Why do people fear the wrong things? - Gerd Gigerenzer
-
0:06 - 0:11A new drug reduces
the risk of heart attacks by 40%. -
0:11 - 0:14Shark attacks are up by a factor of two.
-
0:14 - 0:19Drinking a liter of soda per day
doubles your chance of developing cancer. -
0:19 - 0:22These are all examples of relative risk,
-
0:22 - 0:26a common way risk
is presented in news articles. -
0:26 - 0:30Risk evaluation is a complicated tangle
of statistical thinking -
0:30 - 0:32and personal preference.
-
0:32 - 0:35One common stumbling block
is the difference between -
0:35 - 0:40relative risks like these
and what are called absolute risks. -
0:40 - 0:43Risk is the likelihood
that an event will occur. -
0:43 - 0:45It can be expressed
as either a percentage— -
0:45 - 0:49for example, that heart attacks
occur in 11% of men -
0:49 - 0:51between the ages of 60 and 79—
-
0:51 - 0:57or as a rate— that one in two million
divers along Australia’s western coast -
0:57 - 1:00will suffer a fatal shark bite each year.
-
1:00 - 1:04These numbers express
the absolute risk of heart attacks -
1:04 - 1:06and shark attacks in these groups.
-
1:06 - 1:12Changes in risk can be expressed
in relative or absolute terms. -
1:12 - 1:16For example, a review in 2009
found that mammography screenings -
1:16 - 1:23reduced the number of breast cancer deaths
from five women in one thousand to four. -
1:23 - 1:26The absolute risk reduction
was about .1%. -
1:26 - 1:31But the relative risk reduction
from 5 cases of cancer mortality to four -
1:31 - 1:33is 20%.
-
1:33 - 1:35Based on reports of this higher number,
-
1:35 - 1:38people overestimated
the impact of screening. -
1:38 - 1:42To see why the difference between
the two ways of expressing risk matters, -
1:42 - 1:45let’s consider
the hypothetical example of a drug -
1:45 - 1:49that reduces heart attack risk by 40%.
-
1:49 - 1:51Imagine that out of a group
of 1,000 people -
1:51 - 1:55who didn’t take the new drug,
10 would have heart attacks. -
1:55 - 2:00The absolute risk
is 10 out of 1,000, or 1%. -
2:00 - 2:03If a similar group of 1,000 people
did take the drug, -
2:03 - 2:06the number of heart attacks would be six.
-
2:06 - 2:10In other words, the drug could prevent
four out of ten heart attacks— -
2:10 - 2:13a relative risk reduction of 40%.
-
2:13 - 2:19Meanwhile, the absolute risk
only dropped from 1% to 0.6%— -
2:19 - 2:24but the 40% relative risk decrease
sounds a lot more significant. -
2:24 - 2:27Surely preventing
even a handful of heart attacks, -
2:27 - 2:31or any other negative outcome,
is worthwhile— isn’t it? -
2:31 - 2:33Not necessarily.
-
2:33 - 2:36The problem is that choices
that reduce some risks -
2:36 - 2:39can put you in the path of others.
-
2:39 - 2:45Suppose the heart-attack drug caused
cancer in one half of 1% of patients. -
2:45 - 2:47In our group of 1,000 people,
-
2:47 - 2:50four heart attacks
would be prevented by taking the drug, -
2:50 - 2:54but there would be
five new cases of cancer. -
2:54 - 2:58The relative reduction
in heart attack risk sounds substantial -
2:58 - 3:01and the absolute risk of cancer
sounds small, -
3:01 - 3:04but they work out
to about the same number of cases. -
3:04 - 3:05In real life,
-
3:05 - 3:08everyone’s individual evaluation of risk
will vary -
3:08 - 3:11depending on
their personal circumstances. -
3:11 - 3:14If you know you have a family history
of heart disease -
3:14 - 3:17you might be more strongly motivated
to take a medication -
3:17 - 3:19that would lower your heart-attack risk,
-
3:19 - 3:24even knowing it provided
only a small reduction in absolute risk. -
3:24 - 3:28Sometimes, we have to decide between
exposing ourselves to risks -
3:28 - 3:30that aren’t directly comparable.
-
3:30 - 3:33If, for example, the heart attack drug
carried a higher risk -
3:33 - 3:36of a debilitating,
but not life-threatening, -
3:36 - 3:39side effect like migraines
rather than cancer, -
3:39 - 3:43our evaluation of whether that risk
is worth taking might change. -
3:43 - 3:47And sometimes there isn’t necessarily
a correct choice: -
3:47 - 3:52some might say even a minuscule risk
of shark attack is worth avoiding, -
3:52 - 3:54because all you’d miss out on
is an ocean swim, -
3:54 - 3:57while others wouldn’t even consider
skipping a swim -
3:57 - 4:01to avoid an objectively tiny risk
of shark attack. -
4:01 - 4:05For all these reasons,
risk evaluation is tricky at baseline, -
4:05 - 4:08and reporting on risk can be misleading,
-
4:08 - 4:11especially when it shares some numbers
in absolute terms -
4:11 - 4:14and others in relative terms.
-
4:14 - 4:16Understanding how these measures work
-
4:16 - 4:19will help you cut through
some of the confusion -
4:19 - 4:21and better evaluate risk.
- Title:
- Why do people fear the wrong things? - Gerd Gigerenzer
- Speaker:
- Gerd Gigerenzer
- Description:
-
View full lesson: https://ed.ted.com/lessons/how-likely-is-a-shark-attack-gerd-gigerenzer
A new drug reduces the risk of heart attacks by 40%. Shark attacks are up by a factor of two. Drinking a liter of soda per day doubles your chance of developing cancer. These are all examples of a common way risk is presented in news articles, and can often be misleading. So how can we better evaluate risk? Gerd Gigerenzer explores the difference between relative and absolute risk.
Lesson by Gerd Gigerenzer, directed by visorama.
- Video Language:
- English
- Team:
closed TED
- Project:
- TED-Ed
- Duration:
- 04:21
![]() |
lauren mcalpine edited English subtitles for Why do people fear the wrong things? | |
![]() |
Elise Haadsma approved English subtitles for Why do people fear the wrong things? | |
![]() |
Elise Haadsma accepted English subtitles for Why do people fear the wrong things? | |
![]() |
lauren mcalpine edited English subtitles for Why do people fear the wrong things? |