Epigenetics - our bodies' way to change the destiny written in our DNA | Moshe Szyf | TEDxBratislava
-
0:15 - 0:19So, it all came to life
in a dark bar in Madrid, -
0:19 - 0:25and as I was stepping into the bar,
I encountered my colleague from McGill, -
0:25 - 0:26Michael Meaney.
-
0:27 - 0:29And we're drinking a few beers,
-
0:30 - 0:34and like scientists do,
he told me about his work. -
0:34 - 0:39He told me that he is interested
in how mother rats -
0:39 - 0:43lick their pups after they are born.
-
0:44 - 0:49And I was sitting there and saying,
"This is where my tax dollars are wasted, -
0:49 - 0:50(Laughter)
-
0:50 - 0:53on this kind of soft science."
-
0:54 - 0:58But as the beer got more intense
and the alcohol gets into the brain, -
0:58 - 1:02you become more receptive,
and he started telling me -
1:02 - 1:08that the rats, like humans,
lick their pups in very different ways. -
1:08 - 1:11Some mothers do a lot of that,
-
1:11 - 1:13some mothers do very little,
-
1:13 - 1:16and most are in-between.
-
1:16 - 1:18But what's interesting about it
-
1:18 - 1:23is that when he follows these pups
when they become adults, -
1:23 - 1:27like years in human life,
long after their mother has died, -
1:27 - 1:29they are completely different animals.
-
1:29 - 1:33The animals that were licked
and groomed heavily - -
1:33 - 1:38the high licking and grooming -
are not stressed, -
1:38 - 1:43they have different sexual behavior,
they have a different way of living, -
1:43 - 1:49than those that were not treated
as intensively by her mother. -
1:50 - 1:52So, then I was thinking to myself,
-
1:53 - 1:54Is this magic?
-
1:55 - 1:56How does this work?
-
1:56 - 1:58I'm a biochemist.
-
1:58 - 2:02I believe that there are chemical
explanations to nature. -
2:03 - 2:07I was working in a field
called 'epigenetics,' -
2:08 - 2:14but before I jumped into that conclusion,
we had to do another experiment. -
2:14 - 2:19"Is this genetic?" a geneticist
would like you to think. -
2:19 - 2:23Perhaps the mother
had the 'bad mother' gene -
2:23 - 2:27that caused her pups to be stressful,
-
2:27 - 2:30and then it was passed
from generation to generation; -
2:30 - 2:32it's all determined by genetics.
-
2:32 - 2:36Or is it possible that something else
is going on here? -
2:36 - 2:39In rats, we can ask
this question and answer it. -
2:39 - 2:43So, what we did
is a cross-fostering experiment. -
2:43 - 2:48You essentially separate the litter,
the babies of this rat, at birth, -
2:48 - 2:50to two kinds of fostering mothers,
-
2:50 - 2:53not the real mothers,
but mothers that will take care of them: -
2:53 - 2:55high-licking mothers
and low-licking mothers. -
2:55 - 2:59And you can do the opposite
with the low-licking pups. -
3:00 - 3:02And the remarkable answer was,
-
3:02 - 3:06it wasn't important what gene
you got from your mother. -
3:06 - 3:12It was not the biological mother
that defined this property of these rats, -
3:12 - 3:16it is the mother
that took care of the pups. -
3:17 - 3:20So, how can this work?
-
3:20 - 3:23And as I told you, I am an epigeneticist.
-
3:23 - 3:27I am interested in how genes are marked
-
3:27 - 3:30by a chemical mark during embryogenesis,
-
3:30 - 3:33during the time we're in
the womb of our mothers, -
3:33 - 3:37and decide which gene
will be expressed in what tissue. -
3:37 - 3:41Different genes are expressed in the brain
than in the liver and the eye. -
3:42 - 3:43And we thought,
-
3:43 - 3:49is it possible that the mother
is somehow reprogramming -
3:49 - 3:53the gene of her offspring
through her behavior? -
3:53 - 3:55We spent ten years,
-
3:55 - 3:59and we found that there
is a cascade of biochemical events -
3:59 - 4:02by which the licking and grooming
of the mother, the care of the mother, -
4:02 - 4:08is translated to biochemical signals
that go into the nucleus and into the DNA, -
4:08 - 4:10and program it differently.
-
4:10 - 4:15So now the animal
can prepare itself for life. -
4:15 - 4:18Is life going to be harsh?
-
4:18 - 4:20Is there going to be a lot of food?
-
4:20 - 4:22Are there going to be
a lot of cats and snakes around? -
4:22 - 4:25Or will I live in
an upper class neighborhood -
4:25 - 4:28where all I have to do
is behave well and proper, -
4:28 - 4:31and that will gain me social acceptance?
-
4:31 - 4:38And now, one can think about how important
that process can be for our lives. -
4:38 - 4:41We inherit our DNA from our ancestors.
-
4:42 - 4:46The DNA is old;
it evolved during evolution. -
4:46 - 4:51But it doesn't tell us if you
are going to be born in Stockholm, -
4:51 - 4:54where the days are long in summer
and short in the winter, -
4:54 - 4:58or in Ecuador, where there are an equal
number of hours for day and night -
4:58 - 5:03all year around, and that has such
an enormous [impact] on our physiology. -
5:04 - 5:08So, what we suggest is perhaps
what happens early in life, -
5:08 - 5:12those signals that come
through the mother tell the child -
5:12 - 5:15what kind of social world
you are going to be living in. -
5:15 - 5:18Is it going to be harsh and you better
be anxious and be stressful? -
5:18 - 5:21Or is it going to be an easy world
and you have to be different? -
5:21 - 5:24Is it going to be a world
with a lot of light or a little light? -
5:25 - 5:28Is it going to be a world
with a lot of food or a little food? -
5:29 - 5:30If there's no food around,
-
5:30 - 5:34you better develop your brain
to binge whenever you see a meal, -
5:35 - 5:40or store every piece of food
that you have as fat. -
5:41 - 5:44So, this is good;
evolution has selected this -
5:44 - 5:51to allow our fixed old DNA to function
in a dynamic way in new environments. -
5:52 - 5:54But sometimes things can go wrong.
-
5:55 - 6:00For example, if you're born
to a poor family and the signals are -
6:00 - 6:05'You better binge, you better eat every
piece of food you're going to encounter.' -
6:05 - 6:07But now we humans,
in our brain, have evolved, -
6:07 - 6:09have changed evolution even faster.
-
6:09 - 6:13Now you can buy
a McDonald's [hamburger] for $1.00. -
6:13 - 6:19And therefore, the preparation
that we had by our mothers -
6:19 - 6:22is turning out to be maladaptive.
-
6:23 - 6:28The same preparation that was supposed to
protect us from hunger and famine -
6:28 - 6:32is going to cause obesity,
cardiovascular problems, -
6:32 - 6:34and metabolic disease.
-
6:34 - 6:37So, this concept that genes
could be marked by our experience, -
6:37 - 6:40especially the early life experience,
-
6:40 - 6:45can provide us a unifying explanation
of both health and disease. -
6:46 - 6:48But is it true only for rats?
-
6:48 - 6:51The problem is, we cannot
test this in humans, -
6:51 - 6:55because ethically, we cannot administer
childhood adversity in a random way. -
6:55 - 6:59So, if a poor child
develops a certain property, -
6:59 - 7:02we don't know whether
this is caused by poverty, -
7:02 - 7:05or whether poor people have bad genes.
-
7:05 - 7:07So, geneticists will try to tell you
-
7:07 - 7:10that poor people are poor
because their genes made them poor. -
7:11 - 7:14Epigeneticists will tell you
poor people are in a bad environment, -
7:14 - 7:17or impoverished environment
-
7:17 - 7:20that creates that phenotype,
that property. -
7:21 - 7:27So, we moved to look
into our cousins, the monkeys. -
7:27 - 7:32My colleague Stephen Suomi has been
rearing monkeys in two different ways. -
7:32 - 7:35Randomly separated
the monkey from the mother -
7:35 - 7:41and reared her with a nurse
in surrogate motherhood conditions. -
7:41 - 7:43So, these monkeys didn't have a mother,
they had a nurse. -
7:43 - 7:48And other monkeys were reared
with their normal, natural mothers. -
7:48 - 7:53And when they were old,
they were completely different animals. -
7:53 - 7:56The monkeys that had a mother
would not care about alcohol, -
7:56 - 7:57they were not sexually aggressive.
-
7:57 - 8:01The monkeys that didn't have a mother
were aggressive, were stressed, -
8:01 - 8:03and were alcoholics.
-
8:04 - 8:08So, we looked at their DNA
early after birth, -
8:08 - 8:12to see, is it possible
that the mother is marking? -
8:12 - 8:17There is a signature of the mother
in the DNA of the offspring. -
8:17 - 8:20These are day-14 monkeys,
-
8:20 - 8:24and what you see here is the modern way
by which we study epigenetics. -
8:24 - 8:28We can now map those chemical marks,
which we call methylation marks, -
8:28 - 8:34on DNA at a single nucleotide resolution,
we can map the entire genome. -
8:34 - 8:37We can now compare the monkey
that had a mother and not. -
8:37 - 8:39And here is a visual presentation of this.
-
8:39 - 8:43What you see is the genes
that got more methylated are red; -
8:43 - 8:46the genes that got
less methylated are green. -
8:46 - 8:49You can see many genes are changing.
-
8:49 - 8:51Because not having a mother
is not just one thing, -
8:51 - 8:53if affects the whole way.
-
8:53 - 8:56It sends us signals about the whole way
your world is going to look -
8:56 - 8:58when you become an adult,
-
8:58 - 9:03and you can see the two groups of monkeys
extremely well separated from each other. -
9:03 - 9:07How early does this develop?
-
9:07 - 9:09These monkeys already
didn't see their mother -
9:09 - 9:11so they had a social experience.
-
9:11 - 9:15Do we sense our social status
even at the moment of birth? -
9:16 - 9:17So, in this experiment,
-
9:17 - 9:22we took placentas of monkeys
that had different social status. -
9:22 - 9:28What's interesting about social rank,
is that across all living beings, -
9:28 - 9:31they will structure
themselves by hierarchy. -
9:31 - 9:33Monkey number one is the boss.
-
9:33 - 9:35Monkey number four is the peon.
-
9:36 - 9:40And you put four monkeys in a cage,
there will always be a boss, -
9:40 - 9:41and always be a peon.
-
9:42 - 9:46And, what's interesting,
is that monkey number one -
9:46 - 9:49is much healthier than monkey number four.
-
9:49 - 9:55And if you put them in a cage,
monkey number one will not eat as much, -
9:56 - 9:58monkey number four will eat as much.
-
9:59 - 10:03And what you see here
in this methylation mapping, -
10:03 - 10:06the animals that had a high social status,
-
10:06 - 10:09versus the animals
that did not have a high status. -
10:09 - 10:13So, we are born already knowing
the social information, -
10:13 - 10:16and that social information
is not bad or good, -
10:16 - 10:18it just prepares us for life
-
10:18 - 10:22because we have to program
our biology differently -
10:22 - 10:25if we're in a high or low social status.
-
10:26 - 10:28But how can you study this in humans?
-
10:29 - 10:32We can't do experiments;
we can't administer adversity to humans. -
10:32 - 10:37But God does experiments with humans,
and it's called natural disasters. -
10:37 - 10:42One of the hardest natural disasters
in Canadian history -
10:42 - 10:45happened in my province of Quebec.
-
10:45 - 10:47It's the ice storm of 1998.
-
10:47 - 10:51We lost our entire electrical grid
because of an ice storm -
10:51 - 10:54when the temperatures were
in the dead of winter in Quebec, -
10:54 - 10:59-20 to -30, and there were
pregnant mothers during that time. -
10:59 - 11:05And my colleague, Suzanne King,
followed the children of these mothers -
11:05 - 11:07for 15 years.
-
11:08 - 11:12And what happened was
that as the stress increased, -
11:12 - 11:15and here we had objective
measures of stress: -
11:15 - 11:17How long you were without power;
-
11:17 - 11:19where did you spend your time?
-
11:19 - 11:24Was it in your mothers-in-law apartment
or in some posh country home? -
11:24 - 11:27All these added up
to a social stress scale -
11:27 - 11:31and you can ask the question,
how did the children look? -
11:31 - 11:36It appears that as stress increases,
the children develop more autism, -
11:36 - 11:42they develop more metabolic diseases,
and they develop more autoimmune diseases. -
11:42 - 11:44And we would map the methylation state
-
11:44 - 11:50and again, you see the green genes
becoming red as stress increases. -
11:50 - 11:53The red genes becoming green
as stress increases, -
11:53 - 11:58an entire rearrangement of the genome
in response to stress. -
11:59 - 12:02So, if we can program genes,
-
12:02 - 12:06if we are not just the slaves
of the history of our genes, -
12:06 - 12:09but they can be programmed,
can we deprogram them? -
12:10 - 12:14Because epigenetic causes
can cause diseases like cancer, -
12:14 - 12:18metabolic disease
and mental health diseases. -
12:18 - 12:21Let's talk about cocaine addiction.
-
12:22 - 12:25Cocaine addiction is a terrible situation,
-
12:25 - 12:29that can lead to death
and to loss of human life. -
12:30 - 12:35We ask the question,
can we reprogram the addicted brain -
12:35 - 12:40to make that animal non-addicted anymore?
-
12:41 - 12:47We used a cocaine addiction model
that recapitulates what happens in humans. -
12:47 - 12:52In humans, you're in high school,
some friends suggest you use some cocaine, -
12:52 - 12:54you take cocaine, nothing happens.
-
12:54 - 12:58Months pass by; something reminds you
of what happened the first time, -
12:58 - 13:01a pusher pushes cocaine,
and you become addicted, -
13:01 - 13:03and your life has changed.
-
13:03 - 13:04In rats, we do the same thing.
-
13:04 - 13:09My colleague Gal Yadid, he trains
the animals to get used to cocaine, -
13:09 - 13:12then for one month, no cocaine.
-
13:12 - 13:15And then he reminds them of the party
when they saw cocaine the first time -
13:15 - 13:18via cue - the colors of the cage
when they saw cocaine, -
13:18 - 13:20and they go crazy.
-
13:20 - 13:24They will press the lever
to get cocaine till they die. -
13:24 - 13:29We first determined that the difference
between these animals -
13:29 - 13:31is that during that time,
when nothing happens, -
13:31 - 13:35there's no cocaine around,
their epigenome is rearranged, -
13:35 - 13:38their genes are re-marked
in a different way, -
13:38 - 13:40and when the cue comes,
-
13:40 - 13:45their genome is ready
to develop this addictive phenotype. -
13:45 - 13:52So, we treated these animals with drugs
that either increase DNA methylation, -
13:52 - 13:54which was the epigenetic mark to look at,
-
13:54 - 13:57or decrease epigenetic markings.
-
13:57 - 14:01And we found that
if we increase methylation, -
14:01 - 14:05these animals go even crazier,
they have even more craving for cocaine. -
14:05 - 14:11But if we reduce the DNA methylation,
the animals are not addicted anymore, -
14:11 - 14:12we have reprogrammed them.
-
14:12 - 14:17And the fundamental difference between
an epigenetic drug and any other drug -
14:17 - 14:19is that with epigenetic drugs
-
14:19 - 14:23we essentially remove
the science of experience, -
14:23 - 14:25and once they're gone,
-
14:25 - 14:28they will not come back
unless you have the same experience, -
14:28 - 14:30so the animal now is reprogrammed.
-
14:30 - 14:34So, when we visited the animals
30 days, 60 days longer, -
14:34 - 14:37which is, in human terms,
many years of life, -
14:37 - 14:43they were still not addicted
by a single epigenetic treatment. -
14:45 - 14:51So, what we learned about DNA:
the DNA is not just a sequence of letters, -
14:51 - 14:53it's not just a script.
-
14:53 - 14:56DNA is a dynamic movie.
-
14:57 - 15:02Our experiences are being written
into that movie, which is interactive. -
15:02 - 15:07You're like watching a movie of your life,
with the DNA, with your remote control. -
15:07 - 15:11You can remove an actor, and add an actor.
-
15:11 - 15:17So, in spite of the deterministic
nature of genetics, -
15:17 - 15:20you have control
of the way your genes look. -
15:21 - 15:24And this has a tremendous
optimistic message. -
15:24 - 15:28For the ability to now encounter
some of the deadly diseases -
15:28 - 15:33like cancer, mental health,
with a new approach, -
15:33 - 15:36looking at them as maladaptation,
-
15:36 - 15:42that if we can epigenetically intervene,
reverse the movie by removing an actor -
15:43 - 15:46and setting up a new narrative.
-
15:46 - 15:50So, what I told you today is that our DNA
-
15:50 - 15:56is really a combination of two components,
two layers of information. -
15:56 - 16:00One layer of information is old,
-
16:00 - 16:03evolved from millions
of years of evolution; -
16:04 - 16:07it is fixed and very hard to change.
-
16:08 - 16:12The other layer of information
is the epigenetic layer, -
16:12 - 16:15which is open and dynamic,
-
16:15 - 16:19and sets up a narrative
that is interactive. -
16:20 - 16:25So, even though
we are determined by our genes, -
16:25 - 16:28we have a degree of freedom
-
16:28 - 16:32that can set up our life
to a life of responsibility. -
16:32 - 16:34Thank you.
-
16:34 - 16:36(Applause)
- Title:
- Epigenetics - our bodies' way to change the destiny written in our DNA | Moshe Szyf | TEDxBratislava
- Description:
-
What we thought of as something fixed, DNA is actually very cleverly modified according to current environmental needs. Even before a baby is born, it already knows what world to prepare itself for. In this talk, Moshe sheds light on his groundbreaking findings.
Moshe Szyf is one of the pioneers in the field of epigenetics. Szyf’s lab proposed three decades ago that DNA methylation is a prime therapeutic target in cancer and other diseases, and has postulated and provided the first set of evidence that the “social environment” early in life can alter DNA methylation, launching the emerging field of “social epigenetics.” The Szyf lab is interested in understanding basic epigenetic mechanisms and their broad implications in human behavior, health and disease, as well as in developing epigenetics-based therapeutics and diagnostics.
This talk was given at a TEDx event using the TED conference format but independently organized by a local community. Learn more at http://ted.com/tedx
- Video Language:
- English
- Team:
- closed TED
- Project:
- TEDxTalks
- Duration:
- 16:44
Natsuhiko Mizutani
8:17 These are, today, 14 monkeys,
should be
These are day-14 monkeys,
(meaning these are monkey babies).
Ellen
English edited 14/06/17
8:17 today, 14 monkeys, -> day-14 monkeys