< Return to Video

Why do people fear the wrong things? - Gerd Gigerenzer

  • 0:06 - 0:11
    A new drug reduces
    the risk of heart attacks by 40%.
  • 0:11 - 0:14
    Shark attacks are up by a factor of two.
  • 0:14 - 0:19
    Drinking a liter of soda per day
    doubles your chance of developing cancer.
  • 0:19 - 0:22
    These are all examples of relative risk,
  • 0:22 - 0:26
    a common way risk
    is presented in news articles.
  • 0:26 - 0:30
    Risk evaluation is a complicated tangle
    of statistical thinking
  • 0:30 - 0:32
    and personal preference.
  • 0:32 - 0:35
    One common stumbling block
    is the difference between
  • 0:35 - 0:40
    relative risks like these
    and what are called absolute risks.
  • 0:40 - 0:43
    Risk is the likelihood
    that an event will occur.
  • 0:43 - 0:45
    It can be expressed
    as either a percentage—
  • 0:45 - 0:49
    for example, that heart attacks
    occur in 11% of men
  • 0:49 - 0:51
    between the ages of 60 and 79—
  • 0:51 - 0:57
    or as a rate— that one in two million
    divers along Australia’s western coast
  • 0:57 - 1:00
    will suffer a fatal shark bite each year.
  • 1:00 - 1:04
    These numbers express
    the absolute risk of heart attacks
  • 1:04 - 1:06
    and shark attacks in these groups.
  • 1:06 - 1:12
    Changes in risk can be expressed
    in relative or absolute terms.
  • 1:12 - 1:16
    For example, a review in 2009
    found that mammography screenings
  • 1:16 - 1:23
    reduced the number of breast cancer deaths
    from five women in one thousand to four.
  • 1:23 - 1:26
    The absolute risk reduction
    was about .1%.
  • 1:26 - 1:31
    But the relative risk reduction
    from 5 cases of cancer mortality to four
  • 1:31 - 1:33
    is 20%.
  • 1:33 - 1:35
    Based on reports of this higher number,
  • 1:35 - 1:38
    people overestimated
    the impact of screening.
  • 1:38 - 1:42
    To see why the difference between
    the two ways of expressing risk matters,
  • 1:42 - 1:45
    let’s consider
    the hypothetical example of a drug
  • 1:45 - 1:49
    that reduces heart attack risk by 40%.
  • 1:49 - 1:51
    Imagine that out of a group
    of 1,000 people
  • 1:51 - 1:55
    who didn’t take the new drug,
    10 would have heart attacks.
  • 1:55 - 2:00
    The absolute risk
    is 10 out of 1,000, or 1%.
  • 2:00 - 2:03
    If a similar group of 1,000 people
    did take the drug,
  • 2:03 - 2:06
    the number of heart attacks would be six.
  • 2:06 - 2:10
    In other words, the drug could prevent
    four out of ten heart attacks—
  • 2:10 - 2:13
    a relative risk reduction of 40%.
  • 2:13 - 2:19
    Meanwhile, the absolute risk
    only dropped from 1% to 0.6%—
  • 2:19 - 2:24
    but the 40% relative risk decrease
    sounds a lot more significant.
  • 2:24 - 2:27
    Surely preventing
    even a handful of heart attacks,
  • 2:27 - 2:31
    or any other negative outcome,
    is worthwhile— isn’t it?
  • 2:31 - 2:33
    Not necessarily.
  • 2:33 - 2:36
    The problem is that choices
    that reduce some risks
  • 2:36 - 2:39
    can put you in the path of others.
  • 2:39 - 2:45
    Suppose the heart-attack drug caused
    cancer in one half of 1% of patients.
  • 2:45 - 2:47
    In our group of 1,000 people,
  • 2:47 - 2:50
    four heart attacks
    would be prevented by taking the drug,
  • 2:50 - 2:54
    but there would be
    five new cases of cancer.
  • 2:54 - 2:58
    The relative reduction
    in heart attack risk sounds substantial
  • 2:58 - 3:01
    and the absolute risk of cancer
    sounds small,
  • 3:01 - 3:04
    but they work out
    to about the same number of cases.
  • 3:04 - 3:05
    In real life,
  • 3:05 - 3:08
    everyone’s individual evaluation of risk
    will vary
  • 3:08 - 3:11
    depending on
    their personal circumstances.
  • 3:11 - 3:14
    If you know you have a family history
    of heart disease
  • 3:14 - 3:17
    you might be more strongly motivated
    to take a medication
  • 3:17 - 3:19
    that would lower your heart-attack risk,
  • 3:19 - 3:24
    even knowing it provided
    only a small reduction in absolute risk.
  • 3:24 - 3:28
    Sometimes, we have to decide between
    exposing ourselves to risks
  • 3:28 - 3:30
    that aren’t directly comparable.
  • 3:30 - 3:33
    If, for example, the heart attack drug
    carried a higher risk
  • 3:33 - 3:36
    of a debilitating,
    but not life-threatening,
  • 3:36 - 3:39
    side effect like migraines
    rather than cancer,
  • 3:39 - 3:43
    our evaluation of whether that risk
    is worth taking might change.
  • 3:43 - 3:47
    And sometimes there isn’t necessarily
    a correct choice:
  • 3:47 - 3:52
    some might say even a minuscule risk
    of shark attack is worth avoiding,
  • 3:52 - 3:54
    because all you’d miss out on
    is an ocean swim,
  • 3:54 - 3:57
    while others wouldn’t even consider
    skipping a swim
  • 3:57 - 4:01
    to avoid an objectively tiny risk
    of shark attack.
  • 4:01 - 4:05
    For all these reasons,
    risk evaluation is tricky at baseline,
  • 4:05 - 4:08
    and reporting on risk can be misleading,
  • 4:08 - 4:11
    especially when it shares some numbers
    in absolute terms
  • 4:11 - 4:14
    and others in relative terms.
  • 4:14 - 4:16
    Understanding how these measures work
  • 4:16 - 4:19
    will help you cut through
    some of the confusion
  • 4:19 - 4:21
    and better evaluate risk.
Title:
Why do people fear the wrong things? - Gerd Gigerenzer
Speaker:
Gerd Gigerenzer
Description:

View full lesson: https://ed.ted.com/lessons/how-likely-is-a-shark-attack-gerd-gigerenzer

A new drug reduces the risk of heart attacks by 40%. Shark attacks are up by a factor of two. Drinking a liter of soda per day doubles your chance of developing cancer. These are all examples of a common way risk is presented in news articles, and can often be misleading. So how can we better evaluate risk? Gerd Gigerenzer explores the difference between relative and absolute risk.

Lesson by Gerd Gigerenzer, directed by visorama.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TED-Ed
Duration:
04:21
lauren mcalpine edited English subtitles for Why do people fear the wrong things?
Elise Haadsma approved English subtitles for Why do people fear the wrong things?
Elise Haadsma accepted English subtitles for Why do people fear the wrong things?
lauren mcalpine edited English subtitles for Why do people fear the wrong things?

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions