Brain: memory and multitasking | Adam Gazzaley, MD, PhD | TEDxSanJoseCA
-
0:07 - 0:09Thank you. It's quite a privilege
to be here today. -
0:10 - 0:14I'm going to start with this picture here
and see if anyone in this room - -
0:14 - 0:16let's start with a show of hands -
-
0:16 - 0:18knows what this picture is depicting.
-
0:20 - 0:21Does anyone know what this is?
-
0:21 - 0:25This is the feeling that you have
after you've left the couch -
0:25 - 0:29with a clear idea in mind about
what you wanted to get in the kitchen. -
0:29 - 0:30(Laughter)
-
0:30 - 0:34You arrive there, look in the refrigerator
and have no idea what you're doing. -
0:34 - 0:35(Laughter)
-
0:35 - 0:37Has anyone in this room
ever experienced this? -
0:37 - 0:40Don't be shy if you have.
You could share with us. -
0:40 - 0:41How amazing is that,
-
0:42 - 0:44that our magnificent brains
could let us down, -
0:44 - 0:47that we could know exactly
what we want, one single item, -
0:47 - 0:49and just seven seconds later,
we don't remember it. -
0:49 - 0:53Clearly, we have the ability to remember
a single item for seven seconds. -
0:53 - 0:54How is that possible?
-
0:54 - 0:57How is it possible
that our brains could fail us -
0:57 - 1:00in that very easy task
of just holding one thing in mind? -
1:01 - 1:03The brain is the most remarkable
structure we're aware of. -
1:03 - 1:06It's capable of incredibly rapid
parallel processing, -
1:06 - 1:10allowing us to interpret
complex stimuli in our environment -
1:10 - 1:11within a tenth of a second.
-
1:11 - 1:12Even computers,
-
1:12 - 1:14which are designed to try to do this,
-
1:14 - 1:16have an inability to do that.
-
1:16 - 1:20Plus, we store a massive amount of
information over the course of our lives, -
1:20 - 1:23by some estimates,
a billion bits of information - -
1:23 - 1:27that's 50,000 times the text
stored in the library of Congress. -
1:28 - 1:31Despite living in a rather small space,
the brain is a massive structure, -
1:32 - 1:35having approximately
a hundred billion neurons - -
1:35 - 1:38that's on the order of stars
in the core of our Milky Way galaxy. -
1:39 - 1:40What's even more impressive
-
1:40 - 1:44is that there are hundreds of trillions
of connections between these neurons, -
1:44 - 1:50creating a staggering network
of very, very intricate complexity. -
1:51 - 1:54But I don't have to tell you
a lot of cool facts about the brain -
1:54 - 1:57for you to realize how amazing it is:
-
1:57 - 1:59every emotion you feel,
every thought you have, -
1:59 - 2:02every sensation you experience,
every move you make, -
2:02 - 2:04your very sense of identity -
-
2:04 - 2:07all emerges from
the function of our brains. -
2:08 - 2:11Despite that, the brain
has some very distinct limitations. -
2:11 - 2:13I'll be talking about three of them.
-
2:13 - 2:15The first is attention.
-
2:15 - 2:19We know, just based on our experience
and now an incredible amount of data, -
2:19 - 2:22that we cannot distribute our attention
everywhere, infinitely. -
2:22 - 2:25We can selectively focus our attention,
and that's what it's required to do: -
2:25 - 2:29that we direct our resources
to what our goals are guiding us towards. -
2:30 - 2:33Another limitation is something
known as working memory. -
2:33 - 2:36Working memory is online memory,
not long-term memory. -
2:36 - 2:38It's the memory you use
to guide your actions; -
2:38 - 2:42it happens during a conversation
to carry the thread of what's going on. -
2:42 - 2:44It's also the type
of the memory that you use -- -
2:44 - 2:48in the past, when someone gave you
a phone number instead of send it to you, -
2:48 - 2:51you had to hold it in mind
until you got it into the phone. -
2:51 - 2:54We know that there are very strict
limits on this capacity, -
2:54 - 2:56and they decrease as information
becomes more complex. -
2:57 - 2:59Another limitation is speed.
-
2:59 - 3:03Although, as I described, our brains
and certainly the neurons, -
3:03 - 3:07if you look down at a high resolution,
are capable of very rapid processing. -
3:07 - 3:09Because our brain functions
as this massive network, -
3:09 - 3:11when you have very complex operations,
-
3:12 - 3:16speed of all these areas communicating
with each other becomes a limitation. -
3:16 - 3:19So these three limitations
on our brain's abilities -
3:19 - 3:22leads to a sensitivity to interference.
-
3:23 - 3:24I'm going to describe what that is.
-
3:24 - 3:28We went and we asked folks
when they had this experience - -
3:28 - 3:29and almost everyone describes this;
-
3:29 - 3:33it increases as you get older,
for those of you that had not noticed - -
3:33 - 3:34What happens?
-
3:34 - 3:39What led you to find your way
to the kitchen with a clear idea in mind -
3:39 - 3:42and when you got there
you didn't know what you were doing, -
3:42 - 3:44and you could not remember
what you were trying to get? -
3:45 - 3:48And people describe in their
best experience what is happening, -
3:48 - 3:51and we built a conceptual
framework from this. -
3:51 - 3:53And then we study it in our lab,
I'm a neuroscientist, -
3:53 - 3:56we use tools to study
how the brain functions, -
3:56 - 3:58and how that leads
to higher-order behaviors. -
3:58 - 4:02So we built the framework based on
people's anecdotes and experiences, -
4:02 - 4:05and then tried to pursue it empirically
and see if these were really mechanisms -
4:05 - 4:09that influence behavior
in the way people experience. -
4:09 - 4:11So this is how the framework goes:
-
4:11 - 4:14we see that there's two types
of interference, generally - -
4:14 - 4:17internal interference
and externally induced interference. -
4:17 - 4:21We could split those each into different
types of interference based on your goals. -
4:22 - 4:25So if the information is totally
irrelevant to you, -
4:25 - 4:27we consider it a distraction.
-
4:27 - 4:29Let's say you're at a restaurant
having a conversation. -
4:29 - 4:33If you have any hopes of remembering
the details of that conversation, -
4:33 - 4:36you know you're very busy
suppressing the chatter in the room, -
4:36 - 4:39maybe the waiter is taking
an order at the next table, -
4:39 - 4:41you're just trying to block that out,
-
4:41 - 4:42it's irrelevant information,
-
4:42 - 4:44you're trying to suppress it.
-
4:44 - 4:46That same stimuli can serve
-
4:46 - 4:48as what we call interruptions,
or multitasking, -
4:48 - 4:51when you think maybe you can do
more than one thing at a time. -
4:51 - 4:53Maybe you're having that conversation,
-
4:53 - 4:55but now you're trying to listen
-
4:55 - 4:58to what the specials are
being announced at the next table. -
4:58 - 5:00Or you're on the way
to the refrigerator and a phone rings; -
5:00 - 5:04you pick it up, and now you have to
reengage in your initial task. -
5:04 - 5:06That's how we split external interference.
-
5:06 - 5:08This can all happen internally as well.
-
5:08 - 5:10Some of it is mind wondering:
-
5:10 - 5:14your thoughts leave your focus
against your will, irrelevantly, -
5:15 - 5:17just travel to places that you
did not intend them to. -
5:18 - 5:20Or you could be multitasking internally.
-
5:20 - 5:22Perhaps you're doing it now:
-
5:22 - 5:23you pay attention to what I'm saying,
-
5:23 - 5:25you look at the slides, listening to me,
-
5:25 - 5:28but you might be planning
what to have for lunch later -
5:28 - 5:30and who you're going to meet
in the afternoon. -
5:30 - 5:35So this constant interference keeps us
from being present, directed on our goals. -
5:37 - 5:41So, given those limitations of our brain
and our susceptibility to interference, -
5:41 - 5:44what happens when you take your brain
and you expose it to this? -
5:44 - 5:46(Laughter)
-
5:47 - 5:50You're probably all familiar
with most of these things. -
5:50 - 5:52The last several decades
have literally seen -
5:52 - 5:56an explosion of the diversity
and accessibility of electronic media, -
5:56 - 5:59and the devices that deliver it,
many of which are portable. -
6:00 - 6:03Probably most of you have a computer
in your pockets right now -
6:03 - 6:05that allows you to access
this at any moment. -
6:05 - 6:08What's even more profound,
it's not the change in technology, -
6:08 - 6:12but historically one media
has replaced other medias. -
6:12 - 6:13That's not what's happening now.
-
6:13 - 6:16Now, data shows,
I'm sure you're all aware of it, -
6:16 - 6:18that people are using medias
at the same time. -
6:19 - 6:20Approximately 95 percent of people
-
6:20 - 6:23report using more than one
form of media at a time, -
6:23 - 6:27and that activity takes place
almost 30 percent of your days. -
6:28 - 6:31Children might probably
have this even more so, -
6:31 - 6:33although it's just beginning
to be investigated now. -
6:33 - 6:36Almost an obsessive media
multitasking frenzy - -
6:36 - 6:37you're almost uncomfortable
-
6:37 - 6:41when taken away from your ability
to interact with technology in this way. -
6:41 - 6:44Before I move forward, I want to say
that I'm a friend of technology, -
6:44 - 6:49I'm not anti-technology; I use all these
wonderful toys as well as you do. -
6:49 - 6:52Here I am, working on this talk;
I've two large computer monitors, -
6:52 - 6:56email on the left side,
cell phone in hand accepting texts, -
6:56 - 6:58music playing in the background.
-
6:58 - 7:01This is how we all interact
with technology, at least to some degree. -
7:01 - 7:05Some people have up to six or more forms
of media at any given time. -
7:06 - 7:09Another thing that's changed
are expectations. -
7:09 - 7:11This constant access to communications,
-
7:11 - 7:17computers and data has really changed
societal expectations. -
7:17 - 7:22Now immediate responsiveness
and continuous productivity are expected. -
7:23 - 7:26How many of you are on vacation
and check your email? -
7:26 - 7:30A little dark in here, but I just see
a lot of movement out there. -
7:30 - 7:32So I'll take that
as a resounding ''yes,'' right. -
7:32 - 7:35So we know that our ability to disconnect,
-
7:35 - 7:38to really be present in what
we're experiencing is decreasing, -
7:38 - 7:40even when we're "taking vacations,"
-
7:40 - 7:43the break from all of this
constant productivity. -
7:45 - 7:48So, we should ask the question:
how do we function at all? -
7:48 - 7:51Given those limitations
and how we interact with technology, -
7:51 - 7:53how are we capable of getting
anything done? -
7:54 - 7:57I want to tell you about
the process of cognitive control. -
7:57 - 8:00Cognitive control is what allows us
to function at least to some degree -
8:00 - 8:02under these circumstances.
-
8:02 - 8:04How we perceive the world
is not a passive process. -
8:04 - 8:07Our environment just
doesn't flood into our brains. -
8:07 - 8:09It's sculpted and shaped by our attention.
-
8:09 - 8:11Two types of attention:
-
8:11 - 8:14One attention is external,
stimulus-driven attention, -
8:14 - 8:18it is the environment imposing
itself upon how you perceive the world. -
8:18 - 8:21If there's a flash of light, a loud sound,
-
8:21 - 8:23even if someone calls your name
quietly behind you, -
8:23 - 8:25you pay attention,
you redirect your attention. -
8:25 - 8:29Anything that's very salient or novel
demands your attention -
8:29 - 8:31and increases your perception of it.
-
8:31 - 8:32The other type of attention -
-
8:32 - 8:35the environment coming in
is also known as bottom-up attention. -
8:35 - 8:38The other type is internal,
goal-directed attention, -
8:38 - 8:39what you're doing now.
-
8:39 - 8:41You're choosing to focus
your cognitive resources -
8:41 - 8:44on what you're hearing
and what you're seeing. -
8:44 - 8:46This is known as top-down attention,
-
8:46 - 8:47your goal-directed attention.
-
8:47 - 8:51All of your interactions
with the environment involve this balance -
8:51 - 8:55between these two forms of attention
that are dictating your perception. -
8:55 - 9:00And now we know from a lot of research,
a lot of it from our lab also, -
9:00 - 9:05is that this influence then goes on
and influences what you remember, -
9:05 - 9:08both in the short-term
and in the long-term. -
9:09 - 9:13So what happens when all this interference
that we are being exposed to now -
9:13 - 9:17exceeds the capabilities
of our cognitive control? -
9:18 - 9:22Well, what we now realize is
that there is a broad and deep influence: -
9:23 - 9:28safety, family, our social development,
workplace, education. -
9:28 - 9:30All of these things are affected.
-
9:30 - 9:33There's not nearly enough time
in this talk to talk about these -
9:33 - 9:37since each one of these can have
their own lecture associated with them, -
9:37 - 9:40showing you how broad this issue is.
-
9:40 - 9:43What I'm going to talk to you
about in detail is cognition, -
9:43 - 9:47because this is what we study in our lab,
specifically memory. -
9:47 - 9:50There's a really beautiful quote
by Samuel Johnson, -
9:51 - 9:54who was a British author in the 1700s.
-
9:54 - 9:57He wasn't a psychologist,
he wasn't a neuroscientist, -
9:57 - 9:58but he was incredibly insightful.
-
9:58 - 10:02He said something that really showed
a profound awareness, -
10:02 - 10:05because there was no data
on this at the time. -
10:05 - 10:09What he said is that "the true art
of memory is the art of attention," -
10:09 - 10:11that "no man will read
with much advantage -
10:11 - 10:14who is not able, at pleasure,
to evacuate his mind," -
10:14 - 10:17that "if the repositories
of thought are already full, -
10:17 - 10:18what can they receive?"
-
10:19 - 10:21And "if the mind is employed
on the past or the future, -
10:21 - 10:24the book will be held
before the eyes in vain." -
10:24 - 10:26There's a full career
of research studies here. -
10:26 - 10:29We're slowly working our way through these
-
10:29 - 10:32to show what's going on
in the brain that leads to this, -
10:32 - 10:34and how true is this to our behavior.
-
10:35 - 10:37So, I figured the easiest way to do this
-
10:37 - 10:40is to have you perform
one of our experiments right now, -
10:40 - 10:42so you can get an idea of what we do.
-
10:42 - 10:45And while we do this,
we'll record brain activities. -
10:45 - 10:47You'll do a less demanding
version of this, -
10:47 - 10:49where you're not shoved
inside one of our scanners. -
10:50 - 10:54What we are going have you do now is
"Remember a face," very simple experiment. -
10:55 - 10:56One face will come up,
-
10:56 - 10:58it comes up pretty quickly,
so be ready for it. -
10:58 - 11:01Then around 7 seconds will pass,
there will be an X on the screen, -
11:01 - 11:04and you should remember
the face - just one face. -
11:04 - 11:06Then another face comes up,
and your job is be to say, -
11:06 - 11:09"Yes, that's the face I saw,"
or "No, that's a new face." -
11:09 - 11:11O.K.? Ready to give it a shot?
-
11:21 - 11:22(Audience) No.
-
11:22 - 11:23No. The answer is no.
-
11:23 - 11:25If any of you thinking yes,
-
11:25 - 11:26do not be nervous,
-
11:26 - 11:28no reason to make a clinic
appointment right now. -
11:28 - 11:32It takes practice to get used to this,
people don't always get a 100 percent. -
11:32 - 11:36I'm going to have you do another version
of this experiment, it's the same thing - -
11:36 - 11:37see a face, after a period of time
-
11:37 - 11:40another face'll come up
and you'll have to see if it matches. -
11:40 - 11:43But in this experiment,
another face will pop up in the middle, -
11:43 - 11:47and for that face you'll have to
make a decision: is it a male over 20? -
11:47 - 11:50Then you go back to the business
of remembering that first face. -
11:50 - 11:51Does that make sense?
-
11:51 - 11:52(Laughter)
-
11:52 - 11:55Remember one face and make
a decision on the middle face. -
11:55 - 11:56Pretty simple. Give it a shot.
-
12:05 - 12:07(Audience) Yes.
-
12:07 - 12:08It was the same face.
-
12:08 - 12:11So, did this seem a little harder to you?
-
12:11 - 12:12(Audience) Yes.
-
12:12 - 12:13It is a little harder.
-
12:13 - 12:17If you study even healthy 20-30-year-olds,
you see that the performance diminishes -
12:17 - 12:20when you introduce that second task,
-
12:20 - 12:22even though it's not a hard experiment.
-
12:22 - 12:24Not only that, but if you introduce
a face in the middle -
12:24 - 12:27that's totally irrelevant,
your performance also drops - -
12:27 - 12:29very subtle, but consistently.
-
12:29 - 12:31If you happen to be older than 60,
-
12:31 - 12:34your performance drops
even more from the distraction -
12:34 - 12:36and even more so from the interruption.
-
12:36 - 12:40So we see that there's an exaggerated
effect with age of this impact -
12:40 - 12:42that's even there in 20- and 30-year-olds.
-
12:42 - 12:46We are now looking at how this type
of distraction and interruptions -
12:46 - 12:48might affect your long-term memory.
-
12:48 - 12:53So we did an experiment where we showed
our participants a series of 168 pictures -
12:53 - 12:55that had different numbers of objects.
-
12:55 - 13:00So what you see here are 3 crowns,
4 couches, that's 4 vacuum cleaners - -
13:00 - 13:03though they look like Statues of Liberty
every time I look at that slide. -
13:03 - 13:06But you see all these pictures,
and then, after an hour, -
13:06 - 13:08you go inside our MRI scanner
-
13:08 - 13:10so we could see what's
happening in your brain, -
13:10 - 13:12and you hear these names:
-
13:12 - 13:13"Crown," and when you hear "Crown,"
-
13:13 - 13:16you have to press the button
on how many you remember seeing. -
13:16 - 13:19Here the correct answer would be 3,
or it might be new, -
13:19 - 13:21maybe we never showed that one before.
-
13:21 - 13:24The interesting thing is that we look
at what happens to your memory -
13:24 - 13:26when your eyes are shut,
-
13:26 - 13:28when they are open
looking at a grey screen, -
13:28 - 13:31and when your eyes are open
looking at a busy visual picture. -
13:31 - 13:34And what we found
is that the detailed quality, -
13:34 - 13:37your recollection of those details decline
-
13:37 - 13:40just by having your eyes open
looking at a picture, -
13:40 - 13:42which is basically
what you do all day long. -
13:42 - 13:45Just that very simple stimulus,
-
13:45 - 13:47that you have nothing to do
besides remember, -
13:47 - 13:49declines just by having your eyes open.
-
13:50 - 13:52Later, you should do this experiment:
-
13:52 - 13:55Go up to a significant other
or a friend, look them in the eyes, -
13:55 - 13:58and ask them to tell you in detail
what they had for dinner last night. -
13:58 - 14:02Almost invariably, what you'll see
is that they look away from you. -
14:02 - 14:05What we think is going on
from looking at the brain data -
14:05 - 14:08is that people are looking for
a quiet place in their environment -
14:08 - 14:09to recreate this memory.
-
14:09 - 14:11Just looking at your face
-
14:11 - 14:13is too distracting
to do this at a high level. -
14:13 - 14:14(Laughter)
-
14:15 - 14:16Nothing personal.
-
14:17 - 14:20We did another experiment,
where it was basically the same, -
14:20 - 14:23but now it was done in silence,
or we went into a restaurant -
14:23 - 14:26and we taped the normal chatter
that occurs in a restaurant, -
14:26 - 14:29and we found the same decline in memory.
-
14:29 - 14:30The point of this
-
14:30 - 14:33is not that you should walk around
with blindfolds on and earplugs in. -
14:33 - 14:37The point is to show you
how exquisitely sensitive our memory is -
14:38 - 14:41even to the normal environmental
stimulation that we cannot escape. -
14:41 - 14:42So you can imagine
-
14:42 - 14:46when you layer upon all the normal
complexity that exists in our environment. -
14:46 - 14:49But we do this, the reason
we make such simple experiments -
14:49 - 14:51is we're recording brain activity
with these tools: -
14:51 - 14:56functional MRI that lets us look at blood
flow correlates of neural activity -
14:56 - 14:58and lets us see where in the brain
events are occurring; -
14:58 - 15:03as well as EEG, in which we're looking at
electrical signatures of neural activity, -
15:03 - 15:05where we can see
when events are occurring. -
15:05 - 15:06And we do these tools
-
15:06 - 15:10to understand what happens in our brain
when we have interference -
15:10 - 15:12and how it diminishes our abilities.
-
15:13 - 15:15I'm just going to summarize this
with a couple of cartoons, -
15:15 - 15:17data from our lab and so many labs
-
15:17 - 15:21to give you the latest understanding
about what's going on. -
15:22 - 15:23This is your brain.
-
15:23 - 15:26The front part is over here on this side,
-
15:26 - 15:29and what we see is
that the prefrontal cortex - -
15:29 - 15:32that's the part of our brain
that makes us most human, -
15:32 - 15:35the part that's evolved the most,
the part that develops the latest, -
15:35 - 15:39it's the part that's involved
in this cognitive control. -
15:39 - 15:43And when you're confronted
with distraction, it acts as a bouncer. -
15:43 - 15:46It's saying, "What information
is on the guest list?" -
15:46 - 15:49And through its connectivity in a network
with visual parts of the brain, -
15:49 - 15:51which are in the back,
-
15:51 - 15:53it controls what information gets in.
-
15:53 - 15:55You can see this is
a very busy night club, -
15:55 - 16:00but your visual cortex will only fit
six things or even less at a time, -
16:00 - 16:01depending on how complicated they are.
-
16:02 - 16:05When information gets in
that's not on the guest list, -
16:05 - 16:07you have a cost - your performance drops.
-
16:07 - 16:09This is what changes as we get older:
-
16:09 - 16:12our filter, our ability to block out
information decreases, -
16:12 - 16:15and the degree that it decreases
directly correlates -
16:15 - 16:20with our inability to remember the things
that we're trying to remember - our goals. -
16:20 - 16:23What happens in terms of multitasking?
-
16:23 - 16:25Once again, the prefrontal
cortex is in charge. -
16:25 - 16:27But here it's acting
as a flight controller. -
16:27 - 16:31It's determining what information
is the priority right now, -
16:31 - 16:34and through its connections
with the back of the brain, -
16:34 - 16:35it's making decisions.
-
16:35 - 16:36So, if you're crazy enough
-
16:36 - 16:40to be riding a bike through Manhattan
next to a cab, texting, -
16:40 - 16:42you have a lot of decisions to make.
-
16:42 - 16:45So, maybe, the first decision
is to focus on the traffic, -
16:45 - 16:49and then you think that it's safe enough
to now continue your text message, -
16:49 - 16:51and so you do that.
-
16:51 - 16:55But what you don't do is this:
you don't split those decisions. -
16:55 - 16:58The prefrontal cortex has
what's known as a central bottleneck. -
16:58 - 17:01And so what you do is switch.
-
17:01 - 17:03Your prefrontal cortex
switches between tasks -
17:03 - 17:06even though it feels like
you're doing more than one thing. -
17:06 - 17:09This is what's led this being referred to
as the myth of multitasking - -
17:09 - 17:11that you're not really doing
more than one thing -
17:11 - 17:13unless they become very automated,
-
17:13 - 17:14gum chewing, walking.
-
17:15 - 17:18Although even those have been found
to have some interference with each other, -
17:18 - 17:19believe it or not.
-
17:19 - 17:21(Laughter)
-
17:21 - 17:24With each switch, there is a time delay,
and this leads to a cost - -
17:24 - 17:26an impact on performance.
-
17:26 - 17:29You do not do two things
as well as you do one thing -
17:29 - 17:33if you switch back and forth between them,
and this gets worse as you get older. -
17:33 - 17:38We just had a paper published last week
that shows that as you get older, -
17:38 - 17:40this switching, the letting go,
-
17:40 - 17:43the disengaging and
the reengaging become slower, -
17:43 - 17:46and this creates the
interference in your memory. -
17:47 - 17:48Why do we do it?
-
17:48 - 17:51We get the impression,
and we have lots of data now, -
17:51 - 17:53that this is negative on our performance.
-
17:54 - 17:56There is not a lot of data for this,
some of this is anecdotal, -
17:57 - 17:59based on my impressions,
but it's a resonable place to start. -
18:00 - 18:01Multitasking.
-
18:01 - 18:03We have this sense it gives flexibility,
-
18:03 - 18:05fresh perspective, increased variety,
-
18:05 - 18:07it enables us to use
downtime productively, -
18:08 - 18:11but probably the most salient aspect
is that it's just more fun. -
18:11 - 18:14We are novelty seeking creatures.
-
18:14 - 18:17It's a very strong part
of our evolution to seek out new. -
18:17 - 18:20It stimulates the dopamine system,
the reward system. -
18:20 - 18:24There is no doubt that one chunk of time
in which you're multitasking -
18:24 - 18:28has more novelty than the same
period of time in unitasking. -
18:28 - 18:29And so we probably seek it out.
-
18:29 - 18:33There's even been a question that maybe
it's even addicting at a certain level -
18:34 - 18:38when you're constantly pressured
to get back into a new task -
18:38 - 18:40once you become used to it.
-
18:40 - 18:43How about distraction?
It's a little more complicated. -
18:43 - 18:46Why do people go into noisy coffee shops
to read and to work? -
18:46 - 18:48Does anyone do that in this room?
-
18:48 - 18:49Right, it's very common.
-
18:49 - 18:51Usually you know
which type of person you are. -
18:51 - 18:56So, it's something that's interesting -
all this we're exploring in our lab now -
18:56 - 19:00to understand the driving force of this,
and are there possible benefits? -
19:00 - 19:02So, what can be done about this?
-
19:02 - 19:04It's just too dreary
to say all these negative things -
19:04 - 19:06and then to walk off the stage.
-
19:07 - 19:10So, we don't have all the answers,
-
19:10 - 19:12but I can tell you it seems
there are two paths, -
19:12 - 19:13at least to me:
-
19:13 - 19:16we could change our behavior,
or we could change our brains. -
19:16 - 19:17(Laughter)
-
19:17 - 19:20Not necessarily in this scary way
I'm depicting over here, -
19:20 - 19:21but in a positive way.
-
19:21 - 19:25So, we could change our behavior
because someone forces us to, right? -
19:25 - 19:30They say it's too dangerous to talk
on the phone or to text while driving, -
19:30 - 19:34so we're not letting you do it anymore;
we give you a ticket if you do that. -
19:34 - 19:36But there's other ways
we can change our behavior -
19:36 - 19:38on things less dangerous to society.
-
19:38 - 19:39So we could make decisions.
-
19:39 - 19:42Just because we have
all these wonderful technologies -
19:42 - 19:45doesn't mean we have to use it
all at exactly the same time. -
19:45 - 19:48We get to make decisions
based upon what we now understand -
19:49 - 19:52about how this interaction
with technology and with ourselves -
19:52 - 19:53changes our behavior.
-
19:53 - 19:57So I'm going to tell you what I do;
I will not tell you what to do. -
19:57 - 19:58Since I've researched this,
-
19:58 - 20:01I felt that I needed to make
some decisions about my own behavior, -
20:01 - 20:03and this is what I decided:
-
20:03 - 20:05When working on something very important,
-
20:05 - 20:08something that demands high quality,
especially something time sensitive, -
20:08 - 20:11I do one thing at a time,
singular attention, -
20:11 - 20:15I quit my email - too distracting
with messages coming in - -
20:15 - 20:16I turn off my phone,
-
20:16 - 20:21I close my door, I do one thing,
and I find it quite enjoyable. -
20:21 - 20:24I actually had stopped
doing that for quite a while. -
20:24 - 20:25But I don't do this all the time.
-
20:26 - 20:28There is many things in my day
that are very boring, -
20:28 - 20:30I know they have to get done,
-
20:30 - 20:32but I know if I try to do
one thing at a time, -
20:32 - 20:33I would never finish it.
-
20:33 - 20:36So I might set aside three hours
of just intense multitasking; -
20:36 - 20:39the more I'm switching, the better;
it just keeps it moving along. -
20:39 - 20:42So, it's not a message
that technology is bad, don't do this. -
20:42 - 20:45It's just the point
that decisions need to be made -
20:45 - 20:47when we interact
and our environment changes. -
20:47 - 20:49It's happened many times in the past,
-
20:49 - 20:52this is just another example that we
have to learn how to deal with better. -
20:53 - 20:54Changing our brains.
-
20:54 - 20:57Our brains are capable
of a tremendous degree of plasticity. -
20:57 - 21:01That means it adapts and modifies
to stimuli from the environment. -
21:01 - 21:04And we now know that this
is not just when you're a child, -
21:04 - 21:05it occurs through your entire life.
-
21:05 - 21:10We are looking at exercises - there's
a video game that we developed in our lab -
21:10 - 21:14to see if we can strengthen
these abilities through practice. -
21:14 - 21:17So, in this case, it's a car driving game
and you're interacting with the road, -
21:17 - 21:21signs come up, sometimes relevant
and sometimes irrelevant, -
21:21 - 21:24and you're pressing buttons,
moving, challenging yourself -
21:24 - 21:26in an adaptive way,
in a way that's fun. -
21:26 - 21:31What's interesting about this video game -
it was designed to be played in our lab -
21:31 - 21:34while recording brain activity
and seeing what's going on in our brain -
21:34 - 21:37while you interact with
all this hype of interference. -
21:37 - 21:40We have a study going on now
with healthy older adults, -
21:40 - 21:42they take home a laptop,
-
21:42 - 21:45and they play this game at home,
and while they play it, -
21:45 - 21:48their behavioral performance data
downloads to their computer -
21:48 - 21:50and straight from Dropbox
right into our lab, -
21:50 - 21:52almost in real time,
we can get their brain data, -
21:52 - 21:53their behavioral data,
-
21:53 - 21:55while they are playing at home.
-
21:55 - 21:58Then they come into the lab and we look
what's changed in their brain -
21:58 - 22:00to enable them to function
at a higher level, -
22:00 - 22:02which is what we're seeing -
-
22:02 - 22:03brains are capable of plasticity.
-
22:03 - 22:07We're now looking at wireless
EEG bluetooth headsets -
22:07 - 22:11that allow us to have our
older participants put these on at home -
22:11 - 22:12and record their brain activity,
-
22:12 - 22:16so we could move our lab outside
of our laboratory into people's homes -
22:16 - 22:18and record what are
the changes in the brain -
22:18 - 22:21that lead to this higher
ability with practice. -
22:21 - 22:23And then they can come back into the lab,
-
22:23 - 22:25and we can use fMRI
to look at these changes in networks. -
22:25 - 22:28I think it's an exciting time
where we're learning more -
22:28 - 22:30about how our brains can adapt to this.
-
22:30 - 22:32So with that, I want to thank you
for your attention -
22:32 - 22:35and, of course, your lack
of attention to other things. -
22:35 - 22:40(Applause)
- Title:
- Brain: memory and multitasking | Adam Gazzaley, MD, PhD | TEDxSanJoseCA
- Description:
-
Dr. Adam Gazzaley gives an informative talk on the complexities of our brains. The data presented in the talk is based upon the recent studies in neuroscience with well-known tools, MRI scanner being one of them.
Dr. Gazzaley explains so well and everyone is engaged and able to participate in the live experiment Remember the Face.
The talk touches upon attention, memory, multitasking, unitasking etc.Dr. Gazzaley is a founder and director of Neuroscape, a neuroscience center devoted to technology development and research.
He was featured in the New York Times, the WSJ, the Times, CNN, and NBC Nightly News.
Dr. Gazzaley has received many awards, including Science Educator Award.This talk was given at a TEDx event using the TED conference format but independently organized by a local community. Learn more at http://ted.com/tedx
- Video Language:
- English
- Team:
- closed TED
- Project:
- TEDxTalks
- Duration:
- 22:41
Peter van de Ven approved English subtitles for Brain: memory and multitasking | Adam Gazzaley, MD, PhD | TEDxSanJoseCA | ||
Peter van de Ven accepted English subtitles for Brain: memory and multitasking | Adam Gazzaley, MD, PhD | TEDxSanJoseCA | ||
Peter van de Ven edited English subtitles for Brain: memory and multitasking | Adam Gazzaley, MD, PhD | TEDxSanJoseCA | ||
Mariya Zhdanova edited English subtitles for Brain: memory and multitasking | Adam Gazzaley, MD, PhD | TEDxSanJoseCA | ||
Peter van de Ven declined English subtitles for Brain: memory and multitasking | Adam Gazzaley, MD, PhD | TEDxSanJoseCA | ||
Peter van de Ven edited English subtitles for Brain: memory and multitasking | Adam Gazzaley, MD, PhD | TEDxSanJoseCA | ||
Peter van de Ven edited English subtitles for Brain: memory and multitasking | Adam Gazzaley, MD, PhD | TEDxSanJoseCA | ||
Peter van de Ven edited English subtitles for Brain: memory and multitasking | Adam Gazzaley, MD, PhD | TEDxSanJoseCA |