< Return to Video

How early life experience is written into DNA

  • 0:01 - 0:03
    So it all came to life
  • 0:03 - 0:05
    in a dark bar in Madrid.
  • 0:05 - 0:09
    I encountered my colleague
    from McGill, Michael Meaney.
  • 0:09 - 0:11
    And we were drinking a few beers,
  • 0:11 - 0:14
    and like scientists do,
  • 0:14 - 0:16
    he told me about his work.
  • 0:16 - 0:19
    And he told me that he is interested
  • 0:19 - 0:21
    in how mother rats
  • 0:21 - 0:23
    lick their pups
  • 0:23 - 0:26
    after they were born.
  • 0:26 - 0:28
    And I was sitting there and saying,
  • 0:28 - 0:32
    this is where my tax dollars are wasted,
  • 0:32 - 0:35
    on this kind of soft science.
  • 0:36 - 0:38
    And he started telling me
  • 0:38 - 0:42
    that when the rats, like humans,
  • 0:42 - 0:45
    lick their pups in very different ways.
  • 0:45 - 0:47
    Some mothers do a lot of that,
  • 0:47 - 0:49
    some mothers do very little,
  • 0:49 - 0:52
    and most are in between.
  • 0:52 - 0:54
    But what's interesting about it
  • 0:54 - 0:59
    is when he follows these pups
    when they become adults,
  • 0:59 - 1:01
    like years in human life,
  • 1:01 - 1:03
    long after their mother died,
  • 1:03 - 1:05
    they are completely different animals.
  • 1:05 - 1:10
    The animals that were licked
    and groomed heavily,
  • 1:10 - 1:12
    the high licking and grooming,
  • 1:12 - 1:14
    are not stressed.
  • 1:14 - 1:16
    They have different sexual behavior.
  • 1:16 - 1:19
    They have a different way of living
  • 1:19 - 1:23
    than those that were not treated
  • 1:23 - 1:26
    as intensively by their mothers.
  • 1:26 - 1:29
    So then I was thinking to myself,
  • 1:29 - 1:31
    is this magic?
  • 1:31 - 1:33
    How does this work?
  • 1:33 - 1:35
    Us geneticists would like you to think
  • 1:35 - 1:39
    perhaps the mother had
    the bad mother gene
  • 1:39 - 1:43
    that caused her pups to be stressful,
  • 1:43 - 1:46
    and then it was passed
    from generation to generation.
  • 1:46 - 1:48
    It's all determined by genetics.
  • 1:48 - 1:50
    Or is it possible that something else
  • 1:50 - 1:52
    is going on here?
  • 1:52 - 1:54
    So in rats we can ask this question
  • 1:54 - 1:55
    and answer it.
  • 1:55 - 1:59
    So what we did is
    a cross-fostering experiment.
  • 1:59 - 2:02
    You essentially separate the litter,
  • 2:02 - 2:04
    the babies of this rat, at birth,
  • 2:04 - 2:06
    to two kinds of fostering mothers,
  • 2:06 - 2:07
    not the real mothers,
  • 2:07 - 2:09
    but mothers that will take care of them,
  • 2:09 - 2:12
    high-licking mothers
    and low-licking mothers.
  • 2:12 - 2:16
    And you can do the opposite
    with the low-licking pups.
  • 2:16 - 2:18
    And the remarkable answer was,
  • 2:18 - 2:20
    it wasn't important
  • 2:20 - 2:22
    what the gene you got from your mother.
  • 2:22 - 2:26
    It was not the biological mother
    that defined this property
  • 2:26 - 2:28
    of these rats.
  • 2:28 - 2:32
    It is the mother that
    took care of the pups.
  • 2:32 - 2:36
    So how can this work?
  • 2:36 - 2:39
    I am an a epigeneticist.
  • 2:39 - 2:41
    I am interested
  • 2:41 - 2:44
    in how genes are marked by a chemical mark
  • 2:44 - 2:49
    during embryogenesis, during the time
    we're in the womb of our mothers,
  • 2:49 - 2:52
    and decide which gene will be expressed
  • 2:52 - 2:53
    in what tissue.
  • 2:53 - 2:57
    Different genes are expressed in the brain
    than in the liver and the eye.
  • 2:57 - 3:00
    And we thought, is it possible
  • 3:00 - 3:03
    that the mother is somehow
  • 3:03 - 3:07
    reprogramming the gene of her offspring
  • 3:07 - 3:09
    through her behavior?
  • 3:09 - 3:12
    And we spent 10 years,
  • 3:12 - 3:15
    and we found that there is
    a cascade of biochemical events
  • 3:15 - 3:18
    by which the licking and grooming
    of the mother, the care of the mother
  • 3:18 - 3:21
    is translated to biochemical signals
  • 3:21 - 3:24
    that go into the nucleus and into the DNA
  • 3:24 - 3:26
    and program it differently.
  • 3:26 - 3:31
    So now the animal can
    prepare itself for life.
  • 3:31 - 3:34
    Is life going to be harsh?
  • 3:34 - 3:36
    Is there going to be a lot of food?
  • 3:36 - 3:38
    Are there going to be a lot
    of cats and snakes around,
  • 3:38 - 3:41
    or will I live in an upper
    class neighborhood
  • 3:41 - 3:45
    where all I have to do
    is behave well and proper
  • 3:45 - 3:47
    and that will gain me social acceptance?
  • 3:47 - 3:53
    And now one can think about
    how important that process can be
  • 3:53 - 3:54
    for our lives.
  • 3:54 - 3:57
    We inherit our DNA from our ancestors.
  • 3:57 - 4:00
    The DNA is old.
  • 4:00 - 4:02
    It evolved during evolution.
  • 4:02 - 4:06
    But it doesn't tell us if you are going
    to be born in Stockholm,
  • 4:06 - 4:10
    where the days are long in the summer
    and short in the winter,
  • 4:10 - 4:14
    or in Ecuador, where equal number
    of hours for day and night
  • 4:14 - 4:15
    all year round.
  • 4:15 - 4:19
    And that has such an enormous amount
    on our physiology.
  • 4:19 - 4:24
    So what we suggest is perhaps
    what happens early in life,
  • 4:24 - 4:26
    those signals that come through the mother
  • 4:26 - 4:27
    tell the child
  • 4:27 - 4:31
    what kind of social world
    you're going to be living in.
  • 4:31 - 4:34
    It will be harsh, and you'd better
    be anxious and be stressful,
  • 4:34 - 4:37
    or it's going to be an easy world,
    and you have to be different.
  • 4:37 - 4:40
    Is it going to be a world
    with a lot of light or little light?
  • 4:40 - 4:44
    Is it going to be a world
    with a lot of food or little food?
  • 4:44 - 4:46
    If there's no food around,
  • 4:46 - 4:50
    you'd better develop your brain
    to binge whenever you see a meal,
  • 4:50 - 4:57
    or store every piece of food
    that you have as fat.
  • 4:57 - 4:59
    So this is good.
  • 4:59 - 5:00
    Evolution has selected this
  • 5:00 - 5:04
    to allow our fixed, old DNA
  • 5:04 - 5:05
    to function in a dynamic way
  • 5:05 - 5:08
    in new environments.
  • 5:08 - 5:10
    But sometimes things can go wrong.
  • 5:10 - 5:15
    For example, if you're born
    to a poor family,
  • 5:15 - 5:17
    and the signals are, "You'd better binge.
  • 5:17 - 5:21
    You'd better eat every piece of food
    you're going to encounter."
  • 5:21 - 5:23
    But now we humans
    and our brain have evolved,
  • 5:23 - 5:25
    have changed evolution even faster.
  • 5:25 - 5:27
    Now you can buy a McDonald's
  • 5:27 - 5:29
    for one dollar.
  • 5:29 - 5:32
    And therefore, the preparation
  • 5:32 - 5:33
    that we had
  • 5:33 - 5:35
    by our mothers
  • 5:35 - 5:38
    is turning to be maladaptive.
  • 5:38 - 5:41
    The same preparation that was
    supposed to protect us
  • 5:41 - 5:43
    from hunger and famine
  • 5:43 - 5:45
    is going to cause obesity,
  • 5:45 - 5:49
    cardiovascular problems,
    and metabolic disease.
  • 5:49 - 5:52
    So this concept that genes
    could be marked by our experience,
  • 5:52 - 5:55
    and especially the early life experience,
  • 5:55 - 5:57
    can provide us a unifying explanation
  • 5:57 - 6:01
    of both health and disease.
  • 6:01 - 6:03
    But is true only for rats?
  • 6:03 - 6:06
    The problem is, we cannot
    test this in humans,
  • 6:06 - 6:09
    because ethically, we cannot
    administer child adversity
  • 6:09 - 6:10
    in a random way.
  • 6:10 - 6:13
    So if a poor child develops
    a certain property,
  • 6:13 - 6:16
    we don't know whether this is caused
  • 6:16 - 6:17
    by poverty
  • 6:17 - 6:20
    or whether poor people have bad genes.
  • 6:20 - 6:23
    So geneticists will try to tell you
    that poor people are poor
  • 6:23 - 6:25
    because their genes make them poor.
  • 6:25 - 6:27
    Epigeneticists will tell you
  • 6:27 - 6:31
    poor people are in a bad environment
    or an impoverished environment
  • 6:31 - 6:35
    that creates that phenotype,
    that property.
  • 6:35 - 6:38
    So we moved to look
  • 6:38 - 6:42
    into our cousins, the monkeys.
  • 6:42 - 6:44
    My colleague Steven Soomey
  • 6:44 - 6:47
    has been rearing monkeys
    in two different ways:
  • 6:47 - 6:50
    randomly separated the monkey
    from the mother
  • 6:50 - 6:53
    and reared her with a nurse,
  • 6:53 - 6:56
    and a surrogate motherhood conditions.
  • 6:56 - 6:57
    So these monkeys didn't have a mother.
  • 6:57 - 6:58
    They had a nurse.
  • 6:58 - 7:00
    And other monkeys
  • 7:00 - 7:03
    were reared with their normal,
    natural mothers,
  • 7:03 - 7:05
    and when they were old,
  • 7:05 - 7:08
    they were completely different animals.
  • 7:08 - 7:09
    The monkeys that had a mother
  • 7:09 - 7:11
    would not care about alcohol,
  • 7:11 - 7:12
    they were not sexually aggressive.
  • 7:12 - 7:14
    The monkeys that didn't have a mother
  • 7:14 - 7:16
    were aggressive, were stressed,
  • 7:16 - 7:19
    and were alcoholics.
  • 7:19 - 7:21
    So we looked at their DNA
  • 7:21 - 7:23
    early after birth
  • 7:23 - 7:27
    and see is it possible
    that the mother is marking.
  • 7:27 - 7:32
    There is a signature of the mother
    in the DNA of the offspring.
  • 7:32 - 7:34
    These are Day 14 monkeys,
  • 7:34 - 7:39
    and what you see here is the modern way
    by which we study epigenetics.
  • 7:39 - 7:43
    We can now map those chemical marks,
    which we call methylation marks,
  • 7:43 - 7:47
    on DNA at a single nucleotide resolution.
  • 7:47 - 7:49
    We can map the entire genome.
  • 7:49 - 7:52
    We can now compare the monkey
    that had a mother and not.
  • 7:52 - 7:54
    And here's a visual presentation of this.
  • 7:54 - 7:58
    What you see is the genes that
    got more methylated are red.
  • 7:58 - 8:02
    The genes that got less
    methylated are green.
  • 8:02 - 8:04
    You can see many genes are changing,
  • 8:04 - 8:05
    because not having a mother
  • 8:05 - 8:07
    is not just one thing.
  • 8:07 - 8:09
    It affects the whole way, it sends
    a signals about the whole way
  • 8:09 - 8:12
    your world is going to look like
  • 8:12 - 8:13
    when you become an adult,
  • 8:13 - 8:16
    and you can see the two groups of monkey
  • 8:16 - 8:19
    extremely well separated from each other.
  • 8:19 - 8:21
    How early does this develop?
  • 8:21 - 8:24
    These monkeys already didn't
    see their mothers,
  • 8:24 - 8:26
    so they had a social experience.
  • 8:26 - 8:28
    Do we sense our social status
  • 8:28 - 8:31
    even at the moment of birth?
  • 8:31 - 8:34
    So in this experiment,
    we took placentas
  • 8:34 - 8:35
    of monkeys
  • 8:35 - 8:38
    that had different social status.
  • 8:38 - 8:40
    What's interesting about social rank
  • 8:40 - 8:43
    is that across all living beings,
  • 8:43 - 8:46
    they will structure themselves
    by hierarchy.
  • 8:46 - 8:48
    Monkey number one is the boss.
  • 8:48 - 8:51
    Monkey number four is the peon.
  • 8:51 - 8:53
    And you put four monkeys in a cage,
  • 8:53 - 8:57
    there will always be a boss
    and always be a peon.
  • 8:57 - 9:02
    And what's interesting is that
    the monkey number one
  • 9:02 - 9:05
    is much healthier than monkey number four,
  • 9:05 - 9:06
    and if you put them in a cage,
  • 9:06 - 9:11
    monkey number one will not eat as much.
  • 9:11 - 9:14
    Monkey number four will eat as much.
  • 9:14 - 9:18
    And what you see here in this
    methylation mapping,
  • 9:18 - 9:21
    a dramatic separation at birth
  • 9:21 - 9:24
    of the animals that had
    a high social status
  • 9:24 - 9:27
    versus the animals that did not
    have a high status.
  • 9:27 - 9:31
    So we are born already knowing
    the social information,
  • 9:31 - 9:35
    and that social information
    is not bad or good.
  • 9:35 - 9:38
    It just prepares us for life,
    because we have to program
  • 9:38 - 9:41
    our biology differently
  • 9:41 - 9:44
    if we are in the high
    or the low social status.
  • 9:44 - 9:47
    But how can you study this in humans?
  • 9:47 - 9:48
    So we can't do experiments,
  • 9:48 - 9:51
    we can't administer adversity to humans,
  • 9:51 - 9:53
    but God does experiments with humans,
  • 9:53 - 9:55
    and it's called natural disasters.
  • 9:55 - 9:57
    So one of the natural disasters,
  • 9:57 - 10:00
    the hardest natural disaster
    in Canadian history happened
  • 10:00 - 10:02
    in my province of Quebec.
  • 10:02 - 10:04
    It's the ice storm of 1998.
  • 10:04 - 10:08
    We lost our entire electrical grid
    because of an ice storm
  • 10:08 - 10:11
    when the temperatures were in
    the dead of winter in Quebec,
  • 10:11 - 10:13
    minus 20 to minus 30,
  • 10:13 - 10:16
    and there were pregnant
    mothers during that time.
  • 10:16 - 10:18
    And my colleague Suzanne King
  • 10:18 - 10:22
    followed the children of these mothers
  • 10:22 - 10:25
    for 15 years.
  • 10:25 - 10:27
    And what happened was
  • 10:27 - 10:29
    that as the stress increased,
  • 10:29 - 10:32
    and here we had objective
    measures of stress --
  • 10:32 - 10:36
    how long you were without power,
    where did you spend your time,
  • 10:36 - 10:38
    was it in your mother's in law apartment
  • 10:38 - 10:41
    or in some posh country home?
  • 10:41 - 10:44
    so all of these added up
    to a social stress scale,
  • 10:44 - 10:45
    and you can ask the question,
  • 10:45 - 10:48
    how did the children look like?
  • 10:48 - 10:51
    And it appears that as stress increases,
  • 10:51 - 10:53
    the children develop more autism,
  • 10:53 - 10:55
    they develop more metabolic diseases,
  • 10:55 - 10:58
    and they develop more autoimmune diseases.
  • 10:59 - 11:03
    And we would map the methylation state,
    and again you see the green genes
  • 11:03 - 11:07
    becoming red as stress increases,
  • 11:07 - 11:11
    the red genes becoming green
    as stress increases,
  • 11:11 - 11:15
    an entire rearrangement
    of the genome in response to stress.
  • 11:15 - 11:21
    So if we can program genes,
  • 11:21 - 11:25
    if we are not just the slaves
    of the history of our genes,
  • 11:25 - 11:27
    that they could be programmed,
    can we deprogram them?
  • 11:27 - 11:32
    Because epigenetic causes can cause
    diseases like cancer,
  • 11:32 - 11:35
    metabolic disease,
  • 11:35 - 11:38
    and mental health disease.
  • 11:38 - 11:41
    Let's talk about cocaine addiction.
  • 11:41 - 11:44
    Cocaine addiction is a terrible situation
  • 11:44 - 11:46
    that can lead to death
  • 11:46 - 11:50
    and to loss of human life.
  • 11:50 - 11:52
    We asked the question,
  • 11:52 - 11:55
    can we reprogram the addicted brain
  • 11:55 - 11:58
    to make that animal
  • 11:58 - 12:01
    non-addicted anymore?
  • 12:01 - 12:05
    We used a cocaine addiction model
  • 12:05 - 12:07
    that recapitulates what happens in humans.
  • 12:07 - 12:10
    In humans, you're in high school,
  • 12:10 - 12:12
    some friends suggest you some cocaine,
  • 12:12 - 12:14
    you take cocaine, nothing happens,
  • 12:14 - 12:18
    months pass by, something reminds you
    of what happened the first time,
  • 12:18 - 12:19
    a pusher pushes cocaine,
  • 12:19 - 12:21
    and you become addicted,
  • 12:21 - 12:22
    and your life has changed.
  • 12:22 - 12:24
    In rats, we do the same thing.
  • 12:24 - 12:26
    My colleague [??] did.
  • 12:26 - 12:27
    He trains the animals
  • 12:27 - 12:29
    to get used to cocaine,
  • 12:29 - 12:32
    then for one month, no cocaine,
  • 12:32 - 12:35
    and then he reminds them of the party
    when they saw the cocaine the first time
  • 12:35 - 12:38
    by cues, the colors of the cage
    when they saw cocaine.
  • 12:38 - 12:40
    And they go crazy.
  • 12:40 - 12:42
    They will press the lever to get cocaine
  • 12:42 - 12:44
    until they die.
  • 12:44 - 12:46
    We first determined
  • 12:46 - 12:49
    that the difference between these animals
  • 12:49 - 12:51
    is that during that time
    when nothing happens,
  • 12:51 - 12:53
    there's no cocaine around,
  • 12:53 - 12:57
    their epigenome is rearranged.
  • 12:57 - 12:58
    Their genes are remarked
    in a different way,
  • 12:58 - 13:01
    and when the cue comes,
  • 13:01 - 13:03
    their genome is ready
  • 13:03 - 13:05
    to develop this addictive phenotype.
  • 13:05 - 13:07
    So we treated these animals
  • 13:07 - 13:10
    with drugs that either increase
  • 13:10 - 13:14
    DNA methylation, which was
    the epigenetic marker to look at,
  • 13:14 - 13:17
    or decrease epigenetic markings.
  • 13:17 - 13:20
    And we found that if we
    increased methylation,
  • 13:20 - 13:23
    these animals go even crazier.
