< Return to Video

Dr Norman Doidge , The Brain That Changes Itself full show

  • 0:00 - 0:05
    For a long time, it was believed that the human brain was fixed or hardwired
  • 0:05 - 0:09
    and most forms of brain damage were therefore permanent and irreversible.
  • 0:09 - 0:13
    However, scientists have since come to realize that far from being fixed,
  • 0:14 - 0:18
    the brain has remarkable powers to regenerate itself even in old age.
  • 0:19 - 0:22
    Norman Doidge is a psychiatrist and medical researcher.
  • 0:22 - 0:27
    In his new book, he explores the impact of this revolutionary discovery on all of us.
  • 0:27 - 0:30
    It's called "The Brain That Changes Itself."
  • 0:30 - 0:34
    Stories of personal triumph from the frontiers of brain science.
  • 0:35 - 0:38
    For years, we've been told that the human brain is like a machine,
  • 0:38 - 0:40
    a computer that there are different parts
  • 0:40 - 0:42
    that they're responsible for different functions.
  • 0:42 - 0:48
    And if those parts break or wear down or damaged, we lose that function.
  • 0:48 - 0:51
    Now, in your new book, you throw this all on its head and you say that in fact,
  • 0:51 - 0:53
    the brain is plastic,
  • 0:53 - 0:55
    explain what that means.
  • 0:55 - 0:58
    Well, plastic means plastic in the sense of Plasticine,
  • 0:59 - 1:01
    modifiable, adaptable.
  • 1:01 - 1:03
    The plastic brain is a brain that can
  • 1:03 - 1:05
    change its structure and its function depending on
  • 1:06 - 1:06
    both...
  • 1:07 - 1:08
    Without drugs or invasive...
  • 1:09 - 1:11
    Without drugs. Just depending on what you do with your brain.
  • 1:12 - 1:15
    The task at hand that you are working on,
  • 1:15 - 1:19
    what you're perceiving can cause you to change the structure of your brain
  • 1:19 - 1:22
    on many levels
  • 1:22 - 1:24
    and this is the most fantastic part,
  • 1:24 - 1:27
    what you think and imagine actually can change the structure of your brain
  • 1:28 - 1:29
    down
  • 1:29 - 1:33
    to the very connections between the brain cells, down into the genes
  • 1:33 - 1:34
    even for instance.
  • 1:35 - 1:36
    One of the most,
  • 1:37 - 1:37
    you know,
  • 1:37 - 1:40
    staggeringly interesting discoveries of the late
  • 1:40 - 1:43
    20th century was the discovery that learning
  • 1:43 - 1:46
    changes the number of connections between the neurons,
  • 1:47 - 1:50
    the nerve cells in a nervous system.
  • 1:50 - 1:53
    You can go, for instance, from having perhaps 1300
  • 1:54 - 1:55
    connections between
  • 1:55 - 1:58
    nerve cell A and nerve cell B to 2700
  • 1:58 - 2:03
    with several hours of training. By virtue of more brain activity occurring,
  • 2:03 - 2:06
    By virtue of learning and brain activity. So,
  • 2:06 - 2:09
    what happens is thoughts
  • 2:09 - 2:11
    or activities that you do with your brain
  • 2:12 - 2:15
    actually turn certain genes on and others off inside the nerve
  • 2:15 - 2:19
    cells, which then make proteins which then change the structure.
  • 2:19 - 2:19
    Now,
  • 2:19 - 2:23
    why is this discovery considered so revolutionary and more particularly what
  • 2:23 - 2:27
    are the implications of this discovery for people with brain dysfunction?
  • 2:27 - 2:31
    A stroke for example. When you had a stroke, the general assumption was
  • 2:32 - 2:38
    your brain got swollen, the chemicals were kind of deranged and after a few weeks,
  • 2:38 - 2:40
    whatever you were left with was what you would have
  • 2:40 - 2:42
    to live with for the rest of your life.
  • 2:42 - 2:43
    You basically lost that brain
  • 2:43 - 2:47
    function that was damaged. The cells died, they couldn't be replaced,
  • 2:47 - 2:50
    the healthy ones around it couldn't reorganize.
  • 2:50 - 2:52
    And so,
  • 2:53 - 2:54
    we were kind of waiting for the swelling to
  • 2:54 - 2:56
    go away and rehab was just sort of focused on
  • 2:57 - 3:00
    getting you over that period. Now, we know that rehab can actually
  • 3:00 - 3:05
    reorganize the brain if it's done properly and bring many new functions back.
  • 3:05 - 3:07
    But if I could just say it, it also meant
  • 3:08 - 3:08
    that
  • 3:08 - 3:13
    as you started to get older and your brain started to decline.
  • 3:14 - 3:16
    It was like a machine that was wearing out
  • 3:16 - 3:19
    and the attempt to exercise your brain in the second half
  • 3:19 - 3:22
    of life was really kind of unwarranted or waste of time.
  • 3:23 - 3:24
    And so, we just
  • 3:25 - 3:28
    accepted a view of human development in which
  • 3:29 - 3:33
    the second half of life is a period of necessary mental decline.
  • 3:33 - 3:37
    And then, of course, the whole view of human nature that we have, you know,
  • 3:37 - 3:39
    since the rise of modern science, most
  • 3:39 - 3:44
    educated people see human nature in some way is emerging from the human brain.
  • 3:44 - 3:46
    And if the human brain was fixed, immutable
  • 3:47 - 3:48
    and a rigid structure,
  • 3:48 - 3:52
    it made a lot of sense to think of human nature as fundamentally fixed.
  • 3:52 - 3:53
    And so,
  • 3:53 - 3:57
    we have to re-examine all that and I begin to do that in this book.
  • 3:57 - 4:00
    Well, you do it by way of story and that there's some remarkable,
  • 4:01 - 4:03
    almost logic defying, remarkable,
  • 4:03 - 4:06
    given what our traditional notion of what the brain is,
  • 4:06 - 4:08
    probably the most remarkable.
  • 4:08 - 4:12
    And it is the story of a woman who was born with half a brain. Tell me about Michelle.
  • 4:13 - 4:15
    I went to visit Michelle because I thought that
  • 4:16 - 4:20
    I knew that she was born with one hemisphere alone,
  • 4:20 - 4:21
    just half a brain.
  • 4:21 - 4:22
    And,
  • 4:22 - 4:24
    I thought that whatever changes that she was able
  • 4:24 - 4:27
    to undergo would really test this notion of
  • 4:27 - 4:29
    plasticity, certainly, at least in early life.
  • 4:30 - 4:31
    And
  • 4:31 - 4:36
    basically, when she was in the womb, some catastrophic event occurred, so
  • 4:36 - 4:39
    her left hemisphere never developed.
  • 4:39 - 4:40
    And
  • 4:40 - 4:43
    based on what, you know, the usual view of
  • 4:44 - 4:44
    that
  • 4:44 - 4:47
    the brain basically has certain areas which in the left
  • 4:47 - 4:50
    hemisphere which are responsible for speech and other functions,
  • 4:51 - 4:54
    someone with that level of damage you would imagine
  • 4:55 - 5:00
    would be unable to speak, unable to think, they might be alive, but on a respirator.
  • 5:01 - 5:04
    And it turns out that that's not the case with Michelle.
  • 5:05 - 5:06
    In fact,
  • 5:07 - 5:08
    if you met Michelle,
  • 5:08 - 5:13
    you would detect some subtle things like one of her arms is a little twisted,
  • 5:13 - 5:14
    et cetera
  • 5:14 - 5:15
    and
  • 5:15 - 5:19
    her speech when she's upset, it's a little repetitive,
  • 5:19 - 5:21
    but you would never dream.
  • 5:21 - 5:25
    And nor did the people who examined her for a number of years dream
  • 5:25 - 5:27
    that she had half a brain. Well, explain to this
  • 5:27 - 5:30
    now, if we had thought that
  • 5:30 - 5:32
    this part of the brain is responsible for speech
  • 5:32 - 5:33
    and I don't have this part of the brain,
  • 5:33 - 5:35
    How is Michelle speaking?
  • 5:35 - 5:37
    We know that there's a lot of plasticity early in life.
  • 5:37 - 5:41
    But we also know and I make this very clear in the book,
  • 5:41 - 5:42
    there's plasticity from cradle to grave.
  • 5:43 - 5:46
    But you know, there are waves of increased plasticity
  • 5:47 - 5:49
    and it turns out that what the brain does is
  • 5:50 - 5:53
    it learns how to do what it's got to do at the time. So,
  • 5:54 - 5:56
    parts of the brain that might have been
  • 5:56 - 5:58
    devoted to other things will learn how to move.
  • 5:58 - 6:01
    If it has to see to survive, they'll be devoted to vision.
  • 6:01 - 6:02
    And so
  • 6:02 - 6:02
    all
  • 6:03 - 6:06
    these things can actually be organized and move around,
  • 6:07 - 6:11
    these higher mental functions. To give our viewers even more
  • 6:11 - 6:12
    kind of appreciation of the extent to which
  • 6:12 - 6:14
    that's possible.
  • 6:14 - 6:17
    Retell the story that's in the book that
  • 6:17 - 6:20
    you dub the woman who is perpetually falling.
  • 6:21 - 6:22
    Cheryl Schiltz
  • 6:22 - 6:23
    was a woman.
  • 6:23 - 6:25
    She had a hysterectomy and she was given
  • 6:25 - 6:28
    a medication because she developed an infection called gentamicin
  • 6:29 - 6:30
    and
  • 6:30 - 6:35
    it poisoned the vestibular apparatus, which is the balance apparatus in her brain.
  • 6:35 - 6:36
    And if you lose your sense of balance,
  • 6:37 - 6:40
    you will feel like you're perpetually falling. When I was with Cheryl at one point,
  • 6:40 - 6:43
    I asked her, "What's it like when you've finally fallen to the floor?
  • 6:44 - 6:45
    What do you feel at that point?"
  • 6:46 - 6:46
    And then she says,
  • 6:46 - 6:50
    "Sometimes, I just feel the floor opens up and I fall into a perpetual abyss."
  • 6:50 - 6:53
    So that was her life and she ended up on disability.
  • 6:54 - 6:55
    And one of the great
  • 6:55 - 6:56
    people
  • 6:56 - 6:58
    who were at the cutting edge,
  • 6:58 - 7:01
    I call these people "neuroplastician" to coin a phrase,
  • 7:01 - 7:02
    Paul
  • 7:02 - 7:02
    Bach-y-Rita
  • 7:03 - 7:05
    invented a mechanism
  • 7:05 - 7:08
    and I was there when she was using the mechanism for one
  • 7:08 - 7:12
    of the first times that basically was attached to an accelerometer.
  • 7:12 - 7:14
    An accelerometer is like a gyroscope.
  • 7:14 - 7:15
    And
  • 7:15 - 7:16
    she would...
  • 7:17 - 7:20
    The accelerometer would be in a hat on her head. Like a construction hat.
  • 7:20 - 7:22
    Like a construction hat.
  • 7:22 - 7:26
    And it would give signals that went into her tongue of all places
  • 7:27 - 7:30
    telling her where she was in space. I tried it on.
  • 7:30 - 7:33
    So when you lean forward and you feel like champagne bubbles,
  • 7:33 - 7:36
    they're really little electric shocks, tell you you're forward, you lean back,
  • 7:36 - 7:37
    it goes back
  • 7:37 - 7:39
    and she would put this hat on.
  • 7:40 - 7:40
    And
  • 7:41 - 7:42
    immediately
  • 7:42 - 7:47
    she would... Normally, she'd be holding herself like up on a table lest she fall
  • 7:48 - 7:50
    and immediately her whole body relaxed.
  • 7:51 - 7:53
    And this seemed like a miracle. Why? Because
  • 7:54 - 7:55
    the sensory
  • 7:55 - 7:57
    apparatus, the
  • 7:57 - 7:59
    sense of touch on the tongue goes to a
  • 7:59 - 8:02
    different part of the brain and the balance apparatus.
  • 8:02 - 8:02
    So
  • 8:02 - 8:05
    somehow or rather these signals coming in
  • 8:06 - 8:11
    were making new pathways or strengthening very dormant pathways between
  • 8:11 - 8:14
    her sense of touch and her sense of balance,
  • 8:15 - 8:18
    so that was the first miracle I saw that happening and she had tried it on.
  • 8:18 - 8:19
    But
  • 8:20 - 8:22
    then there was a second miracle which
  • 8:22 - 8:25
    is as she started to use the machine more,
  • 8:26 - 8:28
    she found she had a residual period
  • 8:29 - 8:29
    where
  • 8:30 - 8:31
    she would take the hat off.
  • 8:32 - 8:35
    And at first, the residual period lasted
  • 8:35 - 8:37
    30 seconds. Residual period being that she still had balance. (MULTIPLE VOICES).
  • 8:38 - 8:38
    Exactly.
  • 8:39 - 8:39
    That's
  • 8:39 - 8:39
    right.
  • 8:40 - 8:43
    It lasted for, you know, a few seconds and then
  • 8:43 - 8:44
    it lasted longer and longer.
  • 8:44 - 8:47
    And as she started to use the hat and take it home and use it,
  • 8:48 - 8:50
    she would be going for days. And
  • 8:51 - 8:54
    that was a true miracle because what was happening was
  • 8:54 - 8:55
    a miracle. I mean,
  • 8:55 - 8:56
    it's only a miracle
  • 8:56 - 9:00
    if you think that the brain is fixed, it's actually not a miracle.
  • 9:00 - 9:00
    because
  • 9:01 - 9:03
    she now no longer considers herself a
  • 9:03 - 9:06
    wobbler. Because the brain reordered itself and put
  • 9:06 - 9:08
    the balance in a different part of the brain that wasn't then.
  • 9:08 - 9:12
    Yeah. And, it developed new paths. Now, is this sensory substitution?
  • 9:12 - 9:12
    I mean, this is again,
  • 9:12 - 9:16
    another fascinating notion he introduces. This is what
  • 9:17 - 9:17
    Paul
  • 9:17 - 9:19
    Bach-y-Rita called sensory substitution. He did
  • 9:20 - 9:21
    this
  • 9:21 - 9:25
    as well for people who had been congenitally blind,
  • 9:25 - 9:26
    they've never seen anything.
  • 9:27 - 9:27
    And,
  • 9:27 - 9:32
    he rigged up a camera to a computer and then attached it originally just to the back
  • 9:32 - 9:34
    and there were vibrating pixels that were on the back.
  • 9:34 - 9:36
    And then he put it in to the tongue
  • 9:37 - 9:40
    and he found that he could train a human tongue
  • 9:41 - 9:43
    to function like a retina.
  • 9:43 - 9:45
    And they would be able to see Twiggy.
  • 9:45 - 9:47
    They would say, "Well, that's a picture of Twiggy."
  • 9:47 - 9:52
    That's a picture of a vase...you've just moved a vase in front of a telephone,
  • 9:52 - 9:54
    they could read certain things.
  • 9:54 - 9:58
    If you threw a ball at them, they would duck, they could see perspective.
  • 9:58 - 10:00
    I mean, so this is remarkable.
  • 10:00 - 10:00
    They could see through the tongue.
  • 10:01 - 10:04
    What was happening is the tongue is like a two-dimensional surface
  • 10:04 - 10:06
    just as the retina,
  • 10:06 - 10:07
    the retina is.
  • 10:08 - 10:08
    And yeah,
  • 10:09 - 10:10
    basically...
  • 10:10 - 10:11
    The images would be...
  • 10:11 - 10:12
    Images would be on the tongue.
  • 10:12 - 10:16
    And then, we've recently learned, actually, this was some work also done
  • 10:16 - 10:17
    in Canada,
  • 10:17 - 10:19
    Bach-y-Rita was from the states
  • 10:19 - 10:19
    that
  • 10:20 - 10:25
    the input coming into the tongue was processed in the back of the brain,
  • 10:25 - 10:28
    which we tend to call the visual cortex.
  • 10:28 - 10:30
    So it was all being rerouted
  • 10:30 - 10:30
    and
  • 10:31 - 10:31
    Paul
  • 10:31 - 10:34
    Bach-y-Rita has found different ways to do sensory substitution, which is
  • 10:35 - 10:39
    lay the groundwork for the notion of a retinal implant, but,
  • 10:39 - 10:41
    you know, helping other people who have
  • 10:41 - 10:43
    literally lost the ability to sense certain things.
  • 10:43 - 10:47
    We touched on the whole issue of stroke victims.
  • 10:47 - 10:50
    And that you can tell a great story here about a surgeon in
  • 10:50 - 10:54
    his mid 50s had a terrible debilitating stroke.
  • 10:55 - 10:57
    Tell me how he came back. Michael Bernstein,
  • 10:57 - 11:00
    yeah, out, he was playing tennis. He was a tennis...
  • 11:00 - 11:03
    He spent the morning doing surgery on the eyes,
  • 11:03 - 11:08
    so he needs a lot of fine motor skills. It's microscopic surgery.
  • 11:08 - 11:10
    It's in a small confined space.
  • 11:10 - 11:11
    Then, he was playing tennis,
  • 11:11 - 11:14
    then he developed a stroke and he couldn't move one side of his body.
  • 11:14 - 11:17
    He went for conventional rehabilitation,
  • 11:17 - 11:19
    they gave him whatever it was the 4 to 6 weeks.
  • 11:21 - 11:24
    And basically, they said you're on your own and he had very,
  • 11:24 - 11:29
    very little control of his arm at that point and very little control of his leg,
  • 11:29 - 11:30
    and then he tried a new
  • 11:30 - 11:31
    form of treatment. He
  • 11:32 - 11:33
    happened to be from Birmingham,
  • 11:33 - 11:37
    Alabama where a man named Edward Taub had
  • 11:37 - 11:40
    developed this new treatment called constraint-induced therapy.
  • 11:40 - 11:41
    And
  • 11:41 - 11:45
    one of the things that neuroplasticians have shown in hundreds of experiments
  • 11:45 - 11:47
    now is that it's use it or lose it brain.
  • 11:48 - 11:51
    If you don't use something,
  • 11:51 - 11:55
    it's that cortical real estate is taken over by something else.
  • 11:55 - 11:56
    Now,
  • 11:56 - 11:59
    what happens when a very common form
  • 11:59 - 12:01
    of stroke is you have a stroke in the left
  • 12:01 - 12:03
    hemisphere and you can't move your
  • 12:03 - 12:05
    right hand. You're paralyzed.
  • 12:05 - 12:07
    You can't move it well, et cetera.
  • 12:07 - 12:10
    And so people try to use their arms,
  • 12:10 - 12:11
    it doesn't work
  • 12:12 - 12:12
    and
  • 12:12 - 12:14
    so they stopped using them.
  • 12:14 - 12:14
    Now,
  • 12:15 - 12:17
    what Taub did ingeniously was,
  • 12:17 - 12:20
    he got people to put their good hands in slings.
  • 12:20 - 12:22
    Constrain them. He constrained them
  • 12:22 - 12:24
    and then if a person could just do this,
  • 12:25 - 12:27
    he would give them small amounts just to get
  • 12:27 - 12:29
    a little more control over that very incremental,
  • 12:29 - 12:31
    and he worked them very, very hard.
  • 12:31 - 12:35
    And they would have to do things like, you know, try to wash pots. And,
  • 12:35 - 12:38
    you know, people would come in, they couldn't dress themselves, they
  • 12:38 - 12:40
    couldn't eat, they couldn't put food on a fork.
  • 12:40 - 12:43
    They literally would have been dependent for the rest of their lives.
  • 12:43 - 12:45
    And Michael Bernstein was one of the first to go
  • 12:45 - 12:48
    through this and after two weeks of very intensive training,
  • 12:48 - 12:49
    and that's not a long time. No. No.
  • 12:50 - 12:50
    It
  • 12:50 - 12:50
    isn't,
  • 12:50 - 12:55
    He was able to function, get back to work and function as a surgeon.
  • 12:55 - 12:59
    And there are people who went to Taub clinic, there was one person
  • 12:59 - 13:00
    I spoke to had
  • 13:00 - 13:03
    a stroke almost 50 years before. He had been
  • 13:03 - 13:05
    a little boy playing baseball when he had a stroke.
  • 13:05 - 13:06
    They
  • 13:07 - 13:09
    could help him. All these years,
  • 13:09 - 13:10
    after the fact, I mean,
  • 13:10 - 13:12
    that's just so remarkable because that plasticity
  • 13:12 - 13:14
    and the ability to reorganize
  • 13:14 - 13:15
    is in the brain.
  • 13:15 - 13:18
    And it has even helped people who've had traumatic injuries,
  • 13:18 - 13:20
    traumatic brain injuries.
  • 13:20 - 13:22
    I think that's the kind of thing that would be very helpful
  • 13:23 - 13:23
    for
  • 13:24 - 13:27
    anyone who's got those kinds... Well, not anyone but many people with
  • 13:28 - 13:30
    traumatic brain injuries, for instance,
  • 13:30 - 13:32
    think of the soldiers getting back from Afghanistan.
  • 13:32 - 13:37
    Is this kind of therapy or are these kinds of treatments now finding their way
  • 13:37 - 13:38
    into medical practice?
  • 13:38 - 13:38
    Not quite.
  • 13:39 - 13:42
    No, because normally when someone has a stroke or any kind of brain damage,
  • 13:42 - 13:43
    I mean, we really do look on it very,
  • 13:43 - 13:47
    very fatalistically and if the evidence is what we should, they don't improve.
  • 13:47 - 13:48
    Yeah look.
  • 13:48 - 13:51
    We look on it fatalistically in part because studies that have been
  • 13:51 - 13:54
    done in the past and people with brain damage and strokes showed that
  • 13:55 - 13:56
    interventions didn't work.
  • 13:56 - 14:00
    But now that the neuroplasticians have laid out the laws of this new science,
  • 14:00 - 14:04
    there's reason to be hopeful for a number of kinds
  • 14:04 - 14:05
    of brain injuries.
  • 14:05 - 14:05
    I bet,
  • 14:05 - 14:08
    the notion of phantom pain again
  • 14:08 - 14:11
    documented that people who lose limbs that they continue to
  • 14:11 - 14:12
    to hurt.
  • 14:12 - 14:14
    You again claim that
  • 14:14 - 14:18
    neuroplasticity both explains this and that researchers
  • 14:18 - 14:21
    using the theories of neuroplasticity have been able to cure phantom pain.
  • 14:21 - 14:22
    Tell me about that.
  • 14:22 - 14:27
    Well, phantom pain was one of the great mysteries of medicine.
  • 14:27 - 14:30
    How is it possible to feel pain in a limb that isn't there?
  • 14:30 - 14:32
    And what a neurologist named
  • 14:32 - 14:34
    Ramachandran discovered was
  • 14:35 - 14:37
    that in patients who've lost their arms and then
  • 14:37 - 14:40
    get pain or in one case in scratch or itches,
  • 14:40 - 14:42
    What happens is
  • 14:42 - 14:43
    if you remove an arm,
  • 14:44 - 14:47
    there's cortical real estate if you will, that had been devoted to
  • 14:47 - 14:50
    moving that arm and feeling for that arm, it's now dormant.
  • 14:51 - 14:51
    In the brain.
  • 14:52 - 14:52
    In the brain.
  • 14:53 - 14:57
    So adjacent cortical real estate takes it over.
  • 14:57 - 14:58
    And
  • 14:58 - 15:01
    in the typical human body map,
  • 15:02 - 15:05
    an area that's very close to the arm is actually the face. And
  • 15:05 - 15:11
    we now know that phantom pain is often caused because the face maps in the brain
  • 15:12 - 15:16
    say, "Hey, there's more cortical real estate for us and they start to move in there
  • 15:16 - 15:17
    and
  • 15:17 - 15:18
    reorganize
  • 15:18 - 15:22
    the brain." There was one patient he had who had an unscratchable itch,
  • 15:22 - 15:24
    so that was a terrible affliction for him.
  • 15:24 - 15:25
    And
  • 15:25 - 15:28
    Ramachandran eventually figured that if you just scratched the man's cheek,
  • 15:29 - 15:30
    the itch went away. And
  • 15:31 - 15:34
    Ramachandran was able by the way to sort of trick the brain
  • 15:34 - 15:39
    into rewiring itself using this same neuroplasticity.
  • 15:39 - 15:41
    One of the reasons that people would
  • 15:42 - 15:46
    have these frozen pains is because once the input is stopped into the brain
  • 15:47 - 15:47
    from,
  • 15:48 - 15:49
    you know, from the arm,
  • 15:50 - 15:51
    it's as though
  • 15:51 - 15:54
    that's the picture that remains in the brain, there's no
  • 15:54 - 15:57
    new input to say. Now the arm is moving again.
  • 15:58 - 15:59
    Many people who had phantoms
  • 15:59 - 16:01
    felt their arms were frozen and in pain.
  • 16:02 - 16:06
    So, he set up a mirror device where you would look at what your real arm,
  • 16:06 - 16:08
    let's say this arm had been cut off.
  • 16:08 - 16:11
    You'd look at your real arm and then you look into a mirror right here.
  • 16:11 - 16:12
    The brain would be tricking you thinking that your arm was...
  • 16:13 - 16:16
    Think that your left arm was moving, that which, of course, didn't exist,
  • 16:16 - 16:17
    you'd kind of put your stump near it
  • 16:18 - 16:18
    and
  • 16:19 - 16:22
    that rewired the map so that the frozen phantom could move.
  • 16:23 - 16:23
    Now,
  • 16:23 - 16:25
    I understand that neuroplasticity also tells us something
  • 16:25 - 16:28
    about the nature of sexual attraction and love.
  • 16:28 - 16:29
    What is that?
  • 16:29 - 16:33
    There's tremendous variation, of course, in what people are turned on by,
  • 16:33 - 16:34
    let's say sexually, I mean,
  • 16:35 - 16:36
    you know, take
  • 16:37 - 16:39
    situations in other cultures
  • 16:39 - 16:42
    seem unnatural sometimes.
  • 16:42 - 16:46
    And we learned that people can develop attractions. If you think of
  • 16:46 - 16:50
    the whole idea that Chinese men 100 years ago in the aristocracy were
  • 16:50 - 16:53
    totally turned on by women who had their feet broken and bound up.
  • 16:54 - 16:54
    Okay.
  • 16:55 - 16:59
    We start to realize that sexual tastes can be acquired and we
  • 16:59 - 17:01
    we can acquire sexual types.
  • 17:02 - 17:05
    And we're seeing a lot of this happening now actually
  • 17:05 - 17:07
    with the internet and internet porn in a way.
  • 17:07 - 17:10
    You know, there are a lot of stories that started to
  • 17:11 - 17:11
    become
  • 17:12 - 17:14
    public in the nineties about men
  • 17:14 - 17:16
    who would get on the internet
  • 17:17 - 17:19
    and they'd sort of just be searching
  • 17:19 - 17:22
    around and then they fasten on to certain images
  • 17:22 - 17:24
    that would really turn them on, that really surprise them.
  • 17:24 - 17:27
    And then they would practice them over and over and over again
  • 17:28 - 17:32
    and they would have orgasms and when a person has an orgasm, they secrete a
  • 17:32 - 17:36
    brain chemical called dopamine, which actually reinforces the circuit
  • 17:36 - 17:37
    and rewards them.
  • 17:38 - 17:42
    So, dopamine is a neurotransmitter or a brain chemical
  • 17:42 - 17:45
    that's very involved in consolidating a new circuit.
  • 17:45 - 17:49
    And these men would start to develop new sexual attractions
  • 17:49 - 17:51
    that at times seemed bizarre to them,
  • 17:52 - 17:54
    sometimes they dipped into childhood things.
  • 17:54 - 17:54
    I mean,
  • 17:54 - 17:56
    one story I talked about in the book is
  • 17:56 - 17:58
    a man who developed an attraction to spanking sites.
  • 17:58 - 18:02
    And if you think about it, spanking would seem to have to do with early childhood.
  • 18:02 - 18:04
    But the other thing that was happening for these men and this
  • 18:04 - 18:07
    was reported over and over and I saw it in clinical practice
  • 18:07 - 18:08
    is
  • 18:08 - 18:11
    they said that for a mysterious reason, they were losing
  • 18:12 - 18:13
    interest in
  • 18:13 - 18:14
    their own
  • 18:14 - 18:18
    partners, even though objectively they found them to be attractive.
  • 18:18 - 18:20
    So, here was a case where
  • 18:20 - 18:24
    because of plasticity, people were inadvertently rewiring
  • 18:24 - 18:25
    their brains.
  • 18:25 - 18:31
    And plasticity isn't always a good thing. That it can lead to rigid behaviors (VOICE OVERLAP).
  • 18:31 - 18:36
    And in this case, it leads typically or very frequently to a kind of an addiction.
  • 18:36 - 18:39
    And if you think about it, addictions are about plastic change in the brain.
  • 18:39 - 18:42
    There are certain people who, if they have alcohol,
  • 18:43 - 18:45
    they have a chemical
  • 18:45 - 18:49
    sequence that's fired in their brain and a chemical called delta
  • 18:49 - 18:49
    FosB
  • 18:49 - 18:51
    is released
  • 18:51 - 18:53
    that changes their brain permanently.
  • 18:54 - 18:56
    That's why sometimes it does make sense for a person to say,
  • 18:57 - 19:00
    even though they haven't had a drink for years, I'm an alcoholic.
  • 19:00 - 19:02
    It's in plasticity terms,
  • 19:02 - 19:06
    my brain has been structurally altered by my interactions with alcohol.
  • 19:07 - 19:11
    Now, interesting you say that because they also point out that psychoanalysis
  • 19:12 - 19:13
    can be used
  • 19:13 - 19:16
    to open up the brain's pathways and reorder
  • 19:17 - 19:20
    the brain's function too. I think you call this
  • 19:20 - 19:22
    neuroplastic therapy.
  • 19:23 - 19:26
    Explain to me what that is and how it would work.
  • 19:26 - 19:30
    It turns out, you know, based on what I told you before about,
  • 19:30 - 19:33
    you know, thoughts altering genetic behavior
  • 19:33 - 19:37
    that thoughts in therapy are actually changing brain structure as well.
  • 19:37 - 19:39
    They change different brain structures.
  • 19:39 - 19:42
    But the major therapists that we know that are successful
  • 19:43 - 19:47
    and that includes psychoanalytic therapies, cognitive behavioral therapies,
  • 19:47 - 19:49
    it's something called interpersonal therapy.
  • 19:49 - 19:51
    They're all for slightly different conditions,
  • 19:51 - 19:52
    all
  • 19:52 - 19:56
    rewire the brain, all change the balance in the brain's department.
  • 19:56 - 19:57
    So,
  • 19:57 - 20:00
    they are as every bit as biological, if you were
  • 20:00 - 20:04
    in intervention as the use of drugs and the advantage of psychotherapy is if
  • 20:04 - 20:05
    you don't absolutely need drugs.
  • 20:06 - 20:08
    And I want to say, I mean, I use drugs sometimes
  • 20:08 - 20:10
    but the advantage of them is
  • 20:10 - 20:10
    they
  • 20:11 - 20:13
    don't have as many side effects
  • 20:13 - 20:15
    in general. And,
  • 20:15 - 20:16
    if you think about it, drugs
  • 20:17 - 20:21
    as they are today are very blunt instruments, you take a pill and it goes
  • 20:21 - 20:24
    and it bays every cell in your brain and in fact, in your body,
  • 20:24 - 20:26
    and that's why we get so many side effects.
  • 20:26 - 20:27
    And it would be
  • 20:28 - 20:31
    to a certain extent, if you're using thoughts for the intervention,
  • 20:31 - 20:33
    you're like more like a micro surgeon
  • 20:33 - 20:35
    going into the thought patterns that are probably...
  • 20:36 - 20:41
    Do you know enough about the brain to be that precise in terms of psychiatry
  • 20:41 - 20:43
    and psychoanalysis.
  • 20:43 - 20:45
    In other words, if I make an interpretation about the meaning of behavior, do
  • 20:45 - 20:46
    I know where in the brain
  • 20:46 - 20:47
    it's going?
  • 20:47 - 20:50
    Well if we're treating, let's say an anxiety problem, right?
  • 20:51 - 20:55
    We now know that if you use psychoanalytic psychotherapy
  • 20:55 - 20:56
    and
  • 20:56 - 20:58
    you take a person before the therapy
  • 20:58 - 20:59
    and
  • 20:59 - 21:00
    you
  • 21:00 - 21:03
    take the thing that's making them anxious and you
  • 21:03 - 21:05
    make them think about it or attend to it,
  • 21:05 - 21:09
    the part of the brain that's likely to light up and trigger these
  • 21:09 - 21:11
    sort of bolts of anxiety.
  • 21:11 - 21:15
    And after they go through the therapy, if the therapy works,
  • 21:15 - 21:18
    those parts of the brain aren't triggered. Now you
  • 21:18 - 21:22
    have an absolutely horrifying statistic here about our generation,
  • 21:22 - 21:22
    the baby boomers,
  • 21:22 - 21:28
    those between 40 and 60 that they have over a 50% chance of reaching the age of 85,
  • 21:28 - 21:32
    and that 85-year-olds have a 47% chance of having Alzheimer's.
  • 21:32 - 21:37
    We're going to have a population full of dementia. I mean, what can baby boomers do
  • 21:37 - 21:40
    to enhance their brain power and ward off that disease?
  • 21:40 - 21:41
    Okay.
  • 21:41 - 21:43
    So, there's two things that we worry about when
  • 21:43 - 21:45
    we think of mental decline as we get older.
  • 21:45 - 21:49
    One of them is a more benign thing and more common,
  • 21:49 - 21:51
    it's called age-related cognitive decline.
  • 21:51 - 21:54
    The senior moments that begin in our 50s that
  • 21:54 - 21:56
    frighten people
  • 21:56 - 22:00
    and then there are the serious dementias.
  • 22:01 - 22:03
    So, the good news is that we know for a fact
  • 22:03 - 22:08
    now that age-related cognitive decline is reversible through brain exercises.
  • 22:08 - 22:10
    And I met with a man Stanley
  • 22:10 - 22:11
    Karansky who was 90,
  • 22:12 - 22:14
    who started to have some difficulties
  • 22:14 - 22:16
    with senior moments. His handwriting was sloppy.
  • 22:16 - 22:19
    He was no longer alert, he wasn't socializing much.
  • 22:20 - 22:20
    And
  • 22:20 - 22:21
    in six weeks
  • 22:22 - 22:23
    and
  • 22:23 - 22:23
    roughly
  • 22:23 - 22:25
    an hour a day, he was able to reverse all of that. What was he doing?
  • 22:26 - 22:29
    He was doing a program called Posit Science that I described in the book.
  • 22:29 - 22:31
    Now, this is a company, an actual company.
  • 22:31 - 22:34
    Yeah. Tell me more about because you have been claiming like 80-year-olds to be like 50-year-olds.
  • 22:35 - 22:36
    Well, they've shown it.
  • 22:36 - 22:39
    Posit Science was set up by perhaps
  • 22:40 - 22:41
    the world's leading neuroplastician and
  • 22:42 - 22:44
    neuroplasticity researcher, Michael Merzenich.
  • 22:44 - 22:45
    And,
  • 22:45 - 22:49
    he's already a man of great accomplishment. He was one of
  • 22:49 - 22:52
    the inventors of the cochlear implant that allows the deaf to hear.
  • 22:52 - 22:53
    He's
  • 22:53 - 22:55
    helped kids with learning disabilities,
  • 22:57 - 22:59
    with reading disabilities,
  • 22:59 - 23:03
    moved from problems to basically normal or above normal levels.
  • 23:03 - 23:06
    And they set up a program that rebuilds
  • 23:07 - 23:08
    the auditory cortex,
  • 23:08 - 23:13
    the part of the brain that processes language from scratch in older people.
  • 23:13 - 23:18
    What happens is that we age. One of the reasons that we cannot remember things
  • 23:18 - 23:19
    is
  • 23:20 - 23:23
    our brain maps for sounds
  • 23:23 - 23:25
    are just kind of getting dulled.
  • 23:25 - 23:29
    They need to be tuned up at the very basic level of distinguishing sounds like "ba
  • 23:29 - 23:29
    and da
  • 23:30 - 23:30
    and pa."
  • 23:31 - 23:35
    The reason is we haven't really used them intensively
  • 23:35 - 23:39
    often. Once, we hit middle age, we are usually replaying, mastered skills.
  • 23:39 - 23:41
    But to maintain a brain in good shape,
  • 23:41 - 23:43
    you've got to work as hard as you worked when
  • 23:43 - 23:45
    you were learning a French vocabulary in high school,
  • 23:45 - 23:48
    maybe you didn't work at it, but as hard as you should have worked at.
  • 23:49 - 23:52
    And so people go for decades without
  • 23:52 - 23:54
    putting themselves for that intensive kind of training
  • 23:54 - 23:55
    and
  • 23:56 - 23:58
    the cortex just kind of gets
  • 23:58 - 24:01
    dull and you develop what are called fuzzy engrams.
  • 24:01 - 24:04
    You don't hear the sound of the person's name at the party crisply,
  • 24:05 - 24:09
    so they rebuild it from the ground up. How would our viewers get this program?
  • 24:09 - 24:12
    Posit Science.
  • 24:12 - 24:15
    Just go to put that in the internet and you'll come to the website for that.
  • 24:16 - 24:19
    What do you think the next big breakthrough is in brain research?
  • 24:19 - 24:21
    What's the next big thing we should be watching for?
  • 24:22 - 24:25
    It's the translation of the fundamental laws
  • 24:25 - 24:29
    of brain plasticity into applications for everything
  • 24:30 - 24:34
    in therapeutics, in all kinds of training, you know, that means sports,
  • 24:35 - 24:39
    the military education, anything you want to do and develop
  • 24:39 - 24:40
    Right
  • 24:40 - 24:41
    now
  • 24:41 - 24:42
    basically
  • 24:43 - 24:47
    many of these disciplines have an intuitive grasp of some of the laws of plasticity,
  • 24:48 - 24:51
    but the neuroplasticians can help sharpen and speed up learning.
  • 24:51 - 24:52
    So
  • 24:52 - 24:53
    that's where I imagine it would come.
  • 24:53 - 24:55
    I know that people will want to get
  • 24:55 - 24:58
    very highfalutin and try to facilitate neuroplastic change
  • 24:58 - 25:02
    with chemicals and some of that might be doable.
  • 25:02 - 25:04
    But that's also very problematic for the reason I
  • 25:04 - 25:06
    said that chemicals are still a very blunt instrument.
  • 25:07 - 25:08
    At a personal level,
  • 25:08 - 25:10
    I mean that this book is full of very,
  • 25:10 - 25:14
    not just amazing but inspirational and uplifting stories.
  • 25:14 - 25:14
    I mean,
  • 25:14 - 25:16
    over the course of doing the research,
  • 25:16 - 25:18
    did it kind of change your sense of what
  • 25:18 - 25:21
    human potential and perhaps even human nature is?
  • 25:21 - 25:22
    Yes, it did.
  • 25:22 - 25:24
    One of the most important insights I think I had
  • 25:24 - 25:27
    while doing the book was what I call the "plastic paradox,"
  • 25:27 - 25:28
    and that is this.
  • 25:28 - 25:29
    The plasticity,
  • 25:29 - 25:31
    the brain is always plastic,
  • 25:31 - 25:35
    but it can give rise to both flexible or rigid behaviors.
  • 25:35 - 25:39
    It can give rise to rigid behaviors because once those networks are established,
  • 25:40 - 25:41
    they
  • 25:41 - 25:43
    tend to out compete the other one in a
  • 25:43 - 25:45
    war of nerves that's going on inside our head.
  • 25:45 - 25:48
    You know, the human brain is a habit-forming thing.
  • 25:49 - 25:49
    So,
  • 25:50 - 25:53
    the way to understand plasticity is think of it as like a hill,
  • 25:54 - 25:58
    snow on a hill in winter, and we want to ski down that hill,
  • 25:58 - 26:01
    so we get to the top of it. Because the snow is plastic or pliable,
  • 26:01 - 26:03
    we can take many paths down that hill,
  • 26:03 - 26:04
    but it being a hill,
  • 26:04 - 26:09
    it has rocks and trees and we be inclined towards certain favorite paths.
  • 26:09 - 26:11
    As we
  • 26:11 - 26:13
    keep using those paths
  • 26:13 - 26:16
    again precisely because the snow is pliable and plastic,
  • 26:16 - 26:18
    we'll develop tracks and ruts.
  • 26:19 - 26:21
    And what we do in our lives
  • 26:21 - 26:25
    and what we tend to think that because we're stuck in a rut
  • 26:26 - 26:27
    and repeating something
  • 26:28 - 26:31
    that not only is the behavior rigid, but the underlying brain is rigid.
  • 26:31 - 26:33
    So when I'm working with patients,
  • 26:33 - 26:36
    I try not to get fooled by the plastic paradox and
  • 26:36 - 26:39
    they're often fooled by the plastic paradox and I would submit
  • 26:39 - 26:40
    that
  • 26:40 - 26:44
    all of humanity to a large degree has been fooled by the plastic paradox.
  • 26:44 - 26:46
    I think we've underestimated
  • 26:46 - 26:47
    how plastic our brains really are.
  • 26:48 - 26:50
    Doctor Norman Doidge, I want to thank you very much for joining me.
  • 26:50 - 26:53
    It's been fascinating and a real pleasure. Thank you.
Title:
Dr Norman Doidge , The Brain That Changes Itself full show
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
BYU Continuing Education
Project:
PSYCH-220 (BYUO)
Duration:
26:53

English subtitles

Revisions