< Return to Video

The linguistic genius of babies | Patricia Kuhl | TEDxRainier

  • 0:17 - 0:19
    I want you to take a look at this baby.
  • 0:20 - 0:24
    What you're drawn to are her eyes
    and the skin you love to touch.
  • 0:25 - 0:28
    But today I'm going to talk to you
    about something you can't see.
  • 0:28 - 0:31
    What's going on
    up in that little brain of hers.
  • 0:32 - 0:36
    The modern tools of neuroscience
    are demonstrating to us
  • 0:36 - 0:39
    that what's going on up there
    is nothing short of rocket science.
  • 0:40 - 0:44
    And what we're learning
    is going to shed some light
  • 0:44 - 0:50
    on what the romantic writers and poets
    described as the "celestial openness"
  • 0:50 - 0:51
    of the child's mind.
  • 0:53 - 0:56
    What we see here is a mother in India,
  • 0:56 - 1:00
    and she's speaking Koro,
    which is a newly discovered language.
  • 1:00 - 1:02
    And she's talking to her baby.
  • 1:03 - 1:04
    What this mother --
  • 1:04 - 1:07
    and the 800 people who speak
    Koro in the world --
  • 1:07 - 1:10
    understands is that,
    to preserve this language,
  • 1:10 - 1:13
    they need to speak it to the babies.
  • 1:13 - 1:16
    And therein lies a critical puzzle.
  • 1:16 - 1:18
    Why is it that you can't
    preserve a language
  • 1:18 - 1:21
    by speaking to you and I, to the adults?
  • 1:21 - 1:23
    Well, it's got to do with your brain.
  • 1:24 - 1:28
    What we see here is that language
    has a critical period for learning.
  • 1:29 - 1:33
    The way to read this slide is to look
    at your age on the horizontal axis.
  • 1:33 - 1:35
    (Laughter)
  • 1:35 - 1:39
    And you'll see on the vertical
    your skill at acquiring a second language.
  • 1:40 - 1:44
    The babies and children are geniuses
    until they turn seven,
  • 1:44 - 1:46
    and then there's a systematic decline.
  • 1:46 - 1:48
    After puberty, we fall off the map.
  • 1:49 - 1:51
    No scientists dispute this curve,
  • 1:51 - 1:53
    but laboratories all over the world
  • 1:53 - 1:56
    are trying to figure out
    why it works this way.
  • 1:56 - 2:00
    Work in my lab is focused on the first
    critical period in development,
  • 2:00 - 2:02
    and that is the period in which babies
  • 2:02 - 2:06
    try to master which sounds
    are used in their language.
  • 2:06 - 2:08
    We think, by studying
    how the sounds are learned,
  • 2:08 - 2:11
    we'll have a model
    for the rest of language,
  • 2:11 - 2:13
    and perhaps for critical periods
    that may exist in childhood
  • 2:13 - 2:16
    for social, emotional
    and cognitive development.
  • 2:17 - 2:19
    So we've been studying the babies
  • 2:19 - 2:21
    using a technique
    that we're using all over the world
  • 2:21 - 2:23
    and the sounds of all languages.
  • 2:23 - 2:25
    The baby sits on a parent's lap,
  • 2:25 - 2:28
    and we train them to turn
    their heads when a sound changes --
  • 2:28 - 2:30
    like from "ah" to "ee."
  • 2:30 - 2:33
    If they do so at the appropriate time,
    the black box lights up
  • 2:33 - 2:35
    and a panda bear pounds a drum.
  • 2:35 - 2:37
    A six-monther adores the task.
  • 2:38 - 2:39
    What have we learned?
  • 2:39 - 2:41
    Well, babies all over the world
  • 2:41 - 2:45
    are what I like to describe
    as "citizens of the world."
  • 2:45 - 2:48
    They can discriminate
    all the sounds of all languages,
  • 2:48 - 2:51
    no matter what country we're testing
    and what language we're using,
  • 2:51 - 2:54
    and that's remarkable
    because you and I can't do that.
  • 2:54 - 2:56
    We're culture-bound listeners.
  • 2:56 - 2:59
    We can discriminate the sounds
    of our own language,
  • 2:59 - 3:00
    but not those of foreign languages.
  • 3:00 - 3:04
    So the question arises:
    When do those citizens of the world
  • 3:04 - 3:07
    turn into the language-bound
    listeners that we are?
  • 3:07 - 3:10
    And the answer:
    before their first birthdays.
  • 3:10 - 3:13
    What you see here is performance
    on that head-turn task
  • 3:13 - 3:15
    for babies tested in Tokyo
    and the United States,
  • 3:15 - 3:17
    here in Seattle,
  • 3:17 - 3:19
    as they listened to "ra" and "la" --
  • 3:19 - 3:21
    sounds important to English,
    but not to Japanese.
  • 3:21 - 3:25
    So at six to eight months,
    the babies are totally equivalent.
  • 3:25 - 3:27
    Two months later,
    something incredible occurs.
  • 3:27 - 3:30
    The babies in the United States
    are getting a lot better,
  • 3:30 - 3:32
    babies in Japan are getting a lot worse,
  • 3:32 - 3:36
    but both of those groups of babies
    are preparing for exactly the language
  • 3:36 - 3:37
    that they are going to learn.
  • 3:37 - 3:42
    So the question is: What's happening
    during this critical two-month period?
  • 3:42 - 3:44
    This is the critical period
    for sound development,
  • 3:44 - 3:45
    but what's going on up there?
  • 3:46 - 3:47
    So there are two things going on.
  • 3:48 - 3:50
    The first is that the babies
    are listening intently to us,
  • 3:50 - 3:54
    and they're taking statistics
    as they listen to us talk --
  • 3:54 - 3:56
    they're taking statistics.
  • 3:56 - 3:59
    So listen to two mothers
    speaking motherese --
  • 3:59 - 4:02
    the universal language
    we use when we talk to kids --
  • 4:02 - 4:04
    first in English and then in Japanese.
  • 4:05 - 4:09
    (Video) Ah, I love your big blue eyes --
  • 4:09 - 4:12
    so pretty and nice.
  • 4:12 - 4:17
    (Japanese)
  • 4:19 - 4:21
    Patricia Kuhl: So, what I'm telling you
  • 4:21 - 4:25
    is that during the production
    of speech, when babies listen,
  • 4:25 - 4:30
    what they're doing is taking statistics
    on the language that they hear.
  • 4:31 - 4:34
    And those distributions grow.
  • 4:34 - 4:38
    And what we've learned is that babies
    are sensitive to the statistics,
  • 4:38 - 4:42
    and the statistics of Japanese
    and English are very, very different.
  • 4:43 - 4:45
    English has a lot of Rs and Ls.
  • 4:45 - 4:47
    The distribution shows.
  • 4:47 - 4:50
    And the distribution of Japanese
    is totally different,
  • 4:50 - 4:53
    where we see a group
    of intermediate sounds,
  • 4:53 - 4:55
    which is known as the Japanese "R."
  • 4:55 - 5:00
    So babies absorb
    the statistics of the language
  • 5:00 - 5:01
    and it changes their brains;
  • 5:01 - 5:03
    it changes them
    from the citizens of the world
  • 5:03 - 5:06
    to the culture-bound
    listeners that we are.
  • 5:06 - 5:11
    But we as adults are no longer
    absorbing those statistics.
  • 5:11 - 5:13
    We are governed
    by the representations in memory
  • 5:13 - 5:16
    that were formed early in development.
  • 5:16 - 5:17
    So what we're seeing here
  • 5:18 - 5:21
    is changing our models
    of what the critical period is about.
  • 5:21 - 5:24
    We're arguing from
    a mathematical standpoint
  • 5:24 - 5:27
    that the learning of language
    material may slow down
  • 5:27 - 5:29
    when our distributions stabilize.
  • 5:29 - 5:32
    It's raising lots of questions
    about bilingual people.
  • 5:32 - 5:36
    Bilinguals must keep two sets
    of statistics in mind at once
  • 5:36 - 5:39
    and flip between them,
    one after the other,
  • 5:39 - 5:41
    depending on who they're speaking to.
  • 5:41 - 5:42
    So we asked ourselves,
  • 5:42 - 5:46
    can the babies take statistics
    on a brand new language?
  • 5:46 - 5:49
    And we tested this
    by exposing American babies
  • 5:49 - 5:51
    who'd never heard a second language
  • 5:51 - 5:54
    to Mandarin for the first time
    during the critical period.
  • 5:54 - 5:57
    We knew that, when monolinguals
    were tested in Taipei and Seattle
  • 5:57 - 6:00
    on the Mandarin sounds,
    they showed the same pattern.
  • 6:00 - 6:02
    Six to eight months,
    they're totally equivalent.
  • 6:02 - 6:05
    Two months later,
    something incredible happens.
  • 6:05 - 6:08
    But the Taiwanese babies are getting
    better, not the American babies.
  • 6:08 - 6:13
    What we did was expose American babies,
    during this period, to Mandarin.
  • 6:13 - 6:16
    It was like having Mandarin relatives
    come and visit for a month
  • 6:16 - 6:20
    and move into your house
    and talk to the babies for 12 sessions.
  • 6:20 - 6:22
    Here's what it looked like
    in the laboratory.
  • 6:22 - 6:28
    (Mandarin)
  • 6:44 - 6:46
    PK: So what have we done
    to their little brains?
  • 6:46 - 6:48
    (Laughter)
  • 6:49 - 6:52
    We had to run a control group to make sure
  • 6:52 - 6:55
    that coming into the laboratory
    didn't improve your Mandarin skills.
  • 6:55 - 6:57
    So a group of babies came in
    and listened to English.
  • 6:58 - 6:59
    And we can see from the graph
  • 6:59 - 7:02
    that exposure to English
    didn't improve their Mandarin.
  • 7:02 - 7:05
    But look at what happened to the babies
    exposed to Mandarin for 12 sessions.
  • 7:05 - 7:08
    They were as good as the babies in Taiwan
  • 7:08 - 7:11
    who'd been listening
    for 10 and a half months.
  • 7:11 - 7:14
    What it demonstrated is that babies
    take statistics on a new language.
  • 7:14 - 7:18
    Whatever you put in front of them,
    they'll take statistics on.
  • 7:18 - 7:19
    But we wondered what role
  • 7:19 - 7:23
    the human being played
    in this learning exercise.
  • 7:23 - 7:27
    So we ran another group of babies
    in which the kids got the same dosage,
  • 7:27 - 7:30
    the same 12 sessions,
    but over a television set.
  • 7:30 - 7:33
    And another group of babies
    who had just audio exposure
  • 7:33 - 7:36
    and looked at a teddy bear on the screen.
  • 7:36 - 7:38
    What did we do to their brains?
  • 7:38 - 7:41
    What you see here is the audio result --
  • 7:42 - 7:43
    no learning whatsoever --
  • 7:43 - 7:45
    and the video result --
  • 7:46 - 7:48
    no learning whatsoever.
  • 7:48 - 7:52
    It takes a human being
    for babies to take their statistics.
  • 7:52 - 7:54
    The social brain is controlling
  • 7:54 - 7:57
    when the babies
    are taking their statistics.
  • 7:57 - 8:00
    We want to get inside the brain
    and see this thing happening
  • 8:00 - 8:04
    as babies are in front of televisions,
    as opposed to in front of human beings.
  • 8:04 - 8:09
    Thankfully, we have a new machine,
    magnetoencephalography,
  • 8:09 - 8:10
    that allows us to do this.
  • 8:10 - 8:13
    It looks like a hair dryer from Mars.
  • 8:13 - 8:17
    But it's completely safe,
    completely noninvasive and silent.
  • 8:17 - 8:19
    And babies - we're looking at
  • 8:19 - 8:21
    millimeter accuracy
  • 8:21 - 8:24
    with regard to spatial
    and millisecond accuracy
  • 8:24 - 8:26
    using 306 SQUIDs --
  • 8:26 - 8:29
    these are superconducting
    quantum interference devices --
  • 8:29 - 8:33
    to pick up the magnetic fields
    that change as we do our thinking.
  • 8:33 - 8:39
    We're the first in the world
    to record babies in an MEG machine
  • 8:39 - 8:41
    while they are learning.
  • 8:41 - 8:43
    So this is little Emma.
  • 8:43 - 8:45
    She's a six-monther.
  • 8:45 - 8:49
    And she's listening to various languages
    in the earphones.
  • 8:49 - 8:51
    You can see, she can move around.
  • 8:51 - 8:55
    We're tracking her head
    with little pellets in a cap,
  • 8:55 - 8:58
    so she's free to move
    completely unconstrained.
  • 8:58 - 9:00
    It's a technical tour de force.
  • 9:00 - 9:01
    What are we seeing?
  • 9:01 - 9:03
    We're seeing the baby brain.
  • 9:03 - 9:08
    As the baby hears a word in her language,
    the auditory areas light up,
  • 9:08 - 9:13
    and then subsequently areas surrounding it
    that we think are related to coherence,
  • 9:13 - 9:17
    getting the brain coordinated
    with its different areas, and causality,
  • 9:17 - 9:20
    one brain area
    causing another to activate.
  • 9:20 - 9:25
    We are embarking on a grand
    and golden age.
  • 9:25 - 9:27
    We're going to be able
    to see a child's brain
  • 9:27 - 9:31
    as they experience an emotion,
    as they learn to speak and read,
  • 9:31 - 9:35
    as they solve a math problem,
    as they have an idea.
  • 9:35 - 9:38
    And we're going to be able to invent
    brain-based interventions
  • 9:38 - 9:41
    for children who have difficulty learning.
  • 9:41 - 9:44
    Just as the poets and writers described,
  • 9:44 - 9:48
    we're going to be able to see, I think,
    that wondrous openness,
  • 9:48 - 9:52
    utter and complete openness,
    of the mind of a child.
  • 9:53 - 9:55
    In investigating the child's brain,
  • 9:55 - 9:59
    we're going to uncover deep truths
    about what it means to be human,
  • 9:59 - 10:00
    and in the process,
  • 10:00 - 10:03
    we may be able to help keep
    our own minds open to learning
  • 10:04 - 10:05
    for our entire lives.
  • 10:05 - 10:06
    Thank you.
  • 10:06 - 10:10
    (Applause)
Title:
The linguistic genius of babies | Patricia Kuhl | TEDxRainier
Description:

Patricia Kuhl shares astonishing findings about how babies learn one language over another -- by listening to the humans around them and "taking statistics" on the sounds they need to know. Clever lab experiments (and brain scans) show how 6-month-old babies use sophisticated reasoning to understand their world.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDxTalks
Duration:
10:14
TED Translators admin edited English subtitles for TEDxRainier - Dr. Patricia Kuhl
TED Translators admin edited English subtitles for TEDxRainier - Dr. Patricia Kuhl

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions