< Return to Video

Should you trust your first impression? - Peter Mende-Siedlecki

  • 0:07 - 0:09
    Imagine you're at a football game
  • 0:09 - 0:11
    when this obnoxious guy sits next to you.
  • 0:11 - 0:12
    He's loud,
  • 0:12 - 0:13
    he spills his drink on you,
  • 0:13 - 0:15
    and he makes fun of your team.
  • 0:15 - 0:17
    Days later, you're walking in the park
  • 0:17 - 0:20
    when suddenly it starts to pour rain.
  • 0:20 - 0:21
    Who should show up at your side
  • 0:21 - 0:22
    to offer you an umbrella?
  • 0:22 - 0:25
    The same guy from the football game.
  • 0:25 - 0:26
    Do you change your mind about him
  • 0:26 - 0:28
    based on this second encounter,
  • 0:28 - 0:30
    or do you go with your first impression
  • 0:30 - 0:32
    and write him off?
  • 0:32 - 0:34
    Research in social psychology suggests
  • 0:34 - 0:37
    that we're quick to form lasting impressions of others
  • 0:37 - 0:39
    based on their behaviors.
  • 0:39 - 0:41
    We manage to do this with little effort,
  • 0:41 - 0:43
    inferring stable character traits
  • 0:43 - 0:45
    from a single behavior,
  • 0:45 - 0:45
    like a harsh word
  • 0:45 - 0:47
    or a clumsy step.
  • 0:47 - 0:49
    Using our impressions as guides,
  • 0:49 - 0:50
    we can accurately predict
  • 0:50 - 0:53
    how people are going to behave in the future.
  • 0:53 - 0:54
    Armed with the knowledge
  • 0:54 - 0:55
    the guy from the football game
  • 0:55 - 0:57
    was a jerk the first time you met him,
  • 0:57 - 1:00
    you might expect more of the same down the road.
  • 1:00 - 1:02
    If so, you might choose to avoid him
  • 1:02 - 1:03
    the next time you see him.
  • 1:03 - 1:05
    That said, we can change our impressions
  • 1:05 - 1:07
    in light of new information.
  • 1:07 - 1:10
    Behavioral researchers have identified
  • 1:10 - 1:12
    consistent patterns that seem to guide
  • 1:12 - 1:15
    this process of impression updating.
  • 1:15 - 1:17
    On one hand, learning very negative,
  • 1:17 - 1:20
    highly immoral information about someone
  • 1:20 - 1:21
    typically has a stronger impact
  • 1:21 - 1:26
    than learning very positive, highly moral information.
  • 1:26 - 1:28
    So, unfortunately for our new friend
  • 1:28 - 1:29
    from the football game,
  • 1:29 - 1:30
    his bad behavior at the game
  • 1:30 - 1:33
    might outweigh his good behavior at the park.
  • 1:33 - 1:36
    Research suggests that this bias occurs
  • 1:36 - 1:39
    because immoral behaviors are more diagnostic,
  • 1:39 - 1:40
    or revealing,
  • 1:40 - 1:42
    of a person's true character.
  • 1:42 - 1:44
    Okay, so by this logic,
  • 1:44 - 1:46
    bad is always stronger than good
  • 1:46 - 1:48
    when it comes to updating.
  • 1:48 - 1:50
    Well, not necessarily.
  • 1:50 - 1:52
    Certain types of learning don't seem to lead
  • 1:52 - 1:54
    to this sort of negativity bias.
  • 1:54 - 1:57
    When learning about another person's abilities and competencies,
  • 1:57 - 1:58
    for instance,
  • 1:58 - 2:00
    this bias flips.
  • 2:00 - 2:01
    It's actually the positive information
  • 2:01 - 2:04
    that gets weighted more heavily.
  • 2:04 - 2:05
    Let's go back to that football game.
  • 2:05 - 2:07
    If a player scores a goal,
  • 2:07 - 2:08
    it ultimately has a stronger impact
  • 2:08 - 2:10
    on your impression of their skills
  • 2:10 - 2:12
    than if they miss the net.
  • 2:12 - 2:14
    The two sides of the updating story
  • 2:14 - 2:16
    are ultimately quite consistent.
  • 2:16 - 2:18
    Overall, behaviors that are perceived
  • 2:18 - 2:21
    as being less frequent are also the ones
  • 2:21 - 2:23
    that people tend to weigh more heavily
  • 2:23 - 2:26
    when forming and updating impressions,
  • 2:26 - 2:27
    highly immoral actions
  • 2:27 - 2:29
    and highly competent actions.
  • 2:29 - 2:32
    So, what's happening at the level of the brain
  • 2:32 - 2:34
    when we're updating our impressions?
  • 2:34 - 2:35
    Using fMRI,
  • 2:35 - 2:38
    or functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging,
  • 2:38 - 2:39
    researchers have identified
  • 2:39 - 2:41
    an extended network of brain regions
  • 2:41 - 2:43
    that respond to new information
  • 2:43 - 2:46
    that's inconsistent with initial impressions.
  • 2:46 - 2:48
    These include areas typically associated
  • 2:48 - 2:50
    with social cognition,
  • 2:50 - 2:51
    attention,
  • 2:51 - 2:52
    and cognitive control.
  • 2:52 - 2:55
    Moreover, when updating impressions
  • 2:55 - 2:57
    based on people's behaviors,
  • 2:57 - 3:00
    activity in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex
  • 3:00 - 3:02
    and the superior temporal sulcus
  • 3:02 - 3:03
    correlates with perceptions
  • 3:03 - 3:08
    of how frequently those behaviors occur in daily life.
  • 3:08 - 3:10
    In other words, the brain seems to be tracking
  • 3:10 - 3:13
    low-level, statistical properties of behavior
  • 3:13 - 3:16
    in order to make complex decisions
  • 3:16 - 3:17
    regarding other people's character.
  • 3:17 - 3:18
    It needs to decide
  • 3:18 - 3:21
    is this person's behavior typical
  • 3:21 - 3:22
    or is it out of the ordinary?
  • 3:22 - 3:23
    In the situation
  • 3:23 - 3:26
    with the obnoxious-football-fan-turned-good-samaritan,
  • 3:26 - 3:27
    your brain says,
  • 3:27 - 3:29
    "Well, in my experience,
  • 3:29 - 3:32
    pretty much anyone would lend someone their umbrella,
  • 3:32 - 3:34
    but the way this guy acted at the football game,
  • 3:34 - 3:36
    that was unusual."
  • 3:36 - 3:39
    And so, you decide to go with your first impression.
  • 3:39 - 3:41
    There's a good moral in this data:
  • 3:41 - 3:43
    your brain, and by extension you,
  • 3:43 - 3:45
    might care more about
  • 3:45 - 3:47
    the very negative, immoral things
  • 3:47 - 3:48
    another person has done
  • 3:48 - 3:51
    compared to the very positive, moral things,
  • 3:51 - 3:52
    but it's a direct result
  • 3:52 - 3:56
    of the comparative rarity of those bad behaviors.
  • 3:56 - 3:58
    We're more used to people being basically good,
  • 3:58 - 4:01
    like taking time to help a stranger in need.
  • 4:01 - 4:04
    In this context, bad might be stronger than good,
  • 4:04 - 4:07
    but only because good is more plentiful.
  • 4:07 - 4:09
    Think about the last time you judged someone
  • 4:09 - 4:10
    based on their behavior,
  • 4:10 - 4:12
    especially a time when you really feel
  • 4:12 - 4:14
    like you changed your mind about someone.
  • 4:14 - 4:16
    Was the behavior that caused you
  • 4:16 - 4:17
    to update your impression
  • 4:17 - 4:19
    something you'd expect anyone to do,
  • 4:19 - 4:22
    or was it something totally out of the ordinary?
Title:
Should you trust your first impression? - Peter Mende-Siedlecki
Speaker:
Peter Mende-Siedlecki
Description:

View full lesson: http://ed.ted.com/lessons/should-you-trust-your-first-impression-peter-mende-siedlecki

You can't help it; sometimes, you just get a bad feeling about someone that's hard to shake. So, what's happening in your brain when you make that critical (and often lasting) first judgment? Peter Mende-Siedlecki shares the social psychology of first impressions -- and why they may indicate that, deep down, people are basically good.

Lesson by Peter Mende-Siedlecki, animation by TOGETHER.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TED-Ed
Duration:
04:39

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions