< Return to Video

Monkey business 101, how animals cooperate | Dr. Frans de Waal | TEDxEmory

  • 0:03 - 0:04
    Thank you.
  • 0:04 - 0:05
    Is this working? Yeah.
  • 0:05 - 0:08
    And welcome to Emory
    for those who are not from here.
  • 0:08 - 0:11
    I will be talking
    about primates and economics,
  • 0:11 - 0:15
    even though most of the time,
    I don't think people connect those two,
  • 0:15 - 0:17
    but I will be doing that.
  • 0:17 - 0:22
    And I will be challenging
    the idea of Homo economicus,
  • 0:22 - 0:27
    which is defined
    in reaction to Stuart Mill
  • 0:27 - 0:33
    as someone who desires to possess wealth
    and knows how to get wealth,
  • 0:33 - 0:37
    and that's all that you need
    to get an economy going.
  • 0:37 - 0:40
    Basically that's the view
    of Homo economicus.
  • 0:41 - 0:45
    And of course it was popularized nicely
    by Gordon Gekko in the movie "Wall Street"
  • 0:45 - 0:48
    where he says that
    "Greed is all you need."
  • 0:48 - 0:51
    And actually, connecting it
    to the evolutionary spirit ...
  • 0:51 - 0:54
    greed captures the essence
    of the evolutionary spirit.
  • 0:54 - 0:55
    Now, I'm a biologist,
  • 0:55 - 0:58
    and I know something
    about the evolutionary spirit,
  • 0:58 - 0:59
    and I can tell you,
  • 0:59 - 1:02
    there's much more to it than just greed.
  • 1:02 - 1:05
    That as a basis is a very narrow basis.
  • 1:06 - 1:13
    So I will be going sort of from general
    to more specifics in terms of economics.
  • 1:14 - 1:18
    If the victory, if getting the resources
    is all that matters in life,
  • 1:18 - 1:20
    which is basically what the view is,
  • 1:20 - 1:22
    then you could not explain
  • 1:22 - 1:26
    why so many animals have systems
    where they reconcile after fights,
  • 1:26 - 1:27
    they cooperate,
  • 1:28 - 1:32
    they do all sorts of things
    where they are even generous to others.
  • 1:32 - 1:35
    And so a lot of behavior in primates
    cannot be explained that way.
  • 1:36 - 1:39
    So for example,
    this is an actual reconciliation.
  • 1:39 - 1:41
    These are two male chimpanzees
    who have been in a fight.
  • 1:41 - 1:45
    And one of them holds out his hand
    and begs the other for a contact,
  • 1:45 - 1:47
    and about a second
    after I took this picture,
  • 1:47 - 1:50
    the males came together,
    and they kissed and embraced each other.
  • 1:50 - 1:53
    Now that's not something
    that fits that original view
  • 1:53 - 1:56
    of contest-competition,
    basically, that people had.
  • 1:58 - 2:00
    This is a video of it.
  • 2:00 - 2:03
    This is a female who approaches
    the alpha female.
  • 2:03 - 2:04
    She had a big fight with her.
  • 2:04 - 2:06
    This is the alpha female.
  • 2:06 - 2:09
    And she kisses her, sort of,
    and then they groom.
  • 2:10 - 2:13
    You see the reunion
    is actually a bit assertive -
  • 2:13 - 2:14
    like come here -
  • 2:14 - 2:17
    and that's typical
    for many reunions in chimpanzees.
  • 2:17 - 2:19
    There's an assertive element to it.
  • 2:19 - 2:21
    This is how bonobos do it.
  • 2:21 - 2:22
    (Laughter)
  • 2:22 - 2:26
    Bonobos do everything with sex,
    so why not reconciliation?
  • 2:26 - 2:29
    And so all the primates
  • 2:29 - 2:31
    and not just the primates -
  • 2:31 - 2:35
    there's like 25 primate species
    in which we have found reconciliation -
  • 2:35 - 2:40
    but it has been found in goats,
    in hyenas, in dolphins and elephants.
  • 2:40 - 2:41
    And basically,
  • 2:41 - 2:45
    the fact that, after contest, animals come
    together in a sort of friendly fashion
  • 2:45 - 2:47
    doesn't fit the view that everything
  • 2:47 - 2:50
    is about contest
    and about getting the resources.
  • 2:50 - 2:55
    The way this operates is that you can have
    winners and you can have losers,
  • 2:55 - 2:58
    and of course it's better
    to be the winner than the loser.
  • 2:58 - 3:04
    But a zero-sum game is really
    only something that you find
  • 3:04 - 3:07
    in between, let's say,
    territorial fish or territorial birds:
  • 3:07 - 3:10
    "I chase you out of my territory,
    which is a good thing,
  • 3:10 - 3:13
    and then after that,
    I don't have anything to do with you."
  • 3:13 - 3:16
    But as soon as you live in a social group,
    it's very different, actually.
  • 3:16 - 3:20
    And so, many animals
    may form friendships like these elephants.
  • 3:20 - 3:25
    They cooperate, they help each other when
    there's danger, they support each other,
  • 3:25 - 3:28
    they warn each other for things,
    they find food together.
  • 3:28 - 3:32
    So many animals have these relationships
    that are highly cooperative.
  • 3:32 - 3:34
    And they create a very different image.
  • 3:34 - 3:37
    Basically what you get
    is you have winners and you have losers,
  • 3:37 - 3:40
    but there is an area
    where both of them lose.
  • 3:40 - 3:43
    So if I have a buddy
    with whom I do a lot of cooperation,
  • 3:43 - 3:46
    and I kill him, well,
    I've lost the relationship.
  • 3:46 - 3:49
    And if I harm him or injure him
    that's also not good.
  • 3:49 - 3:51
    So there is this overlapping area,
  • 3:51 - 3:55
    which is what we define
    as the value of the relationship.
  • 3:55 - 3:57
    And it's because of the value
    of relationships
  • 3:57 - 4:01
    that you have reconciliations going on
    and conflict resolution going on.
  • 4:01 - 4:05
    And as I said, it's actually found
    all over the animal kingdom;
  • 4:05 - 4:08
    it's not just found in the primates.
  • 4:09 - 4:12
    So we do experiments
    on teamwork in primates,
  • 4:12 - 4:15
    and one of the basic questions
    that we look at is:
  • 4:15 - 4:17
    "Do they understand
    that they need the partner?"
  • 4:17 - 4:21
    Basically, is there an understanding
    of need, which underlies the cooperation.
  • 4:21 - 4:24
    I'm going to show you a very old
    little movie from the Yerkes primates,
  • 4:24 - 4:28
    and it's now almost a century old;
    it was made in the 1920s.
  • 4:28 - 4:30
    And what it shows
    is chimpanzees cooperating.
  • 4:31 - 4:33
    This by itself is not so remarkable.
  • 4:33 - 4:35
    They're highly synchronized, as you see.
  • 4:35 - 4:39
    They're pulling a heavy box
    that is too heavy for one of them.
  • 4:39 - 4:41
    And there's of course food on the box,
  • 4:41 - 4:43
    otherwise they wouldn't
    be pulling like this.
  • 4:43 - 4:44
    (Laughter)
  • 4:44 - 4:46
    So that's where greed comes in, actually.
  • 4:46 - 4:51
    So now they bring it in,
    and it gets more interesting after this.
  • 4:52 - 4:56
    So these are experiments that were done
    by Robert Yerkes, long ago,
  • 4:57 - 5:01
    and at the moment, there's an increased
    interest in these kinds of things.
  • 5:02 - 5:04
    So now it gets more interesting.
  • 5:04 - 5:06
    What they do is they feed
    one of the two chimpanzees.
  • 5:06 - 5:10
    So one of the two is not interested
    in the task anymore.
  • 5:13 - 5:15
    (Laughter)
  • 5:19 - 5:22
    (Laughter)
  • 5:30 - 5:33
    (Laughter)
  • 5:42 - 5:43
    (Laughter)
  • 5:49 - 5:51
    (Laughter)
  • 5:53 - 5:55
    So now look at what happens
    at the very end.
  • 6:03 - 6:05
    This one basically takes everything.
  • 6:05 - 6:08
    So there are two interesting
    elements about it.
  • 6:08 - 6:11
    This chimp on the right has a full
    understanding that he needs a partner -
  • 6:11 - 6:12
    he activates the partner.
  • 6:12 - 6:14
    And the chimp on the left
    is willing to work
  • 6:14 - 6:17
    even though
    he's not interested in the food.
  • 6:17 - 6:20
    So there was much more going on here
    than the food incentive.
  • 6:20 - 6:23
    There's something else going on,
    which is the relationship,
  • 6:23 - 6:26
    which is probably reciprocity
    between these two juvenile chimpanzees.
  • 6:26 - 6:29
    And reciprocity is a very
    big topic in cooperation.
  • 6:29 - 6:32
    So we recently set up
    this experiment with elephants.
  • 6:32 - 6:35
    This was done with Dr. Josh Plotnik,
    who is here shown.
  • 6:35 - 6:39
    Just a year ago,
    he graduated here at school.
  • 6:39 - 6:42
    We set this up with elephants
    and the funny thing is,
  • 6:42 - 6:44
    apart from elephants
    being very dangerous,
  • 6:44 - 6:47
    is that you cannot design an apparatus
  • 6:47 - 6:49
    that is too heavy for one elephant,
    let alone two elephants.
  • 6:49 - 6:53
    And so to set up this experiment
    we had to do something totally different.
  • 6:53 - 6:56
    What we did is we had a rope
    around an apparatus -
  • 6:56 - 6:57
    a single rope -
  • 6:57 - 6:59
    and so if you pull
    on this side of the rope,
  • 6:59 - 7:01
    the rope disappears here,
  • 7:01 - 7:04
    meaning that two elephants
    need to grab it at the same time,
  • 7:04 - 7:07
    otherwise the rope disappears,
    and you cannot do anything.
  • 7:07 - 7:11
    So it's a very smart apparatus,
    and I'm going to show you the first clip,
  • 7:11 - 7:14
    which is elephants doing this.
  • 7:14 - 7:18
    It's very hard to do these experiments,
    so Josh worked in Thailand on this.
  • 7:18 - 7:21
    Here you see a rope -
  • 7:21 - 7:23
    a rope here and a rope here -
  • 7:23 - 7:27
    and you should see two elephants
    who have been released at the same time.
  • 7:27 - 7:30
    They're going to walk up to these ropes
    and pick them up at the same time,
  • 7:30 - 7:33
    which by itself is not really
    so challenging for elephants.
  • 7:33 - 7:35
    So they pick up the rope.
  • 7:36 - 7:37
    Since they arrive at the same time,
  • 7:37 - 7:42
    the question of who is going to do what
    is not particularly relevant to them.
  • 7:42 - 7:44
    And so now they're eating the food.
  • 7:44 - 7:48
    So we're going to make it more difficult
    to see what they understand of this.
  • 7:48 - 7:50
    And the way to make it more difficult
  • 7:50 - 7:52
    is to release them
    with a delay between them.
  • 7:52 - 7:56
    Elephants are very patient animals,
    so we went up to delays of 45 seconds.
  • 7:56 - 7:58
    Here you have an elephant who arrives.
  • 7:58 - 8:01
    And this elephant needs to be
    smart enough to wait for the other,
  • 8:01 - 8:04
    because if he grabs the rope
    and pulls it, it disappears.
  • 8:05 - 8:07
    This elephant had his own technique,
  • 8:07 - 8:09
    which was an illegal technique
    that we didn't approve of.
  • 8:09 - 8:10
    (Laughter)
  • 8:10 - 8:13
    What he did is he would
    put his big foot on the rope
  • 8:13 - 8:15
    and then stand there and not do anything.
  • 8:15 - 8:19
    And so you will see - the other elephant
    is going to do all the work.
  • 8:19 - 8:23
    And in terms of cooperation,
    this is called "freeloading."
  • 8:23 - 8:24
    (Laughter)
  • 8:24 - 8:26
    So ... we had a freeloader.
  • 8:26 - 8:28
    And of course you can have only one.
  • 8:28 - 8:31
    If both elephants would do the same thing,
    nothing would happen.
  • 8:31 - 8:34
    So this one is just going to wait
    until the other one arrives.
  • 8:37 - 8:40
    And the other one
    is now going to pull it in.
  • 8:45 - 8:47
    (Laughter)
  • 8:52 - 8:55
    You will notice at the end
    that he doesn't forget to eat.
  • 8:56 - 8:58
    He's not asleep.
  • 8:58 - 8:59
    (Laughter)
  • 9:01 - 9:05
    So that's the sort of studies we do
    on cooperation in primates and elephants.
  • 9:06 - 9:08
    And we're very interested
    in the issue of reciprocity
  • 9:08 - 9:13
    because we think reciprocity underlies
    a lot of the evolution of cooperation,
  • 9:13 - 9:16
    and we study that in chimpanzees
    and in other primates.
  • 9:17 - 9:20
    So this is actually my office
    at the Yerkes Field Station,
  • 9:20 - 9:22
    which is in Lawrenceville.
  • 9:22 - 9:24
    These are chimpanzees who live there.
  • 9:24 - 9:27
    And that window up there
    is my view of the chimpanzees.
  • 9:27 - 9:32
    And so in these chimpanzees we do,
    for example, food sharing studies.
  • 9:32 - 9:37
    Here you see a food sharing scene
    where some chimpanzees have a watermelon.
  • 9:37 - 9:38
    And the male on the left,
  • 9:38 - 9:41
    who's reaching into the watermelon
    and grabbing pieces out of it,
  • 9:42 - 9:44
    is the alpha male of the group.
  • 9:44 - 9:47
    Now this is very interesting to me
    because the alpha male -
  • 9:47 - 9:48
    you would think -
  • 9:48 - 9:51
    could also grab the watermelon
    and dominate the whole situation.
  • 9:51 - 9:52
    But he's not doing that.
  • 9:52 - 9:54
    He's begging for his food.
  • 9:54 - 9:56
    And this has been known for a long time -
  • 9:56 - 9:57
    also for wild chimpanzees -
  • 9:57 - 9:59
    is that the highest ranking male
  • 9:59 - 10:02
    doesn't necessarily appropriate
    all the goodies in a group.
  • 10:02 - 10:03
    Why would that be?
  • 10:03 - 10:07
    I think it has to do - I wrote a book,
    "Chimpanzee Politics" -
  • 10:07 - 10:10
    Alpha males need to have
    a support base in the group;
  • 10:10 - 10:12
    otherwise, their position is very shaky.
  • 10:12 - 10:15
    So they need to be popular in the group.
  • 10:15 - 10:18
    And to be popular is to share food.
  • 10:18 - 10:22
    For example, if alpha males were to take
    all the food away from everybody,
  • 10:22 - 10:24
    they would probably be highly unpopular.
  • 10:24 - 10:27
    And then everyone would be waiting
    for the right occasion to topple them,
  • 10:27 - 10:31
    and as soon as there's a challenge,
    they will lose their position.
  • 10:31 - 10:32
    So that's why these things happen.
  • 10:32 - 10:34
    And it's all related to reciprocity:
  • 10:34 - 10:38
    deal making between males,
    deal making between males and females,
  • 10:38 - 10:39
    and that's why these things happen.
  • 10:39 - 10:42
    So if might is right,
    why would an alpha male beg?
  • 10:42 - 10:44
    And that has to do with reciprocity.
  • 10:44 - 10:47
    Let me show you one piece of evidence
    for reciprocity in chimpanzees.
  • 10:47 - 10:51
    What we do in one study
    is we measure in the morning
  • 10:51 - 10:55
    who grooms whom for how long
    in a group of chimpanzees.
  • 10:55 - 10:58
    It's very simple because
    they groom a lot, chimpanzees.
  • 10:58 - 11:01
    Then we wait a couple of hours
    and then we introduce food.
  • 11:01 - 11:03
    And if you have enough
    interactions over food -
  • 11:03 - 11:05
    so we have 7,000
    interactions to work with -
  • 11:05 - 11:08
    you can of course statistically
    compare the one with the other.
  • 11:08 - 11:09
    You can say,
  • 11:09 - 11:11
    does grooming in the morning
  • 11:11 - 11:13
    have any effect on food sharing
    in the afternoon?
  • 11:14 - 11:15
    And that's what we find.
  • 11:15 - 11:18
    What we find is that if A grooms B,
  • 11:18 - 11:21
    then a couple of hours later,
    B is more likely to groom A.
  • 11:21 - 11:24
    It's not that B is more likely
    to groom anybody else.
  • 11:24 - 11:28
    It's not like B is in a good mood
    and is going to groom everybody.
  • 11:28 - 11:29
    No.
  • 11:29 - 11:31
    B is specifically sharing food
  • 11:31 - 11:33
    with the one who did
    the grooming in the morning.
  • 11:33 - 11:35
    So if you think about it,
  • 11:35 - 11:38
    what you need for this kind of mechanism
    is, first of all, memory.
  • 11:38 - 11:41
    You need to remember the grooming bout
    from a couple of hours ago,
  • 11:41 - 11:43
    which for chimpanzees is no big deal.
  • 11:43 - 11:45
    They remember a human face,
  • 11:45 - 11:46
    and I've seen this myself,
  • 11:46 - 11:48
    for 25 years, and so
    this is really simple.
  • 11:49 - 11:51
    But the second thing that you need
  • 11:51 - 11:53
    is that that memory
    triggers a positive attitude
  • 11:53 - 11:55
    toward the one who did the grooming.
  • 11:55 - 11:57
    And in humans we have a word for that.
  • 11:57 - 11:58
    We call that gratitude.
  • 11:58 - 12:02
    And actually, Bob Trivers, who designed
    the theory of reciprocal altruism,
  • 12:02 - 12:03
    already said, 30 years ago,
  • 12:03 - 12:06
    that gratitude needs to be part
    of the whole mechanism.
  • 12:09 - 12:11
    So finally, the last study
    I want to show you
  • 12:11 - 12:16
    I did with Sarah Brosnan,
    which is on inequality.
  • 12:16 - 12:20
    And now we're getting even closer to,
    let's say, behavioral economics.
  • 12:20 - 12:23
    Because this is a very big topic
    in behavioral economics.
  • 12:23 - 12:25
    And we do that on capuchin monkeys.
  • 12:26 - 12:27
    So we noticed with capuchin monkeys -
  • 12:27 - 12:30
    this is again the same apparatus
    that you saw before,
  • 12:30 - 12:32
    with a weight and two pulling bars,
  • 12:32 - 12:33
    and so on -
  • 12:33 - 12:34
    we noticed in these studies
  • 12:34 - 12:38
    that the capuchins are very sensitive
    to the reward distribution.
  • 12:38 - 12:41
    "Do I get more, or do I get less
    than somebody else?"
  • 12:41 - 12:43
    Now that's already very intriguing
  • 12:43 - 12:46
    because normally you would think
    anything would be fine.
  • 12:46 - 12:48
    Why would you watch
    what somebody else is getting?
  • 12:48 - 12:50
    What is the point of doing that?
  • 12:51 - 12:55
    So if any reward is better
    than no reward at all,
  • 12:55 - 12:57
    then why would you reject unfair offers?
  • 12:57 - 13:00
    And that's actually
    what we're finding in the monkeys
  • 13:00 - 13:02
    and certainly finding in the chimpanzees.
  • 13:03 - 13:04
    So the way this study was done
  • 13:04 - 13:09
    is that Sarah would give a token,
    basically a pebble, to a capuchin monkey,
  • 13:09 - 13:10
    who would need to hand it back.
  • 13:10 - 13:12
    She would hold out her hand
    and get it back.
  • 13:12 - 13:16
    And then immediately after, she would give
    the monkey a little piece of cucumber.
  • 13:16 - 13:19
    And the monkeys
    are perfectly fine to do that.
  • 13:19 - 13:21
    Any time you give them
    a piece of cucumber they will eat it,
  • 13:21 - 13:24
    so they're perfectly fine
    with this kind of thing.
  • 13:24 - 13:26
    But then we made a variation.
  • 13:26 - 13:29
    One monkey would still get cucumber
    for this exchange,
  • 13:29 - 13:31
    but the other one would now get grapes.
  • 13:31 - 13:33
    And so what is the better food?
  • 13:34 - 13:37
    Grapes, because they're all primates,
    they all go for sugar content,
  • 13:37 - 13:39
    so that's an old characteristic.
  • 13:39 - 13:43
    Actually, the food preferences
    of our monkeys correspond perfectly
  • 13:43 - 13:45
    with the prices in the supermarket.
  • 13:45 - 13:47
    (Laughter)
  • 13:49 - 13:51
    So then, here you see the exchange,
  • 13:51 - 13:53
    and I'm going to show you
    a little video clip of it.
  • 13:53 - 13:56
    This is a monkey watching
    another monkey returning the token.
  • 13:56 - 13:59
    This is what happens
    if both of them get cucumber.
  • 13:59 - 14:02
    There's only 5% rejections, meaning 95 -
  • 14:02 - 14:04
    we do like 25 trials in a row -
  • 14:04 - 14:07
    95% of the time they do it.
  • 14:07 - 14:10
    So if both get cucumber,
    they're fine with it.
  • 14:10 - 14:14
    If you give grape to the other guy,
    then 50% rejection,
  • 14:14 - 14:17
    meaning they become agitated,
    and they reject the food,
  • 14:17 - 14:19
    and they reject the pebbles.
  • 14:19 - 14:22
    In the third condition,
  • 14:22 - 14:25
    if you give the grape
    to the other guy without any effort,
  • 14:25 - 14:27
    then they go to 80% rejection.
  • 14:27 - 14:29
    So they're also sensitive to effort,
    apparently.
  • 14:29 - 14:31
    We've been testing this out lately.
  • 14:31 - 14:33
    So let me show you a little video,
  • 14:33 - 14:36
    and before I show it,
    let me say something about it.
  • 14:36 - 14:39
    What you see here is a monkey
    on the left who gets cucumber.
  • 14:39 - 14:42
    The monkey on the right gets grapes.
  • 14:42 - 14:45
    Now the cucumber and the grapes
    are going to be visible to them both.
  • 14:45 - 14:47
    You could argue - and it's been argued -
  • 14:47 - 14:49
    maybe they're holding out
    for the better food they see.
  • 14:49 - 14:53
    We've actually done a lot of control
    tests to show it's not the case.
  • 14:53 - 14:58
    They're really more interested in what
    the other monkey gets than what they see.
  • 14:58 - 15:01
    In this particular clip you will see
    that the first piece of cucumber
  • 15:01 - 15:04
    is happily eaten
    by the monkey on the left.
  • 15:04 - 15:07
    It's only after this guy has gotten grapes
  • 15:07 - 15:10
    that this one starts to object
    to the procedure.
  • 15:12 - 15:15
    So he gives a rock;
    he gets a piece of cucumber.
