< Return to Video

This one weird trick will help you spot clickbait - Jeff Leek and Lucy McGowan

  • 0:07 - 0:12
    One simple vitamin can reduce
    your risk of heart disease.
  • 0:12 - 0:16
    Eating chocolate reduces
    stress in students.
  • 0:16 - 0:20
    New drug prolongs lives of
    patients with rare disease.
  • 0:20 - 0:23
    Health headlines like these
    are published every day,
  • 0:23 - 0:26
    sometimes making opposite claims
    from each other.
  • 0:26 - 0:28
    There can be a disconnect between broad,
  • 0:28 - 0:32
    attention-grabbing headlines
    and the often specific,
  • 0:32 - 0:35
    incremental results of the medical
    research they cover.
  • 0:35 - 0:39
    So how can you avoid being
    misled by grabby headlines?
  • 0:39 - 0:42
    The best way to assess a headline’s
    credibility
  • 0:42 - 0:46
    is to look at the original
    research it reports on.
  • 0:46 - 0:48
    We’ve come up with a hypothetical research
    scenario
  • 0:48 - 0:51
    for each of these three headlines.
  • 0:51 - 0:53
    Keep watching for the explanation
    of the first example;
  • 0:53 - 0:56
    then pause at the headline to
    answer the question.
  • 0:56 - 0:59
    These are simplified scenarios.
  • 0:59 - 1:03
    A real study would detail many more
    factors and how it accounted for them,
  • 1:03 - 1:05
    but for the purposes of this exercise,
  • 1:05 - 1:09
    assume all the information
    you need is included.
  • 1:11 - 1:14
    Let’s start by considering the
    cardiovascular effects
  • 1:14 - 1:17
    of a certain vitamin, Healthium.
  • 1:17 - 1:20
    The study finds that participants taking
    Healthium
  • 1:20 - 1:24
    had a higher level of healthy cholesterol
    than those taking a placebo.
  • 1:24 - 1:28
    Their levels became similar to those of
    people with naturally high levels
  • 1:28 - 1:30
    of this kind of cholesterol.
  • 1:30 - 1:33
    Previous research has shown that people
    with naturally high levels
  • 1:33 - 1:37
    of healthy cholesterol have lower
    rates of heart disease.
  • 1:37 - 1:40
    So what makes this headline misleading:
  • 1:40 - 1:44
    "Healthium reduces risk of heart disease."
  • 1:44 - 1:49
    The problem with this headline is that the
    research didn’t actually investigate
  • 1:49 - 1:52
    whether Healthium reduces heart disease.
  • 1:52 - 1:54
    It only measured Healthium’s impact
  • 1:54 - 1:57
    on levels of a particular
    kind of cholesterol.
  • 1:57 - 2:00
    The fact that people with naturally high
    levels of that cholesterol
  • 2:00 - 2:02
    have lower risk of heart attacks
  • 2:02 - 2:04
    doesn’t mean that the same
    will be true of people
  • 2:04 - 2:08
    who elevate their cholesterol
    levels using Healthium.
  • 2:08 - 2:10
    Now that you’ve cracked the
    case of Healthium,
  • 2:10 - 2:14
    try your hand at a particularly alluring
    mystery:
  • 2:14 - 2:17
    the relationship between eating chocolate
    and stress.
  • 2:17 - 2:20
    This hypothetical study
    recruits ten students.
  • 2:20 - 2:24
    Half begin consuming a
    daily dose of chocolate,
  • 2:24 - 2:25
    while half abstain.
  • 2:25 - 2:29
    As classmates, they all follow
    the same schedule.
  • 2:29 - 2:32
    By the end of the study, the chocolate
    eaters are less stressed
  • 2:32 - 2:35
    than their chocolate-free counterparts.
  • 2:35 - 2:37
    What’s wrong with this headline:
  • 2:37 - 2:41
    "Eating chocolate reduces
    stress in students"
  • 2:43 - 2:49
    It’s a stretch to draw a conclusion about
    students in general from a sample of ten.
  • 2:49 - 2:52
    That’s because the fewer participants are
    in a random sample,
  • 2:52 - 2:55
    the less likely it is that the sample will
    closely represent
  • 2:55 - 2:58
    the target population as a whole.
  • 2:58 - 3:03
    For example, if the broader population of
    students is half male and half female,
  • 3:03 - 3:05
    the chance of drawing a sample of 10
  • 3:05 - 3:10
    that’s skewed 70% male and
    30% is about 12%.
  • 3:10 - 3:16
    In a sample of 100 that would be less than
    a .0025% chance,
  • 3:16 - 3:18
    and for a sample of 1000,
  • 3:18 - 3:23
    the odds are less than 6 x 10^-36.
  • 3:23 - 3:25
    Similarly, with fewer participants,
  • 3:25 - 3:29
    each individual’s outcome has a larger
    impact on the overall results—
  • 3:29 - 3:33
    and can therefore skew big-picture trends.
  • 3:33 - 3:37
    Still, there are a lot of good reasons for
    scientists to run small studies.
  • 3:37 - 3:39
    By starting with a small sample,
  • 3:39 - 3:42
    they can evaluate whether the results are
    promising enough
  • 3:42 - 3:45
    to run a more comprehensive,
    expensive study.
  • 3:45 - 3:49
    And some research requires very specific
    participants
  • 3:49 - 3:52
    that may be impossible to
    recruit in large numbers.
  • 3:52 - 3:54
    The key is reproducibility—
  • 3:54 - 3:58
    if an article draws a conclusion
    from one small study,
  • 3:58 - 4:00
    that conclusion may be suspect—
  • 4:00 - 4:03
    but if it’s based on many studies
    that have found similar results,
  • 4:03 - 4:05
    it’s more credible.
  • 4:05 - 4:07
    We’ve still got one more puzzle.
  • 4:07 - 4:12
    In this scenario, a study tests a new drug
    for a rare, fatal disease.
  • 4:12 - 4:14
    In a sample of 2,000 patients,
  • 4:14 - 4:18
    the ones who start taking the drug upon
    diagnosis
  • 4:18 - 4:21
    live longer than those who
    take the placebo.
  • 4:21 - 4:24
    This time, the question
    is slightly different.
  • 4:24 - 4:28
    What’s one more thing you’d like to know
    before deciding if the headline,
  • 4:28 - 4:34
    "New drug prolongs lives of patients
    with rare disease", is justified?
  • 4:35 - 4:36
    Before making this call,
  • 4:36 - 4:41
    you’d want to know how much the drug
    prolonged the patients’ lives.
  • 4:41 - 4:43
    Sometimes, a study can have results that,
  • 4:43 - 4:48
    while scientifically valid, don’t have
    much bearing on real world outcomes.
  • 4:48 - 4:53
    For example, one real-life clinical trial
    of a pancreatic cancer drug
  • 4:53 - 4:57
    found an increase in life expectancy—
    of ten days.
  • 4:57 - 5:00
    The next time you see a surprising medical
    headline,
  • 5:00 - 5:04
    take a look at the science
    it’s reporting on.
  • 5:04 - 5:07
    Even when full papers aren’t
    available without a fee,
  • 5:07 - 5:10
    you can often find summaries of
    experimental design
  • 5:10 - 5:13
    and results in freely available abstracts,
  • 5:13 - 5:16
    or even within the text
    of a news article.
  • 5:16 - 5:20
    It’s exciting to see scientific research
    covered in the news,
  • 5:20 - 5:23
    and important to understand
    the studies’ findings.
Title:
This one weird trick will help you spot clickbait - Jeff Leek and Lucy McGowan
Speaker:
Jeff Leek and Lucy McGowan
Description:

View full lesson: https://ed.ted.com/lessons/this-one-weird-trick-will-help-you-spot-clickbait-jeff-leek-and-lucy-mcgowan

Health headlines are published every day, sometimes making opposite claims from each other. There can be a disconnect between broad, attention-grabbing headlines and the often specific, incremental results of the medical research they cover. So how can you avoid being misled by grabby headlines? Jeff Leek and Lucy McGowan explain how to read past the clickbait.

Lesson by Jeff Leek and Lucy McGowan, directed by Zedem Media.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TED-Ed
Duration:
04:49

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions