
-
1:43 - 1:46and then it was passed
from generation to generation; -
1:46 - 1:48it's all determined by genetics.
-
1:48 - 1:52Or is it possible that something
else is going on here? -
1:52 - 1:55In rats, we can ask
this question and answer it. -
1:55 - 1:59So what we did is
a cross-fostering experiment. -
1:59 - 2:04You essentially separate the litter,
the babies of this rat, at birth, -
2:04 - 2:06to two kinds of fostering mothers --
-
2:06 - 2:09not the real mothers,
but mothers that will take care of them: -
2:09 - 2:11high-licking mothers
and low-licking mothers. -
2:11 - 2:15And you can do the opposite
with the low-licking pups. -
2:16 - 2:18And the remarkable answer was,
-
2:18 - 2:22it wasn't important
what gene you got from your mother. -
2:22 - 2:28It was not the biological mother
that defined this property of these rats. -
2:28 - 2:32It is the mother that
took care of the pups. -
2:33 - 2:36So how can this work?
-
2:37 - 2:39I am an a epigeneticist.
-
2:39 - 2:42I am interested in how genes are marked
-
2:42 - 2:44by a chemical mark
-
2:44 - 2:49during embryogenesis, during the time
we're in the womb of our mothers, -
2:49 - 2:51and decide which gene will be expressed
-
2:52 - 2:53in what tissue.
-
2:53 - 2:57Different genes are expressed in the brain
than in the liver and the eye. -
2:58 - 3:01And we thought: Is it possible
-
3:01 - 3:08that the mother is somehow
reprogramming the gene of her offspring -
3:08 - 3:09through her behavior?
-
3:09 - 3:11And we spent 10 years,
-
3:11 - 3:15and we found that there is a cascade
of biochemical events -
3:15 - 3:18by which the licking and grooming
of the mother, the care of the mother, -
3:18 - 3:21is translated to biochemical signals
-
3:21 - 3:24that go into the nucleus and into the DNA
-
3:24 - 3:26and program it differently.
-
3:26 - 3:31So now the animal can prepare
itself for life: -
3:31 - 3:34Is life going to be harsh?
-
3:34 - 3:36Is there going to be a lot of food?
-
3:36 - 3:38Are there going to be a lot of cats
and snakes around, -
3:38 - 3:40or will I live
in an upper-class neighborhood -
3:40 - 3:43where all I have to do
is behave well and proper, -
3:43 - 3:46and that will gain me social acceptance?
-
3:47 - 3:53And now one can think about
how important that process can be -
3:53 - 3:54for our lives.
-
3:54 - 3:57We inherit our DNA from our ancestors.
-
3:57 - 3:59The DNA is old.
-
3:59 - 4:01It evolved during evolution.
-
4:02 - 4:06But it doesn't tell us
if you are going to be born in Stockholm, -
4:06 - 4:10where the days are long in the summer
and short in the winter, -
4:10 - 4:11or in Ecuador,
-
4:11 - 4:15where there's an equal number of hours
for day and night all year round. -
4:15 - 4:18And that has such an enormous [effect]
on our physiology. -
4:19 - 4:24So what we suggest is,
perhaps what happens early in life, -
4:24 - 4:26those signals that come
through the mother, -
4:26 - 4:30tell the child what kind of social world
you're going to be living in. -
4:30 - 4:34It will be harsh, and you'd better
be anxious and be stressful, -
4:34 - 4:37or it's going to be an easy world,
and you have to be different. -
4:37 - 4:40Is it going to be a world
with a lot of light or little light? -
4:40 - 4:44Is it going to be a world
with a lot of food or little food? -
4:44 - 4:46If there's no food around,
-
4:46 - 4:50you'd better develop your brain to binge
whenever you see a meal, -
4:50 - 4:55or store every piece of food
that you have as fat. -
4:57 - 4:58So this is good.
-
4:58 - 5:00Evolution has selected this
-
5:00 - 5:05to allow our fixed, old DNA
to function in a dynamic way -
5:05 - 5:07in new environments.
-
5:07 - 5:10But sometimes things can go wrong;
-
5:11 - 5:15for example, if you're born
to a poor family -
5:15 - 5:18and the signals are, "You better binge,
-
5:18 - 5:21you better eat every piece of food
you're going to encounter." -
5:21 - 5:23But now we humans
and our brain have evolved, -
5:23 - 5:25have changed evolution even faster.
-
5:25 - 5:29Now you can buy McDonald's for one dollar.
-
5:29 - 5:35And therefore, the preparation
that we had by our mothers -
5:35 - 5:38is turning out to be maladaptive.
-
5:38 - 5:43The same preparation that was supposed
to protect us from hunger and famine -
5:43 - 5:45is going to cause obesity,
-
5:45 - 5:48cardiovascular problems
and metabolic disease. -
5:49 - 5:52So this concept that genes
could be marked by our experience, -
5:52 - 5:54and especially the early life experience,
-
5:54 - 5:57can provide us a unifying explanation
-
5:57 - 6:00of both health and disease.
-
6:01 - 6:03But is true only for rats?
-
6:03 - 6:06The problem is, we cannot
test this in humans, -
6:06 - 6:10because ethically, we cannot administer
child adversity in a random way. -
6:10 - 6:13So if a poor child develops
a certain property, -
6:13 - 6:17we don't know whether
this is caused by poverty -
6:17 - 6:20or whether poor people have bad genes.
-
6:20 - 6:23So geneticists will try to tell you
that poor people are poor -
6:23 - 6:25because their genes make them poor.
-
6:25 - 6:27Epigeneticists will tell you
-
6:27 - 6:31poor people are in a bad environment
or an impoverished environment -
6:31 - 6:34that creates that phenotype,
that property. -
6:36 - 6:41So we moved to look
into our cousins, the monkeys. -
6:42 - 6:46My colleague, Stephen Suomi,
has been rearing monkeys -
6:46 - 6:47in two different ways:
-
6:47 - 6:50randomly separated the monkey
from the mother -
6:50 - 6:53and reared her with a nurse
-
6:53 - 6:55and surrogate motherhood conditions.
-
6:56 - 6:58So these monkeys didn't have
a mother; they had a nurse. -
6:58 - 7:03And other monkeys were reared
with their normal, natural mothers. -
7:03 - 7:08And when they were old,
they were completely different animals. -
7:08 - 7:11The monkeys that had a mother
did not care about alcohol, -
7:11 - 7:12they were not sexually aggressive.
-
7:12 - 7:16The monkeys that didn't have a mother
were aggressive, were stressed -
7:16 - 7:18and were alcoholics.
-
7:18 - 7:24So we looked at their DNA
early after birth, to see: -
7:24 - 7:27Is it possible that the mother is marking?
-
7:27 - 7:32Is there a signature of the mother
in the DNA of the offspring? -
7:32 - 7:34These are Day-14 monkeys,
-
7:34 - 7:39and what you see here is the modern way
by which we study epigenetics. -
7:39 - 7:43We can now map those chemical marks,
which we call methylation marks, -
7:43 - 7:46on DNA at a single nucleotide resolution.
-
7:47 - 7:48We can map the entire genome.
-
7:48 - 7:51We can now compare the monkey
that had a mother or not. -
7:51 - 7:53And here's a visual presentation of this.
-
7:53 - 7:58What you see is the genes
that got more methylated are red. -
7:58 - 8:01The genes that got
less methylated are green. -
8:01 - 8:04You can see many genes are changing,
-
8:04 - 8:06because not having a mother
is not just one thing -- -
8:07 - 8:08it affects the whole way;
-
8:08 - 8:12it sends signals about the whole way
your world is going to look -
8:12 - 8:13when you become an adult.
-
8:13 - 8:16And you can see the two groups of monkeys
-
8:16 - 8:19extremely well-separated from each other.
-
8:19 - 8:22How early does this develop?
-
8:22 - 8:24These monkeys already
didn't see their mothers, -
8:24 - 8:26so they had a social experience.
-
8:26 - 8:30Do we sense our social status,
even at the moment of birth? -
8:31 - 8:35So in this experiment,
we took placentas of monkeys -
8:35 - 8:37that had different social status.
-
8:38 - 8:43What's interesting about social rank
is that across all living beings, -
8:43 - 8:46they will structure
themselves by hierarchy. -
8:46 - 8:49Monkey number one is the boss;
-
8:49 - 8:51monkey number four is the peon.
-
8:51 - 8:53You put four monkeys in a cage,
-
8:53 - 8:56there will always be a boss
and always be a peon. -
8:57 - 9:01And what's interesting
is that the monkey number one -
9:01 - 9:05is much healthier than monkey number four.
-
9:05 - 9:07And if you put them in a cage,
-
9:07 - 9:11monkey number one will not eat as much.
-
9:11 - 9:13Monkey number four will eat [a lot].
-
9:14 - 9:18And what you see here
in this methylation mapping, -
9:18 - 9:21a dramatic separation at birth
-
9:21 - 9:24of the animals that had
a high social status -
9:24 - 9:27versus the animals
that did not have a high status. -
9:27 - 9:32So we are born already knowing
the social information, -
9:32 - 9:35and that social information
is not bad or good, -
9:35 - 9:36it just prepares us for life,
-
9:36 - 9:40because we have to program
our biology differently -
9:40 - 9:43if we are in the high
or the low social status. -
9:44 - 9:46But how can you study this in humans?
-
9:47 - 9:50We can't do experiments,
we can't administer adversity to humans. -
9:50 - 9:53But God does experiments with humans,
-
9:53 - 9:55and it's called natural disasters.
-
9:55 - 9:59One of the hardest natural disasters
in Canadian history -
9:59 - 10:02happened in my province of Quebec.
-
10:02 - 10:04It's the ice storm of 1998.
-
10:04 - 10:08We lost our entire electrical grid
because of an ice storm -
10:08 - 10:11when the temperatures
were, in the dead of winter in Quebec, -
10:11 - 10:13minus 20 to minus 30.
-
10:13 - 10:16And there were pregnant
mothers during that time. -
10:16 - 10:22And my colleague Suzanne King
followed the children of these mothers -
10:22 - 10:24for 15 years.
-
10:25 - 10:29And what happened was,
that as the stress increased -- -
10:29 - 10:31and here we had objective
measures of stress: -
10:31 - 10:36How long were you without power?
Where did you spend your time? -
10:36 - 10:41Was it in your mother-in-law's apartment
or in some posh country home? -
10:41 - 10:44So all of these added up
to a social stress scale, -
10:44 - 10:45and you can ask the question:
-
10:45 - 10:48How did the children look?
-
10:48 - 10:51And it appears that as stress increases,
-
10:51 - 10:53the children develop more autism,
-
10:53 - 10:55they develop more metabolic diseases
-
10:55 - 10:58and they develop more autoimmune diseases.
-
10:59 - 11:01We would map the methylation state,
-
11:01 - 11:07and again, you see the green genes
becoming red as stress increases, -
11:07 - 11:10the red genes becoming green
as stress increases, -
11:10 - 11:15an entire rearrangement
of the genome in response to stress. -
11:17 - 11:21So if we can program genes,
-
11:21 - 11:25if we are not just the slaves
of the history of our genes, -
11:25 - 11:27that they could be programmed,
can we deprogram them? -
11:28 - 11:33Because epigenetic causes
can cause diseases like cancer, -
11:34 - 11:35metabolic disease
-
11:35 - 11:38and mental health diseases.
-
11:38 - 11:41Let's talk about cocaine addiction.
-
11:42 - 11:45Cocaine addiction is a terrible situation
-
11:45 - 11:48that can lead to death
and to loss of human life. -
11:50 - 11:51We asked the question:
-
11:51 - 11:55Can we reprogram the addicted brain
-
11:55 - 12:00to make that animal not addicted anymore?
-
12:00 - 12:05We used a cocaine addiction model
-
12:05 - 12:07that recapitulates what happens in humans.
-
12:07 - 12:09In humans, you're in high school,
-
12:09 - 12:12some friends suggest you use some cocaine,
-
12:12 - 12:13you take cocaine, nothing happens.
-
12:13 - 12:18Months pass by, something reminds you
of what happened the first time, -
12:18 - 12:19a pusher pushes cocaine,
-
12:19 - 12:22and you become addicted
and your life has changed. -
12:22 - 12:24In rats, we do the same thing.
-
12:24 - 12:25My colleague, Gal Yadid,
-
12:25 - 12:28he trains the animals
to get used to cocaine, -
12:28 - 12:32then for one month, no cocaine.
-
12:32 - 12:35Then he reminds them of the party
when they saw the cocaine the first time -
12:35 - 12:38by cue, the colors of the cage
when they saw cocaine. -
12:38 - 12:40And they go crazy.
-
12:40 - 12:42They will press the lever to get cocaine
-
12:42 - 12:44until they die.
-
12:44 - 12:48We first determined that the difference
between these animals -
12:49 - 12:51is that during that time
when nothing happens, -
12:51 - 12:53there's no cocaine around,
-
12:53 - 12:55their epigenome is rearranged.
-
12:55 - 12:58Their genes are re-marked
in a different way, -
12:58 - 13:02and when the cue comes,
their genome is ready -
13:02 - 13:04to develop this addictive phenotype.
-
13:05 - 13:11So we treated these animals with drugs
that either increase DNA methylation, -
13:11 - 13:14which was the epigenetic
marker to look at, -
13:14 - 13:17or decrease epigenetic markings.
-
13:17 - 13:20And we found that
if we increased methylation, -
13:20 - 13:22these animals go even crazier.
-
13:22 - 13:25They become more craving for cocaine.
-
13:25 - 13:28But if we reduce the DNA methylation,
-
13:28 - 13:30the animals are not addicted anymore.
-
13:30 - 13:32We have reprogrammed them.
-
13:32 - 13:35And a fundamental difference
between an epigenetic drug -
13:35 - 13:37and any other drug
-
13:37 - 13:39is that with epigenetic drugs,
-
13:39 - 13:43we essentially remove
the signs of experience, -
13:43 - 13:45and once they're gone,
-
13:45 - 13:48they will not come back
unless you have the same experience. -
13:48 - 13:50The animal now is reprogrammed.
-
13:50 - 13:54So when we visited the animals
30 days, 60 days later, -
13:54 - 13:57which is in human terms
many years of life, -
13:57 - 14:02they were still not addicted --
by a single epigenetic treatment. -
14:04 - 14:08So what did we learn about DNA?
-
14:08 - 14:11DNA is not just a sequence of letters;
-
14:11 - 14:13it's not just a script.
-
14:13 - 14:15DNA is a dynamic movie.
-
14:16 - 14:21Our experiences are being written
into this movie, which is interactive. -
14:21 - 14:25You're, like, watching a movie
of your life, with the DNA, -
14:25 - 14:27with your remote control.
-
14:27 - 14:30You can remove an actor and add an actor.
-
14:31 - 14:37And so you have, in spite
of the deterministic nature of genetics, -
14:37 - 14:40you have control of the way
your genes look, -
14:41 - 14:44and this has a tremendous
optimistic message -
14:44 - 14:47for the ability to now encounter
some of the deadly diseases -
14:47 - 14:50like cancer, mental health,
-
14:50 - 14:53with a new approach,
-
14:53 - 14:56looking at them as maladaptation.
-
14:56 - 14:59And if we can epigenetically intervene,
-
14:59 - 15:02[we can] reverse the movie
by removing an actor -
15:02 - 15:05and setting up a new narrative.
-
15:06 - 15:09So what I told you today is,
-
15:09 - 15:14our DNA is really combined
of two components, -
15:14 - 15:15two layers of information.
-
15:16 - 15:20One layer of information is old,
-
15:20 - 15:23evolved from millions
of years of evolution. -
15:23 - 15:27It is fixed and very hard to change.
-
15:27 - 15:31The other layer of information
is the epigenetic layer, -
15:31 - 15:35which is open and dynamic
-
15:35 - 15:40and sets up a narrative
that is interactive, -
15:40 - 15:47that allows us to control,
to a large extent, our destiny, -
15:48 - 15:51to help the destiny of our children
-
15:51 - 15:55and to hopefully conquer disease
-
15:55 - 16:00and serious health challenges
-
16:00 - 16:03that have plagued humankind
for a long time. -
16:03 - 16:07So even though we are determined
-
16:07 - 16:09by our genes,
-
16:09 - 16:12we have a degree of freedom
-
16:12 - 16:16that can set up our life
to a life of responsibility. -
16:16 - 16:17Thank you.
-
16:17 - 16:22(Applause)
- Title:
- How early life experience is written into DNA
- Speaker:
- Moshe Szyf
- Description:
-
Moshe Szyf is a pioneer in the field of epigenetics, the study of how living things reprogram their genome in response to social factors like stress and lack of food. His research suggests that biochemical signals passed from mothers to offspring tell the child what kind of world they're going to live in, changing the expression of genes. "DNA isn't just a sequence of letters; it's not just a script." Szyf says. "DNA is a dynamic movie in which our experiences are being written."
- Video Language:
- English
- Team:
- closed TED
- Project:
- TEDTalks
- Duration:
- 16:35
Brian Greene edited English subtitles for How early life experience is written into DNA | ||
Brian Greene edited English subtitles for How early life experience is written into DNA | ||
Camille Martínez accepted English subtitles for How early life experience is written into DNA | ||
Camille Martínez edited English subtitles for How early life experience is written into DNA | ||
Camille Martínez edited English subtitles for How early life experience is written into DNA | ||
Joseph Geni edited English subtitles for How early life experience is written into DNA | ||
Joseph Geni edited English subtitles for How early life experience is written into DNA | ||
Joseph Geni edited English subtitles for How early life experience is written into DNA |