  • 13:23 - 13:25
    They become more craving for cocaine.
  • 13:25 - 13:28
    But if we reduce the DNA methylation,
  • 13:28 - 13:31
    the animals are not addicted anymore.
  • 13:31 - 13:32
    We have reprogrammed them.
  • 13:32 - 13:35
    And a fundamental difference
    between an epigenetic drug
  • 13:35 - 13:37
    and any other drug
  • 13:37 - 13:39
    is that with epigenetic drugs,
    we essentially remove
  • 13:39 - 13:43
    the signs of experience,
  • 13:43 - 13:45
    and once they're gone,
  • 13:45 - 13:48
    they will not come back
    unless you have the same experience.
  • 13:48 - 13:50
    So the animal now is reprogrammed.
  • 13:50 - 13:54
    So when we visited the animals
    30 days, 60 days longer,
  • 13:54 - 13:57
    which is in human terms
    many years of life,
  • 13:57 - 14:00
    they were still not addicted
  • 14:00 - 14:04
    by a single epigenetic treatment.
  • 14:04 - 14:08
    So what we learned about DNA?
  • 14:08 - 14:11
    The DNA is not just a sequence of letters.
  • 14:11 - 14:13
    It's not just a script.
  • 14:13 - 14:17
    DNA is a dynamic movie.
  • 14:17 - 14:20
    Our experiences are being
    written into this movie,
  • 14:20 - 14:21
    which is interactive.
  • 14:21 - 14:25
    You are like watching a movie
    of your life with the DNA
  • 14:25 - 14:28
    with your remote control.
  • 14:28 - 14:31
    You can remove an actor and add an actor.
  • 14:31 - 14:37
    And so you have, in spite
    of deterministic nature of genetics,
  • 14:37 - 14:41
    you have control of the way
    your genes look like,
  • 14:41 - 14:44
    and this has a tremendous
    optimistic message
  • 14:44 - 14:48
    for ability to now encounter
    some of the deadly diseases
  • 14:48 - 14:50
    like cancer, mental health,
  • 14:50 - 14:53
    with new approach
  • 14:53 - 14:56
    looking at them as maladaptations
  • 14:56 - 14:59
    that if we can epigenetically intervene,
  • 14:59 - 15:00
    reverse the movie
  • 15:00 - 15:02
    by removing an actor
  • 15:02 - 15:06
    and setting up a new narrative.
  • 15:06 - 15:09
    So what I told you today is
  • 15:09 - 15:12
    that our DNA is really combined
  • 15:12 - 15:14
    of two components,
  • 15:14 - 15:16
    two layers of information.
  • 15:16 - 15:23
    One layer of information is old,
    evolved from millions of years
  • 15:23 - 15:26
    of evolution.
  • 15:26 - 15:27
    It is fixed
  • 15:27 - 15:28
    and very hard to change.
  • 15:28 - 15:32
    The other layer of information
    is the epigenetic layer
  • 15:32 - 15:35
    which is open and dynamic
  • 15:35 - 15:38
    and sets up a narrative
  • 15:38 - 15:40
    that is interactive,
  • 15:40 - 15:44
    that allows us to control,
  • 15:44 - 15:47
    to a large extent, our destiny,
  • 15:47 - 15:51
    to help the destiny of our children,
  • 15:51 - 15:55
    and to hopefully conquer disease
  • 15:55 - 16:00
    and serious health challenges
  • 16:00 - 16:03
    that have plagued humankind
    for a long time.
  • 16:03 - 16:07
    So even though we are determined
  • 16:07 - 16:09
    by our genes,
  • 16:09 - 16:12
    we have a degree of freedom
  • 16:12 - 16:16
    that can set up our life
    to a life of responsibility.
  • 16:16 - 16:17
    Thank you.
  • 16:17 - 16:22
    (Applause)
Title:
How early life experience is written into DNA
Speaker:
Moshe Szyf
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
16:35

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions