What we learn before we're born
-
0:00 - 0:03My subject today is learning.
-
0:03 - 0:06And in that spirit, I want to spring on you all a pop quiz.
-
0:06 - 0:08Ready?
-
0:08 - 0:11When does learning begin?
-
0:11 - 0:13Now as you ponder that question,
-
0:13 - 0:15maybe you're thinking about the first day of preschool
-
0:15 - 0:17or kindergarten,
-
0:17 - 0:20the first time that kids are in a classroom with a teacher.
-
0:20 - 0:23Or maybe you've called to mind the toddler phase
-
0:23 - 0:26when children are learning how to walk and talk
-
0:26 - 0:28and use a fork.
-
0:28 - 0:31Maybe you've encountered the Zero-to-Three movement,
-
0:31 - 0:34which asserts that the most important years for learning
-
0:34 - 0:36are the earliest ones.
-
0:36 - 0:39And so your answer to my question would be:
-
0:39 - 0:41Learning begins at birth.
-
0:41 - 0:43Well today I want to present to you
-
0:43 - 0:46an idea that may be surprising
-
0:46 - 0:49and may even seem implausible,
-
0:49 - 0:51but which is supported by the latest evidence
-
0:51 - 0:54from psychology and biology.
-
0:54 - 0:57And that is that some of the most important learning we ever do
-
0:57 - 0:59happens before we're born,
-
0:59 - 1:02while we're still in the womb.
-
1:02 - 1:04Now I'm a science reporter.
-
1:04 - 1:06I write books and magazine articles.
-
1:06 - 1:08And I'm also a mother.
-
1:08 - 1:11And those two roles came together for me
-
1:11 - 1:14in a book that I wrote called "Origins."
-
1:14 - 1:17"Origins" is a report from the front lines
-
1:17 - 1:19of an exciting new field
-
1:19 - 1:21called fetal origins.
-
1:21 - 1:24Fetal origins is a scientific discipline
-
1:24 - 1:27that emerged just about two decades ago,
-
1:27 - 1:30and it's based on the theory
-
1:30 - 1:33that our health and well-being throughout our lives
-
1:33 - 1:35is crucially affected
-
1:35 - 1:38by the nine months we spend in the womb.
-
1:38 - 1:42Now this theory was of more than just intellectual interest to me.
-
1:42 - 1:44I was myself pregnant
-
1:44 - 1:47while I was doing the research for the book.
-
1:47 - 1:49And one of the most fascinating insights
-
1:49 - 1:51I took from this work
-
1:51 - 1:54is that we're all learning about the world
-
1:54 - 1:57even before we enter it.
-
1:57 - 1:59When we hold our babies for the first time,
-
1:59 - 2:02we might imagine that they're clean slates,
-
2:02 - 2:04unmarked by life,
-
2:04 - 2:07when in fact, they've already been shaped by us
-
2:07 - 2:11and by the particular world we live in.
-
2:11 - 2:13Today I want to share with you some of the amazing things
-
2:13 - 2:15that scientists are discovering
-
2:15 - 2:17about what fetuses learn
-
2:17 - 2:20while they're still in their mothers' bellies.
-
2:21 - 2:23First of all,
-
2:23 - 2:26they learn the sound of their mothers' voices.
-
2:26 - 2:29Because sounds from the outside world
-
2:29 - 2:32have to travel through the mother's abdominal tissue
-
2:32 - 2:36and through the amniotic fluid that surrounds the fetus,
-
2:36 - 2:38the voices fetuses hear,
-
2:38 - 2:41starting around the fourth month of gestation,
-
2:41 - 2:43are muted and muffled.
-
2:43 - 2:45One researcher says
-
2:45 - 2:48that they probably sound a lot like the the voice of Charlie Brown's teacher
-
2:48 - 2:51in the old "Peanuts" cartoon.
-
2:51 - 2:54But the pregnant woman's own voice
-
2:54 - 2:56reverberates through her body,
-
2:56 - 2:59reaching the fetus much more readily.
-
2:59 - 3:02And because the fetus is with her all the time,
-
3:02 - 3:05it hears her voice a lot.
-
3:05 - 3:08Once the baby's born, it recognizes her voice
-
3:08 - 3:10and it prefers listening to her voice
-
3:10 - 3:12over anyone else's.
-
3:12 - 3:14How can we know this?
-
3:14 - 3:16Newborn babies can't do much,
-
3:16 - 3:19but one thing they're really good at is sucking.
-
3:19 - 3:22Researchers take advantage of this fact
-
3:22 - 3:25by rigging up two rubber nipples,
-
3:25 - 3:27so that if a baby sucks on one,
-
3:27 - 3:29it hears a recording of its mother's voice
-
3:29 - 3:31on a pair of headphones,
-
3:31 - 3:33and if it sucks on the other nipple,
-
3:33 - 3:37it hears a recording of a female stranger's voice.
-
3:37 - 3:40Babies quickly show their preference
-
3:40 - 3:43by choosing the first one.
-
3:43 - 3:46Scientists also take advantage of the fact
-
3:46 - 3:48that babies will slow down their sucking
-
3:48 - 3:50when something interests them
-
3:50 - 3:52and resume their fast sucking
-
3:52 - 3:55when they get bored.
-
3:55 - 3:57This is how researchers discovered
-
3:57 - 4:00that, after women repeatedly read aloud
-
4:00 - 4:04a section of Dr. Seuss' "The Cat in the Hat" while they were pregnant,
-
4:04 - 4:07their newborn babies recognized that passage
-
4:07 - 4:10when they hear it outside the womb.
-
4:10 - 4:13My favorite experiment of this kind
-
4:13 - 4:15is the one that showed that the babies
-
4:15 - 4:17of women who watched a certain soap opera
-
4:17 - 4:20every day during pregnancy
-
4:20 - 4:23recognized the theme song of that show
-
4:23 - 4:26once they were born.
-
4:26 - 4:28So fetuses are even learning
-
4:28 - 4:31about the particular language that's spoken
-
4:31 - 4:33in the world that they'll be born into.
-
4:33 - 4:36A study published last year
-
4:36 - 4:39found that from birth, from the moment of birth,
-
4:39 - 4:41babies cry in the accent
-
4:41 - 4:44of their mother's native language.
-
4:44 - 4:47French babies cry on a rising note
-
4:47 - 4:50while German babies end on a falling note,
-
4:50 - 4:52imitating the melodic contours
-
4:52 - 4:54of those languages.
-
4:54 - 4:56Now why would this kind of fetal learning
-
4:56 - 4:58be useful?
-
4:58 - 5:01It may have evolved to aid the baby's survival.
-
5:01 - 5:03From the moment of birth,
-
5:03 - 5:05the baby responds most to the voice
-
5:05 - 5:07of the person who is most likely to care for it --
-
5:07 - 5:09its mother.
-
5:09 - 5:11It even makes its cries
-
5:11 - 5:13sound like the mother's language,
-
5:13 - 5:16which may further endear the baby to the mother,
-
5:16 - 5:18and which may give the baby a head start
-
5:18 - 5:20in the critical task
-
5:20 - 5:23of learning how to understand and speak
-
5:23 - 5:25its native language.
-
5:25 - 5:27But it's not just sounds
-
5:27 - 5:29that fetuses are learning about in utero.
-
5:29 - 5:32It's also tastes and smells.
-
5:32 - 5:34By seven months of gestation,
-
5:34 - 5:36the fetus' taste buds are fully developed,
-
5:36 - 5:39and its olfactory receptors, which allow it to smell,
-
5:39 - 5:41are functioning.
-
5:41 - 5:44The flavors of the food a pregnant woman eats
-
5:44 - 5:46find their way into the amniotic fluid,
-
5:46 - 5:48which is continuously swallowed
-
5:48 - 5:50by the fetus.
-
5:50 - 5:53Babies seem to remember and prefer these tastes
-
5:53 - 5:56once they're out in the world.
-
5:56 - 5:59In one experiment, a group of pregnant women
-
5:59 - 6:01was asked to drink a lot of carrot juice
-
6:01 - 6:04during their third trimester of pregnancy,
-
6:04 - 6:06while another group of pregnant women
-
6:06 - 6:08drank only water.
-
6:08 - 6:11Six months later, the women's infants
-
6:11 - 6:14were offered cereal mixed with carrot juice,
-
6:14 - 6:18and their facial expressions were observed while they ate it.
-
6:18 - 6:20The offspring of the carrot juice drinking women
-
6:20 - 6:22ate more carrot-flavored cereal,
-
6:22 - 6:24and from the looks of it,
-
6:24 - 6:26they seemed to enjoy it more.
-
6:26 - 6:29A sort of French version of this experiment
-
6:29 - 6:31was carried out in Dijon, France
-
6:31 - 6:33where researchers found
-
6:33 - 6:36that mothers who consumed food and drink
-
6:36 - 6:41flavored with licorice-flavored anise during pregnancy
-
6:41 - 6:43showed a preference for anise
-
6:43 - 6:45on their first day of life,
-
6:45 - 6:47and again, when they were tested later,
-
6:47 - 6:49on their fourth day of life.
-
6:49 - 6:53Babies whose mothers did not eat anise during pregnancy
-
6:53 - 6:57showed a reaction that translated roughly as "yuck."
-
6:57 - 6:59What this means
-
6:59 - 7:01is that fetuses are effectively being taught by their mothers
-
7:01 - 7:04about what is safe and good to eat.
-
7:04 - 7:06Fetuses are also being taught
-
7:06 - 7:09about the particular culture that they'll be joining
-
7:09 - 7:12through one of culture's most powerful expressions,
-
7:12 - 7:14which is food.
-
7:14 - 7:17They're being introduced to the characteristic flavors and spices
-
7:17 - 7:19of their culture's cuisine
-
7:19 - 7:22even before birth.
-
7:22 - 7:25Now it turns out that fetuses are learning even bigger lessons.
-
7:25 - 7:27But before I get to that,
-
7:27 - 7:31I want to address something that you may be wondering about.
-
7:31 - 7:33The notion of fetal learning
-
7:33 - 7:36may conjure up for you attempts to enrich the fetus --
-
7:36 - 7:38like playing Mozart through headphones
-
7:38 - 7:40placed on a pregnant belly.
-
7:40 - 7:43But actually, the nine-month-long process
-
7:43 - 7:46of molding and shaping that goes on in the womb
-
7:46 - 7:50is a lot more visceral and consequential than that.
-
7:50 - 7:54Much of what a pregnant woman encounters in her daily life --
-
7:54 - 7:56the air she breathes,
-
7:56 - 7:58the food and drink she consumes,
-
7:58 - 8:00the chemicals she's exposed to,
-
8:00 - 8:02even the emotions she feels --
-
8:02 - 8:05are shared in some fashion with her fetus.
-
8:05 - 8:08They make up a mix of influences
-
8:08 - 8:10as individual and idiosyncratic
-
8:10 - 8:12as the woman herself.
-
8:12 - 8:14The fetus incorporates these offerings
-
8:14 - 8:16into its own body,
-
8:16 - 8:19makes them part of its flesh and blood.
-
8:19 - 8:21And often it does something more.
-
8:21 - 8:24It treats these maternal contributions
-
8:24 - 8:26as information,
-
8:26 - 8:28as what I like to call biological postcards
-
8:28 - 8:31from the world outside.
-
8:31 - 8:34So what a fetus is learning about in utero
-
8:34 - 8:36is not Mozart's "Magic Flute"
-
8:36 - 8:40but answers to questions much more critical to its survival.
-
8:40 - 8:42Will it be born into a world of abundance
-
8:42 - 8:44or scarcity?
-
8:44 - 8:47Will it be safe and protected,
-
8:47 - 8:50or will it face constant dangers and threats?
-
8:50 - 8:52Will it live a long, fruitful life
-
8:52 - 8:55or a short, harried one?
-
8:55 - 8:58The pregnant woman's diet and stress level in particular
-
8:58 - 9:01provide important clues to prevailing conditions
-
9:01 - 9:04like a finger lifted to the wind.
-
9:04 - 9:06The resulting tuning and tweaking
-
9:06 - 9:09of a fetus' brain and other organs
-
9:09 - 9:11are part of what give us humans
-
9:11 - 9:13our enormous flexibility,
-
9:13 - 9:15our ability to thrive
-
9:15 - 9:17in a huge variety of environments,
-
9:17 - 9:19from the country to the city,
-
9:19 - 9:22from the tundra to the desert.
-
9:22 - 9:24To conclude, I want to tell you two stories
-
9:24 - 9:27about how mothers teach their children about the world
-
9:27 - 9:30even before they're born.
-
9:31 - 9:33In the autumn of 1944,
-
9:33 - 9:36the darkest days of World War II,
-
9:36 - 9:39German troops blockaded Western Holland,
-
9:39 - 9:42turning away all shipments of food.
-
9:42 - 9:44The opening of the Nazi's siege
-
9:44 - 9:47was followed by one of the harshest winters in decades --
-
9:47 - 9:51so cold the water in the canals froze solid.
-
9:51 - 9:53Soon food became scarce,
-
9:53 - 9:57with many Dutch surviving on just 500 calories a day --
-
9:57 - 10:00a quarter of what they consumed before the war.
-
10:00 - 10:03As weeks of deprivation stretched into months,
-
10:03 - 10:06some resorted to eating tulip bulbs.
-
10:06 - 10:08By the beginning of May,
-
10:08 - 10:10the nation's carefully rationed food reserve
-
10:10 - 10:12was completely exhausted.
-
10:12 - 10:15The specter of mass starvation loomed.
-
10:15 - 10:18And then on May 5th, 1945,
-
10:18 - 10:20the siege came to a sudden end
-
10:20 - 10:22when Holland was liberated
-
10:22 - 10:24by the Allies.
-
10:24 - 10:27The "Hunger Winter," as it came to be known,
-
10:27 - 10:29killed some 10,000 people
-
10:29 - 10:31and weakened thousands more.
-
10:31 - 10:34But there was another population that was affected --
-
10:34 - 10:36the 40,000 fetuses
-
10:36 - 10:39in utero during the siege.
-
10:39 - 10:41Some of the effects of malnutrition during pregnancy
-
10:41 - 10:43were immediately apparent
-
10:43 - 10:45in higher rates of stillbirths,
-
10:45 - 10:47birth defects, low birth weights
-
10:47 - 10:49and infant mortality.
-
10:49 - 10:52But others wouldn't be discovered for many years.
-
10:52 - 10:54Decades after the "Hunger Winter,"
-
10:54 - 10:56researchers documented
-
10:56 - 11:00that people whose mothers were pregnant during the siege
-
11:00 - 11:02have more obesity, more diabetes
-
11:02 - 11:05and more heart disease in later life
-
11:05 - 11:08than individuals who were gestated under normal conditions.
-
11:08 - 11:12These individuals' prenatal experience of starvation
-
11:12 - 11:14seems to have changed their bodies
-
11:14 - 11:16in myriad ways.
-
11:16 - 11:18They have higher blood pressure,
-
11:18 - 11:20poorer cholesterol profiles
-
11:20 - 11:22and reduced glucose tolerance --
-
11:22 - 11:25a precursor of diabetes.
-
11:25 - 11:27Why would undernutrition in the womb
-
11:27 - 11:29result in disease later?
-
11:29 - 11:31One explanation
-
11:31 - 11:34is that fetuses are making the best of a bad situation.
-
11:34 - 11:36When food is scarce,
-
11:36 - 11:39they divert nutrients towards the really critical organ, the brain,
-
11:39 - 11:41and away from other organs
-
11:41 - 11:43like the heart and liver.
-
11:43 - 11:46This keeps the fetus alive in the short-term,
-
11:46 - 11:49but the bill comes due later on in life
-
11:49 - 11:51when those other organs, deprived early on,
-
11:51 - 11:54become more susceptible to disease.
-
11:54 - 11:57But that may not be all that's going on.
-
11:57 - 11:59It seems that fetuses are taking cues
-
11:59 - 12:02from the intrauterine environment
-
12:02 - 12:04and tailoring their physiology accordingly.
-
12:04 - 12:06They're preparing themselves
-
12:06 - 12:08for the kind of world they will encounter
-
12:08 - 12:10on the other side of the womb.
-
12:10 - 12:12The fetus adjusts its metabolism
-
12:12 - 12:15and other physiological processes
-
12:15 - 12:18in anticipation of the environment that awaits it.
-
12:18 - 12:21And the basis of the fetus' prediction
-
12:21 - 12:23is what its mother eats.
-
12:23 - 12:25The meals a pregnant woman consumes
-
12:25 - 12:27constitute a kind of story,
-
12:27 - 12:29a fairy tale of abundance
-
12:29 - 12:32or a grim chronicle of deprivation.
-
12:32 - 12:35This story imparts information
-
12:35 - 12:37that the fetus uses
-
12:37 - 12:39to organize its body and its systems --
-
12:39 - 12:42an adaptation to prevailing circumstances
-
12:42 - 12:45that facilitates its future survival.
-
12:45 - 12:48Faced with severely limited resources,
-
12:48 - 12:51a smaller-sized child with reduced energy requirements
-
12:51 - 12:53will, in fact, have a better chance
-
12:53 - 12:55of living to adulthood.
-
12:55 - 12:57The real trouble comes
-
12:57 - 13:00when pregnant women are, in a sense, unreliable narrators,
-
13:00 - 13:02when fetuses are led
-
13:02 - 13:04to expect a world of scarcity
-
13:04 - 13:07and are born instead into a world of plenty.
-
13:07 - 13:10This is what happened to the children of the Dutch "Hunger Winter."
-
13:10 - 13:12And their higher rates of obesity,
-
13:12 - 13:14diabetes and heart disease
-
13:14 - 13:16are the result.
-
13:16 - 13:19Bodies that were built to hang onto every calorie
-
13:19 - 13:21found themselves swimming in the superfluous calories
-
13:21 - 13:24of the post-war Western diet.
-
13:24 - 13:27The world they had learned about while in utero
-
13:27 - 13:29was not the same
-
13:29 - 13:32as the world into which they were born.
-
13:32 - 13:34Here's another story.
-
13:34 - 13:38At 8:46 a.m. on September 11th, 2001,
-
13:38 - 13:40there were tens of thousands of people
-
13:40 - 13:42in the vicinity of the World Trade Center
-
13:42 - 13:44in New York --
-
13:44 - 13:46commuters spilling off trains,
-
13:46 - 13:49waitresses setting tables for the morning rush,
-
13:49 - 13:53brokers already working the phones on Wall Street.
-
13:53 - 13:561,700 of these people were pregnant women.
-
13:56 - 13:59When the planes struck and the towers collapsed,
-
13:59 - 14:02many of these women experienced the same horrors
-
14:02 - 14:05inflicted on other survivors of the disaster --
-
14:05 - 14:07the overwhelming chaos and confusion,
-
14:07 - 14:09the rolling clouds
-
14:09 - 14:13of potentially toxic dust and debris,
-
14:13 - 14:15the heart-pounding fear for their lives.
-
14:15 - 14:17About a year after 9/11,
-
14:17 - 14:20researchers examined a group of women
-
14:20 - 14:22who were pregnant
-
14:22 - 14:24when they were exposed to the World Trade Center attack.
-
14:24 - 14:26In the babies of those women
-
14:26 - 14:29who developed post-traumatic stress syndrome, or PTSD,
-
14:29 - 14:31following their ordeal,
-
14:31 - 14:34researchers discovered a biological marker
-
14:34 - 14:36of susceptibility to PTSD --
-
14:36 - 14:39an effect that was most pronounced
-
14:39 - 14:42in infants whose mothers experienced the catastrophe
-
14:42 - 14:44in their third trimester.
-
14:44 - 14:46In other words,
-
14:46 - 14:49the mothers with post-traumatic stress syndrome
-
14:49 - 14:52had passed on a vulnerability to the condition
-
14:52 - 14:55to their children while they were still in utero.
-
14:55 - 14:57Now consider this:
-
14:57 - 14:59post-traumatic stress syndrome
-
14:59 - 15:02appears to be a reaction to stress gone very wrong,
-
15:02 - 15:06causing its victims tremendous unnecessary suffering.
-
15:06 - 15:09But there's another way of thinking about PTSD.
-
15:09 - 15:12What looks like pathology to us
-
15:12 - 15:14may actually be a useful adaptation
-
15:14 - 15:16in some circumstances.
-
15:16 - 15:19In a particularly dangerous environment,
-
15:19 - 15:22the characteristic manifestations of PTSD --
-
15:22 - 15:25a hyper-awareness of one's surroundings,
-
15:25 - 15:28a quick-trigger response to danger --
-
15:28 - 15:31could save someone's life.
-
15:31 - 15:35The notion that the prenatal transmission of PTSD risk is adaptive
-
15:35 - 15:37is still speculative,
-
15:37 - 15:40but I find it rather poignant.
-
15:40 - 15:42It would mean that, even before birth,
-
15:42 - 15:44mothers are warning their children
-
15:44 - 15:46that it's a wild world out there,
-
15:46 - 15:49telling them, "Be careful."
-
15:49 - 15:51Let me be clear.
-
15:51 - 15:54Fetal origins research is not about blaming women
-
15:54 - 15:56for what happens during pregnancy.
-
15:56 - 15:59It's about discovering how best to promote
-
15:59 - 16:02the health and well-being of the next generation.
-
16:02 - 16:04That important effort must include a focus
-
16:04 - 16:06on what fetuses learn
-
16:06 - 16:09during the nine months they spend in the womb.
-
16:09 - 16:12Learning is one of life's most essential activities,
-
16:12 - 16:14and it begins much earlier
-
16:14 - 16:16than we ever imagined.
-
16:16 - 16:18Thank you.
-
16:18 - 16:25(Applause)
- Title:
- What we learn before we're born
- Speaker:
- Annie Murphy Paul
- Description:
-
Pop quiz: When does learning begin? Answer: Before we are born. Science writer Annie Murphy Paul talks through new research that shows how much we learn in the womb -- from the lilt of our native language to our soon-to-be-favorite foods.
- Video Language:
- English
- Team:
closed TED
- Project:
- TEDTalks
- Duration:
- 16:26
![]() |
TED edited English subtitles for What we learn before we're born | |
![]() |
TED added a translation |