  • 15:15 - 15:17
    And notice that he eats it.
  • 15:17 - 15:18
    There it goes.
  • 15:18 - 15:24
    This one gives a rock, gets a grape.
  • 15:24 - 15:27
    The one on the left sees this, you see.
  • 15:27 - 15:28
    (Laughter)
  • 15:28 - 15:32
    Gives a rock, gets a piece of cucumber.
  • 15:35 - 15:38
    (Laughter)
  • 15:42 - 15:45
    Gives a rock...
  • 16:00 - 16:03
    (Laughter)
  • 16:07 - 16:09
    Well, this response -
  • 16:09 - 16:11
    in behavioral economics,
    or in economics in general,
  • 16:11 - 16:14
    you would call it an irrational response
  • 16:14 - 16:17
    because a piece of cucumber
    is better than no piece of cucumber.
  • 16:17 - 16:20
    So you should always
    accept what you can get.
  • 16:20 - 16:22
    This relates a bit to the ultimatum game.
  • 16:22 - 16:26
    Rajesh will be talking about the ultimatum
    game that is played with humans later.
  • 16:26 - 16:28
    Now recently, Sarah Brosnan
    found with chimpanzees,
  • 16:28 - 16:31
    where we're doing
    the same sorts of experiments,
  • 16:31 - 16:32
    even something more dramatic,
  • 16:32 - 16:37
    which I usually explain this by saying
    this is an egocentric sense of fairness,
  • 16:37 - 16:40
    only a sense of fairness that relates
    to "Do I get less than somebody else?"
  • 16:40 - 16:43
    But now recently she found
    in certain pairs of chimpanzees -
  • 16:43 - 16:46
    if you play this game -
  • 16:46 - 16:49
    the one who gets the grape
    refuses to accept the grape
  • 16:49 - 16:51
    until the other guy also gets a grape.
  • 16:51 - 16:54
    Now that gets much closer to the human
    full-blown sense of fairness.
  • 16:54 - 16:57
    And so these are so-called
    "irrational responses"
  • 16:57 - 17:01
    that we think relate to high levels
    of cooperation in certain animals,
  • 17:01 - 17:02
    like in dogs,
  • 17:02 - 17:04
    it has been demonstrated recently also,
  • 17:04 - 17:06
    which is a very cooperative
    group of animals.
  • 17:06 - 17:08
    And so in cooperative animals,
  • 17:08 - 17:11
    you need to watch what you get
    if you invest in cooperation.
  • 17:11 - 17:13
    You need to make sure
    that you don't get less -
  • 17:13 - 17:15
    you don't get screwed, so to speak.
  • 17:15 - 17:17
    You don't get less than somebody else.
  • 17:18 - 17:22
    So my conclusion is that primates,
    including Homo sapiens,
  • 17:22 - 17:25
    are far more social
    and far more cooperative
  • 17:25 - 17:29
    than they have been given credit for
    in the Homo economicus view.
  • 17:29 - 17:32
    And I thank you for your attention.
  • 17:32 - 17:35
    (Applause)
Title:
Monkey business 101, how animals cooperate | Dr. Frans de Waal | TEDxEmory
Description:

Dr. Frans de Waal tells about the concept of reciprocity when it comes to working cooperatively among primate species. Will a chimpanzee do extra work for a better reward? Will a chimpanzee continue to work for a lesser reward when his colleague receives a better reward for the same work? Would you?

Dr. Frans de Waal is a primatologist and ethologist. He is the author of numerous books including "Chimpanzee Politics" (1982) and "Our Inner Ape" (2005), and he is director of the of the Living Links Center at the Yerkes National Primate Research Center at Emory University in Atlanta, Georgia.

This talk was given at a TEDx event using the TED conference format but independently organized by a local community. Learn more at http://ted.com/tedx

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDxTalks
Duration:
17:39

English subtitles

Revisions