< Return to Video

This one weird trick will help you spot clickbait - Jeff Leek and Lucy McGowan

  • 0:07 - 0:12
    One simple vitamin can reduce
    your risk of heart disease.
  • 0:12 - 0:16
    Eating chocolate reduces
    stress in students.
  • 0:16 - 0:20
    New drug prolongs lives of
    patients with rare disease.
  • 0:20 - 0:23
    Health headlines like these are published
    every day,
  • 0:23 - 0:26
    sometimes making opposite claims
    from each other.
  • 0:26 - 0:28
    There can be a disconnect between broad,
  • 0:28 - 0:32
    attention-grabbing headlines
    and the often specific,
  • 0:32 - 0:35
    incremental results of the medical
    research they cover.
  • 0:35 - 0:39
    So how can you avoid being
    misled by grabby headlines?
  • 0:39 - 0:42
    The best way to assess a headline’s
    credibility
  • 0:42 - 0:46
    is to look at the original
    research it reports on.
  • 0:46 - 0:48
    We’ve come up with a hypothetical research
    scenario
  • 0:48 - 0:51
    for each of these three headlines.
  • 0:51 - 0:53
    Keep watching for the explanation
    of the first example;
  • 0:53 - 0:56
    then pause at the headline to
    answer the question.
  • 0:56 - 0:59
    These are simplified scenarios.
  • 0:59 - 1:03
    A real study would detail many more
    factors and how it accounted for them,
  • 1:03 - 1:05
    but for the purposes of this exercise,
  • 1:05 - 1:09
    assume all the information
    you need is included.
  • 1:11 - 1:14
    Let’s start by considering the
    cardiovascular effects
  • 1:14 - 1:17
    of a certain vitamin, Healthium.
  • 1:17 - 1:20
    The study finds that participants taking
    Healthium
  • 1:20 - 1:24
    had a higher level of healthy cholesterol
    than those taking a placebo.
  • 1:24 - 1:28
    Their levels became similar to those of
    people with naturally high levels
  • 1:28 - 1:30
    of this kind of cholesterol.
  • 1:30 - 1:33
    Previous research has shown that people
    with naturally high levels
  • 1:33 - 1:37
    of healthy cholesterol have lower
    rates of heart disease.
  • 1:37 - 1:40
    So what makes this headline misleading:
  • 1:40 - 1:44
    Healthium reduces risk of heart disease.
  • 1:44 - 1:49
    The problem with this headline is that the
    research didn’t actually investigate
  • 1:49 - 1:52
    whether Healthium reduces heart disease.
  • 1:52 - 1:54
    It only measured Healthium’s impact
  • 1:54 - 1:57
    on levels of a particular
    kind of cholesterol.
  • 1:57 - 2:00
    The fact that people with naturally high
    levels of that cholesterol
  • 2:00 - 2:02
    have lower risk of heart attacks
  • 2:02 - 2:04
    doesn’t mean that the same
    will be true of people
  • 2:04 - 2:08
    who elevate their cholesterol
    levels using Healthium.
  • 2:08 - 2:10
    Now that you’ve cracked the
    case of Healthium,
  • 2:10 - 2:14
    try your hand at a particularly alluring
    mystery:
  • 2:14 - 2:17
    the relationship between eating chocolate
    and stress.
  • 2:17 - 2:20
    This hypothetical study
    recruits ten students.
  • 2:20 - 2:24
    Half begin consuming a
    daily dose of chocolate,
  • 2:24 - 2:25
    while half abstain.
  • 2:25 - 2:29
    As classmates, they all follow
    the same schedule.
  • 2:29 - 2:32
    By the end of the study, the chocolate
    eaters are less stressed
  • 2:32 - 2:35
    than their chocolate-free counterparts.
  • 2:35 - 2:37
    What’s wrong with this headline:
  • 2:37 - 2:41
    Eating chocolate reduces
    stress in students
  • 2:43 - 2:49
    It’s a stretch to draw a conclusion about
    students in general from a sample of ten.
  • 2:49 - 2:52
    That’s because the fewer participants are
    in a random sample,
  • 2:52 - 2:55
    the less likely it is that the sample will
    closely represent
  • 2:55 - 2:58
    the target population as a whole.
  • 2:58 - 3:03
    For example, if the broader population of
    students is half male and half female,
  • 3:03 - 3:05
    the chance of drawing a sample of 10
  • 3:05 - 3:10
    that’s skewed 70% male and
    30% is about 12%.
  • 3:10 - 3:16
    In a sample of 100 that would be less than
    a .0025% chance,
  • 3:16 - 3:18
    and for a sample of 1000,
  • 3:18 - 3:23
    the odds are less than 6 x 10^-36.
  • 3:23 - 3:25
    Similarly, with fewer participants,
  • 3:25 - 3:29
    each individual’s outcome has a larger
    impact on the overall results—
  • 3:29 - 3:33
    and can therefore skew big-picture trends.
  • 3:33 - 3:37
    Still, there are a lot of good reasons for
    scientists to run small studies.
  • 3:37 - 3:39
    By starting with a small sample,
  • 3:39 - 3:42
    they can evaluate whether the results are
    promising enough
  • 3:42 - 3:45
    to run a more comprehensive,
    expensive study.
  • 3:45 - 3:49
    And some research requires very specific
    participants
  • 3:49 - 3:52
    that may be impossible to
    recruit in large numbers.
  • 3:52 - 3:54
    The key is reproducibility—
  • 3:54 - 3:58
    if an article draws a conclusion
    from one small study,
  • 3:58 - 4:00
    that conclusion may be suspect—
  • 4:00 - 4:03
    but if it’s based on many studies
    that have found similar results,
  • 4:03 - 4:05
    it’s more credible.
  • 4:05 - 4:07
    We’ve still got one more puzzle.
  • 4:07 - 4:12
    In this scenario, a study tests a new drug
    for a rare, fatal disease.
  • 4:12 - 4:14
    In a sample of 2,000 patients,
  • 4:14 - 4:18
    the ones who start taking the drug upon
    diagnosis
  • 4:18 - 4:21
    live longer than those who
    take the placebo.
  • 4:21 - 4:24
    This time, the question
    is slightly different.
  • 4:24 - 4:28
    What’s one more thing you’d like to know
    before deciding if the headline,
  • 4:28 - 4:34
    New drug prolongs lives of patients
    with rare disease, is justified?
  • 4:35 - 4:36
    Before making this call,
  • 4:36 - 4:41
    you’d want to know how much the drug
    prolonged the patients’ lives.
  • 4:41 - 4:43
    Sometimes, a study can have results that,
  • 4:43 - 4:48
    while scientifically valid, don’t have
    much bearing on real world outcomes.
  • 4:48 - 4:53
    For example, one real-life clinical trial
    of a pancreatic cancer drug
  • 4:53 - 4:57
    found an increase in life expectancy—
    of ten days.
  • 4:57 - 5:00
    The next time you see a surprising medical
    headline,
  • 5:00 - 5:04
    take a look at the science
    it’s reporting on.
  • 5:04 - 5:07
    Even when full papers aren’t
    available without a fee,
  • 5:07 - 5:10
    you can often find summaries of
    experimental design
  • 5:10 - 5:13
    and results in freely available abstracts,
  • 5:13 - 5:16
    or even within the text
    of a news article.
  • 5:16 - 5:20
    It’s exciting to see scientific research
    covered in the news,
  • 5:20 - 5:24
    and important to understand
    the studies’ findings.
Title:
This one weird trick will help you spot clickbait - Jeff Leek and Lucy McGowan
Speaker:
Jeff Leek and Lucy McGowan
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TED-Ed
Duration:
04:49

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions