< Return to Video

Z-Day 2014 London - Melissa Saviste - Rewards and Motivation

  • 0:04 - 0:05
    Lost Hobbit
  • 0:05 - 0:10
    Z-Day London 2014
    The Zeitgeist Movement
  • 0:12 - 0:15
    On Rewards and Motivation
    Zeitgeist Day 2014
  • 0:16 - 0:17
    [inaudible conversation]
  • 0:19 - 0:23
    Melissa Saviste
  • 0:24 - 0:24
    [plays clip from the talk show
    "The Lang and O'Leary Exchange"]
  • 0:25 - 0:25
    (Amanda Lang) The combined wealth
    - this according to Oxfam -
  • 0:41 - 0:43
    of the world's 85 richest people
  • 0:43 - 0:47
    is equal to the 3.5 billion
    poorest people.
  • 0:47 - 0:49
    (Kevin O’Leary) It’s fantastic.
  • 0:49 - 0:51
    And this is a great thing
    because it inspires everybody,
  • 0:51 - 0:54
    gets them motivation
    to look up to the 1% and say:
  • 0:54 - 0:56
    “I wanna become one of those people,
  • 0:56 - 0:58
    "I’m gonna fight hard to get up to the top.”
  • 0:58 - 1:00
    This is fantastic news,
    and of course I applaud it.
  • 1:01 - 1:02
    [awkward silence]
  • 1:02 - 1:03
    (O'Leary) What can be wrong with this?
  • 1:03 - 1:04
    (Lang) Really?
  • 1:04 - 1:04
    (O’Leary) Yes, really.
  • 1:04 - 1:06
    (Lang) So, somebody living on...
  • 1:06 - 1:07
    (O'Leary) I celebrate capitalism.
  • 1:07 - 1:09
    (Lang) ...a dollar a day in Africa
    is getting up in the morning
  • 1:09 - 1:11
    and saying “I’m gonna be Bill Gates”?
  • 1:11 - 1:12
    - O'Leary: That’s the motivation everybody needs.
    - Lang: The only thing...
  • 1:12 - 1:14
    - Lang: ...between me and that guy is 'motivation',
    - O'Leary: I'm not against charity!
  • 1:14 - 1:16
    - Lang: ... I just need to pull up my socks…
    - O'Leary: I am not against...
  • 1:16 - 1:18
    - Lang: ...oh wait, I don’t have socks!"
  • 1:18 - 1:20
    (O'Leary) Look. Don't tell me that you want to
    redistribute wealth again.
  • 1:20 - 1:21
    That's never gonna happen, ok?
  • 1:21 - 1:25
    (Lang) You know what, you take a simple stat like this
    which is neither good nor bad. It's just a fact...
  • 1:25 - 1:29
    (O'Leary) It's a celebratory stat. I'm very excited about it.
    I'm wonderful to see it happen.
  • 1:29 - 1:31
    I tell kids everyday: "if you"...
  • 1:31 - 1:32
    - O'Leary: What's wrong with this?
    - Lang: If this comes up at a cocktail party...
  • 1:32 - 1:36
    - O'Leary: No, no. Amanda: What's wrong with this statement?
    - Lang: ...what possible response to it...
  • 1:36 - 1:39
    - O'Leary: If you work hard, you might be stinking rich someday.
    - Lang: We're talking about people...
  • 1:39 - 1:41
    ...in extreme abject poverty.
  • 1:41 - 1:42
    - Lang: ...that's how you get 3.5 billion...
    - O'Leary: No we're not!
  • 1:42 - 1:43
    - O'Leary: You were just talking...
    - Lang: ...in this category.
  • 1:43 - 1:46
    - O'Leary: ...about really rich people.
    - Lang: No.
  • 1:51 - 1:55
    That was Kevin O'Leary,
    a Canadian businessman
  • 1:55 - 1:58
    and probably someone
    you would not want in charge of the world.
  • 1:59 - 2:03
    Those were his ideas on human motivation.
  • 2:04 - 2:06
    He claims that
    money is what motivates people
  • 2:06 - 2:11
    and especially the prospect of being among
    the world's top percent richest
  • 2:11 - 2:15
    which unfortunately seems to be
    quite a common worldview today.
  • 2:15 - 2:17
    But is that true?
  • 2:18 - 2:22
    In Zeitgeist spirit, we're going to
    look at what the actual evidence says
  • 2:23 - 2:29
    because, as we obviously know,
    majority opinion or common sense
  • 2:29 - 2:31
    sometimes turns out to be incorrect.
  • 2:36 - 2:39
    First, some psychological experiments
  • 2:39 - 2:45
    which actually say that
    rewards can reduce already existing motivation.
  • 2:46 - 2:51
    In a 1971 experiment, Edward Deci
    had students assembling puzzle cubes
  • 2:51 - 2:56
    first without rewards, then for money
    and finally without rewards again.
  • 2:56 - 3:00
    He observed them secretly,
    what they were doing in their free time
  • 3:00 - 3:04
    and he noticed that, in their free time
  • 3:04 - 3:07
    they would happily play with the puzzle cubes
  • 3:07 - 3:12
    however, after the reward condition,
    and when rewards were removed,
  • 3:12 - 3:14
    they would work on the puzzles less,
  • 3:14 - 3:18
    they suddenly had less motivation
    to work on puzzles.
  • 3:19 - 3:22
    Another similar experiment
    was done with children
  • 3:22 - 3:24
    who had an intrinsic interest in drawing.
  • 3:24 - 3:27
    This means that they liked to draw
    for its own sake
  • 3:27 - 3:29
    just for the fun of it.
  • 3:29 - 3:32
    Not because of any external motivation.
  • 3:32 - 3:36
    Then, some of the children
    were given a reward certificate
  • 3:36 - 3:39
    on the condition of
    continuing to draw
  • 3:39 - 3:42
    and afterwards,
    they showed less interest in drawing
  • 3:42 - 3:44
    than those who never got any rewards
  • 3:44 - 3:48
    and also less interest
    than they themselves have had at the beginning.
  • 3:49 - 3:53
    The research has called this effect
    "over-justification":
  • 3:53 - 3:56
    if you give and additional reason
    to do something that is already interesting,
  • 3:56 - 3:57
    that makes people think
  • 3:57 - 4:00
    that the task is not worth doing
    without the rewards.
  • 4:00 - 4:01
    So, in other words,
  • 4:01 - 4:05
    the focus shifts from doing the task
    to just getting the reward.
  • 4:08 - 4:12
    Now, you might say that, well,
    motivation isn't that important,
  • 4:12 - 4:15
    as long as things get done,
    and things get done well.
  • 4:15 - 4:18
    And obviously offering rewards
    should make people try harder
  • 4:18 - 4:20
    and get better results.
  • 4:20 - 4:24
    So, Sam Glucksberg tested this
    with a simple puzzle.
  • 4:24 - 4:28
    Participants were given
    a box of tacks, a candle, a matchbox
  • 4:28 - 4:31
    and they were told
    to attach the candle to the wall.
  • 4:31 - 4:33
    Can you think of how you would do this?
  • 4:34 - 4:37
    Half of the participants were told
    that they would receive a monetary reward
  • 4:37 - 4:40
    if they were among the quickest
    to solve the problem.
  • 4:42 - 4:44
    This is a simplified version of the puzzle
  • 4:44 - 4:47
    that was given to another group of participants.
  • 4:47 - 4:51
    There is one crucial difference:
    the box of tacks has already been emptied
  • 4:51 - 4:55
    and in this version,
    the puzzle is really easy to solve.
  • 4:58 - 5:01
    The results showed that for the first task,
  • 5:01 - 5:04
    which is more difficult and requires some creative thinking,
  • 5:04 - 5:07
    performance suffers if rewards are involved.
  • 5:07 - 5:11
    This could again be that the focus shifts
    from the task to getting the money
  • 5:11 - 5:14
    or they might be getting too excited
    about the bonus reward.
  • 5:14 - 5:19
    On the other hand,
    they did do better in this version,
  • 5:19 - 5:20
    in the simple one,
  • 5:20 - 5:22
    when they were offered rewards,
  • 5:23 - 5:26
    because the task was really easy.
  • 5:26 - 5:33
    So, it would seem that is okay to keep paying people
    for simple manual tasks.
  • 5:34 - 5:38
    However, as you all know,
    this is not very relevant anymore
  • 5:38 - 5:42
    because you have things like self-checkouts
    and self-driving cars,
  • 5:42 - 5:46
    and in some places in Asia,
    you even have robot restaurants.
  • 5:50 - 5:54
    So what's left for humans to do
    are the mentally demanding creative tasks,
  • 5:54 - 5:57
    such as building and maintaining those robots.
  • 5:57 - 5:59
    These are the sort of tasks that
  • 5:59 - 6:02
    Glucksberg showed are hindered
    by the promise of rewards.
  • 6:05 - 6:10
    In case you're wondering whether
    increasing the reward amount might work,
  • 6:10 - 6:15
    there was an experiment done
    by Dan Ariely and colleagues in rural India,
  • 6:15 - 6:17
    where they could offer the participants money
  • 6:17 - 6:20
    that to them was worth a year's salary.
  • 6:20 - 6:23
    They found that performance suffered most
  • 6:23 - 6:25
    when they were offered the biggest amount.
  • 6:25 - 6:27
    So, perhaps, when all you can think about is
  • 6:27 - 6:29
    what you're gonna do
    with your big bonus once you get it,
  • 6:29 - 6:33
    you might not get much actual work done.
  • 6:35 - 6:39
    One thing that all these experiments had in common
  • 6:39 - 6:43
    was that a tangible reward such as
    money or a reward certificate
  • 6:43 - 6:46
    was offered on condition
    of the participant doing something
  • 6:46 - 6:50
    and even the biggest critics of this line of research agree that
  • 6:50 - 6:53
    this is one condition where performance does suffer.
  • 6:54 - 6:59
    So, what should we do while we're in a system
    where we do need money to live?
  • 6:59 - 7:03
    Dan Pink, in his book "Drive",
    suggest that companies should pay their employees
  • 7:03 - 7:05
    well and unconditionally,
  • 7:05 - 7:08
    so the the issue of money would be off the table
  • 7:08 - 7:10
    and they could focus on their work.
  • 7:10 - 7:12
    The same idea could be applied to whole countries,
  • 7:12 - 7:16
    which is what the proponents of Basic Income are suggesting.
  • 7:17 - 7:20
    They believe that, if you provide everyone
    with enough money to cover basic needs,
  • 7:20 - 7:23
    then people would still continue to work.
  • 7:23 - 7:25
    They would work on what they love doing
  • 7:25 - 7:28
    because of people's natural desire
    to make the world a better place,
  • 7:28 - 7:30
    while being stuck in a non-rewarding job,
  • 7:30 - 7:33
    just for the sake of being alive,
  • 7:33 - 7:35
    more often hinders their ability to do so.
  • 7:37 - 7:41
    This is similar to Maslow's Hierarchy of
    Needs idea
  • 7:41 - 7:44
    that there are some basic needs
  • 7:44 - 7:46
    but once they're taken care of
  • 7:46 - 7:50
    and people have security
    about their job, house and so on,
  • 7:50 - 7:52
    they can focus on higher needs
  • 7:52 - 7:56
    which are relationships, self-esteem
    and self-actualization.
  • 7:56 - 7:58
    But if there's uncertainty about the future,
  • 7:58 - 8:03
    then that prevents the individuals from achieving
    their true potential.
  • 8:03 - 8:07
    On top of this pyramid of needs is
    self-actualization
  • 8:07 - 8:12
    which essentially means contributing
    something useful and lasting to society
  • 8:12 - 8:14
    which is quite a different view
  • 8:14 - 8:17
    from those who think that
    people only want to work
  • 8:17 - 8:20
    when externally incentivized.
  • 8:23 - 8:25
    Back to the idea of Basic Income.
  • 8:25 - 8:29
    So, what happens if people's
    basic and security needs are taken care of?
  • 8:29 - 8:34

    Thankfully this experiment has been
    carried out a few times in the world.
  • 8:34 - 8:36
    Here is an early example from Canada.
  • 8:36 - 8:40
    At that time, North American governments were
    quite enthusiastic about the idea.
  • 8:40 - 8:46
    They were even thinking about expanding this
    all over the United States and Canada.
  • 8:46 - 8:51
    This pilot project was run from 1974 to '79,
  • 8:51 - 8:55
    but, unfortunately, it stopped due to the recession,
  • 8:55 - 8:58
    and all the data from the project was archived
  • 8:58 - 9:00
    because the government thought it had failed.
  • 9:00 - 9:05
    They were finally discovered and analyzed in 2009
  • 9:05 - 9:06
    and the results were quite interesting.
  • 9:06 - 9:10
    They showed minimal effects on employment.
  • 9:10 - 9:15
    The only groups that worked substantially less
    were new mothers and teenagers,
  • 9:15 - 9:20
    because, well, taking care of children and
    studying are very important
  • 9:20 - 9:24
    and also, as a result, graduation rates went up,
  • 9:24 - 9:28
    other people had more opportunities
    to choose the sort of work they were doing
  • 9:28 - 9:33
    and, very importantly, hospital visits went down
    and mental health improved
  • 9:33 - 9:36
    which actually saves the country some money.
  • 9:37 - 9:42
    A more recent pilot study was carried
    out in a small settlement in Namibia,
  • 9:42 - 9:45
    in the years 2008-09.
  • 9:46 - 9:48
    The results were very dramatic.
  • 9:48 - 9:50
    When at the start of the experiment,
  • 9:50 - 9:55
    food poverty levels were at 76%
    then they went down to 16%.
  • 9:55 - 10:00
    So, essentially, people didn't have to worry about food anymore.
  • 10:00 - 10:02
    They could have stopped working at all,
  • 10:02 - 10:07
    but, to the contrary,
    employment actually went up by 10%
  • 10:07 - 10:10
    because people now had money
    to start their own businesses
  • 10:10 - 10:13
    and they had money to buy from those businesses.
  • 10:14 - 10:20
    This shows how even a small boost to resources
    helps people put them to good use
  • 10:20 - 10:23
    rather than increasing dependency on the free money
  • 10:23 - 10:26
    which the critics were predicting.
  • 10:26 - 10:30
    In addition, parents could now pay their kids' school fees
  • 10:30 - 10:31

    so school attendance doubled,
  • 10:31 - 10:34
    and drop out rates went to almost zero,
  • 10:34 - 10:38

    which is largely because the children
    were no longer malnourished.
  • 10:39 - 10:42
    Also crime rates went down by 42%
  • 10:42 - 10:45
    'cause people no longer had to steal from each other.
  • 10:46 - 10:53
    Unfortunately, though, the Namibian government
    isn't planning to make this nationwide,
  • 10:53 - 10:59
    even though the calculations show it would
    only cost 3% of the GDP.
  • 11:01 - 11:05
    However, there is a government somewhere
    in the world where it might be possible.
  • 11:05 - 11:09
    In Switzerland, there's still the old tradition
    of direct democracy,
  • 11:09 - 11:13
    and they hold regular referenda on major issues.
  • 11:13 - 11:17
    To propose a referendum, you need 100 000 signatures
  • 11:17 - 11:20

    which is what the proponents of Basic Income have done.
  • 11:20 - 11:23
    The votes will be held in the next two or three years,
  • 11:23 - 11:27
    and it will be an interesting experiment to see.
  • 11:27 - 11:30
    Will the Swiss people become desensitized and stop working?
  • 11:30 - 11:34

    The evidence so far is just probably not,
  • 11:34 - 11:37
    but if the evidence so far has not been enough,
  • 11:37 - 11:39
    there are plenty of more examples.
  • 11:39 - 11:42
    [different open-source projects shown on screen]
  • 11:42 - 11:45
    And in addition to all of these,
  • 11:45 - 11:46
    various statistics show that
  • 11:46 - 11:51
    between 30 to 50% of people
    volunteer at least once a month,
  • 11:51 - 11:55
    and remember, all of this happens while they
    still have day jobs.
  • 11:58 - 12:01
    So, after all these examples, you might be wondering
  • 12:01 - 12:04
    what is it then that motivates people?
  • 12:04 - 12:11
    I'll give you four theories, which probably,
    all have a lot of truth in it and they will overlap.
  • 12:12 - 12:16
    First of all there's the Maslow theory mentioned before,
  • 12:16 - 12:20
    then there's Deci and Ryan's Self-Determination theory
  • 12:20 - 12:25
    which lists the elements as competence,
    autonomy and relatedness,
  • 12:25 - 12:28
    a large emphasis is on autonomy,
  • 12:28 - 12:31
    being able to choose what to do, how to do it,
  • 12:31 - 12:34
    when to do it and who to do it with.
  • 12:34 - 12:38
    They say that this also explains the previous experiments
  • 12:38 - 12:43
    because getting someone to do something for a reward
  • 12:43 - 12:46
    is essentially a form of control,
  • 12:46 - 12:49
    it's trying to control the people's behavior
  • 12:49 - 12:52
    by offering them something
    that they really need.
  • 12:54 - 13:01
    Often in workplaces,
    the needs for autonomy are not covered,
  • 13:01 - 13:03
    'cause you don't often have a choice
  • 13:03 - 13:06
    of what sort of work you do
    or how.
  • 13:06 - 13:11
    However, in some more progressive work places,
  • 13:11 - 13:13
    they do try to increase autonomy, for example,
  • 13:13 - 13:17
    Google gives employees 20% of time
    to work on whatever they want,
  • 13:17 - 13:22
    and from this you have things
    like Gmail and Google News.
  • 13:22 - 13:27
    Also "Post-it notes" were developed by
    the stationery company 3M
  • 13:27 - 13:31
    by letting the employees come up
    with whatever they wanted.
  • 13:33 - 13:36
    Dan Pink slightly modified the three words
  • 13:36 - 13:39
    and his version is autonomy,
    mastery and purpose.
  • 13:39 - 13:43
    We'll come back to mastery and
    purpose in the next few slides.
  • 13:43 - 13:46
    Finally, Alfie Kohn in his book "Punished by Rewards",
  • 13:46 - 13:51
    he uses 3 C's:
    collaboration, content and choice,
  • 13:51 - 13:54
    to which you could possibly add challenge.
  • 13:54 - 13:57
    He also quotes this fellow name Herzberg, who said:
  • 13:57 - 14:00
    "If you want people to do a good job,
  • 14:00 - 14:03
    "give them a good job to do", and
  • 14:03 - 14:06
    "Idleness, indifference and irresponsibility
  • 14:06 - 14:09
    are a perfectly valid responses to absurd work".
  • 14:11 - 14:14
    [Screen: MOTIVATION
    It's not that I'm lazy, it's that I just don't care]
  • 14:14 - 14:16
    This is from the film "Office Space".
  • 14:19 - 14:22
    Coming back to mastery.
  • 14:22 - 14:26
    There's a theory by a Hungarian psychologist
    with a ridiculously difficult name [screen: Csikszentmihalyi],
  • 14:26 - 14:29
    which I'm not going to pronounce,
  • 14:29 - 14:32
    he describes the state of optimal experience,
    called "flow"
  • 14:32 - 14:35
    which means that
    you're completely immersed in your task,
  • 14:35 - 14:38
    you almost loose sense of the passing of time,
  • 14:38 - 14:42
    and you even forget if you're hungry or thirsty.
  • 14:43 - 14:46
    Kinda of like the quote by Isaac Asimov:
  • 14:46 - 14:48
    "Nothing interferes with my concentration.
  • 14:48 - 14:50
    "You could put an orgy in my office
  • 14:50 - 14:52
    "and I wouldn't look up - well, maybe once".
  • 14:55 - 14:58
    According to the theory,
    the state of flow will be reached
  • 14:58 - 15:02
    if the difficulty of the task
    matches the person's skills.
  • 15:02 - 15:07
    So if the task is just a little bit challenging,
    as if it's too easy, then it will be boring;
  • 15:07 - 15:11
    if it's too hard, it will produce more anxiety than anything.
  • 15:11 - 15:15
    The study showed that this state is three times
    as likely to happen at work
  • 15:15 - 15:19
    than in free time and also
    people are often happier in workflow
  • 15:19 - 15:22
    than in leisure activities that
    do not produce a flow state
  • 15:22 - 15:24
    such as watching TV,
  • 15:24 - 15:26
    which creates a quite odd paradox
  • 15:26 - 15:30
    because people still spend
    an awful lot of time watching TV
  • 15:30 - 15:34
    and this brings up an important point
    that people often don't realize:
  • 15:34 - 15:36
    how much they can enjoy work.
  • 15:36 - 15:40
    And they think that work must be boring
    just because it's work,
  • 15:40 - 15:44
    which is an attitude worth challenging.
  • 15:47 - 15:51
    The importance of purpose is
    pretty much self-evident.
  • 15:51 - 15:55
    If you have a purpose then you
    have a reason to do something
  • 15:55 - 15:58
    and then you must have motivation to do it.
  • 15:59 - 16:01
    As we said about common sense before,
  • 16:01 - 16:04
    it's still worth looking at it scientifically.
  • 16:04 - 16:12
    Dan Ariely has tested this with experiments
    with Lego Bionicles and paper shredders.
  • 16:12 - 16:18
    In the Bionicle experiment, participants
    were instructed to put together Lego Bionicles,
  • 16:18 - 16:20
    and they were paid
    decreasing amounts of money to do it
  • 16:20 - 16:22
    but everyone was paid the same.
  • 16:22 - 16:25
    However, there were two conditions.
  • 16:25 - 16:28
    In the first condition,
    as soon as they built the Bionicle,
  • 16:28 - 16:31
    the experimenter would take it apart again;
  • 16:31 - 16:34
    In the second condition, the Bionicles would stay there
  • 16:34 - 16:38
    and the participants could see all
    the previous Bionicoles they had built.
  • 16:39 - 16:45
    In the second condition, they built on average
    more Bionicles than in the other condition.
  • 16:46 - 16:50
    Also, with paper shredders,
    they did a task on paper
  • 16:50 - 16:58
    and in the first condition, the experimenter
    would acknowledge the work they had done,
  • 16:58 - 17:00
    scan it in, and put in a pile.
  • 17:00 - 17:03
    In the second condition,
    the experimenter ignored what they had done
  • 17:03 - 17:05
    and just put it in a pile;
  • 17:05 - 17:07
    and in a third condition,
    they ran it through a paper shredder.
  • 17:07 - 17:10
    As you can guess, in the third condition,
  • 17:10 - 17:13
    the participants lost their motivation quite quickly,
  • 17:13 - 17:18
    whereas if their work was acknowledged,
    then they had more motivation to do it.
  • 17:18 - 17:22
    In fact, having to do an pointless job is so unpleasant
  • 17:22 - 17:24
    that has actually been used as a punishment.
  • 17:24 - 17:29
    First in the Greek myth of Sisyphus who,
    as a punishment, had to run a boulder up a hill,
  • 17:29 - 17:32
    but as soon as he almost reached the top,
  • 17:32 - 17:35
    the boulder come down again
    and he had to start over.
  • 17:36 - 17:40
    In the real world, it has been used in prisons
    where prisoners had to dig holes
  • 17:40 - 17:42
    and fill them up again.
  • 17:42 - 17:46
    That was before the institutions realized
    that they could actually profit off the prisoners
  • 17:46 - 17:48
    by making them do real work.
  • 17:49 - 17:54
    So it's no wonder that too many people
    in today's jobs lack motivation
  • 17:54 - 17:56
    and hate going to work every day,
  • 17:56 - 17:59
    'cause there are so many jobs that
    simply don't seem to have a purpose,
  • 17:59 - 18:01
    sometimes jobs we could even do without
  • 18:01 - 18:04
    such as sandwich board advertisements.
  • 18:06 - 18:12
    But often jobs are even created
    for the sake of creating jobs
  • 18:12 - 18:15
    just because of the way the monetary system works
  • 18:15 - 18:17
    ...or malfunctions, more like.
  • 18:18 - 18:24
    Sadly, though, sometimes the most purposeful jobs
    don't even pay you a single penny
  • 18:24 - 18:28
    or if they do, then they pay you
    much less than the other jobs.
  • 18:28 - 18:31
    But on the bright side,
    people do them anyway.
  • 18:33 - 18:39
    So, what I would like to leave you with is a few ideas
    on what to do with all of this info.
  • 18:40 - 18:42
    First , obviously, you can use all these examples
  • 18:42 - 18:46
    whenever someone claims that
    without money nothing would get done
  • 18:46 - 18:49
    which is something we all probably hear a lot.
  • 18:49 - 18:54
    Secondly, I would like you to ask yourself:
    what motivates you?
  • 18:54 - 18:59
    What is it that you do, in your free time,
    that you feel has a purpose,
  • 18:59 - 19:02
    that fulfills you, that challenges you?
  • 19:02 - 19:07
    And if there isn't anything like that
    right now, maybe you can think of something
  • 19:07 - 19:12
    that you could be doing because often
    it's best to lead by example
  • 19:12 - 19:17
    and you could also be
    yet another contribution
  • 19:17 - 19:20
    to the growing evidence that
    people do things
  • 19:20 - 19:24
    just because of intrinsic motivation,
  • 19:24 - 19:25
    not because of money.
  • 19:26 - 19:30
    And finally you could ask your friends the same questions:
  • 19:30 - 19:36
    What motivates them? Would they still be
    working if a Resource-Based Economy happened
  • 19:36 - 19:38
    or if they received a Basic Income?
  • 19:38 - 19:41
    Or would they just sit on their couch all day?
  • 19:42 - 19:47
    Well, after all,
    even Kevin from the beginning video...
  • 19:48 - 19:50
    (There should be a picture here...)
  • 19:55 - 19:56
    (There we go.)
  • 19:56 - 20:01
    Even Kevin from the beginning video is
    occasionally intrinsically motivated.
  • 20:01 - 20:03
    [Screen: Kevin O'Leary playing guitar]
  • 20:04 - 20:05
    Thank you.
Title:
Z-Day 2014 London - Melissa Saviste - Rewards and Motivation
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English, British
Duration:
41:02

English subtitles

Revisions