< Return to Video

Why you don't like the sound of your own voice

  • 0:01 - 0:04
    If you ask evolutionary biologists
  • 0:04 - 0:07
    when did humans become humans,
  • 0:07 - 0:09
    some of them will say that,
  • 0:09 - 0:12
    well, at some point
    we started standing on our feet,
  • 0:12 - 0:16
    became biped and became
    the masters of our environment.
  • 0:17 - 0:22
    Others will say that because our brain
    started growing much bigger,
  • 0:22 - 0:25
    that we were able to have
    much more complex cognitive processes.
  • 0:27 - 0:31
    And others might argue
    that it's because we developed language
  • 0:31 - 0:33
    that allowed us to evolve as a species.
  • 0:35 - 0:39
    Interestingly, those three
    phenomena are all connected.
  • 0:40 - 0:42
    We are not sure how or in which order,
  • 0:42 - 0:44
    but they are all linked
  • 0:44 - 0:48
    with the change of shape of a little bone
    in the back of your neck
  • 0:49 - 0:52
    that changed the angle
    between our head and our body.
  • 0:53 - 0:56
    That means we were able to stand upright
  • 0:56 - 0:59
    but also for our brain
    to evolve in the back
  • 0:59 - 1:05
    and for our voice box to grow
    from seven centimeters for primates
  • 1:05 - 1:08
    to 11 and up to 17 centimetres for humans.
  • 1:09 - 1:11
    And this is called
    the descent of the larynx.
  • 1:12 - 1:14
    And the larynx is the site of your voice.
  • 1:16 - 1:20
    When baby humans are born today,
    their larynx is not descended yet.
  • 1:21 - 1:23
    That only happens
    at about three months old.
  • 1:25 - 1:27
    So, metaphorically, each of us here
  • 1:27 - 1:31
    has relived the evolution
    of our whole species.
  • 1:33 - 1:34
    And talking about babies,
  • 1:34 - 1:37
    when you were starting to develop
    in your mother's womb,
  • 1:38 - 1:42
    the first sensation that you had
    coming from the outside world,
  • 1:42 - 1:46
    at only three weeks old,
    when you were about the size of a shrimp,
  • 1:46 - 1:48
    were through the tactile sensation
  • 1:49 - 1:51
    coming from the vibrations
    of your mother's voice.
  • 1:52 - 1:57
    So, as we can see, the human voice
    is quite meaningful and important
  • 1:57 - 2:00
    at the level of the species,
  • 2:00 - 2:02
    at the level of the society --
  • 2:02 - 2:05
    this is how we communicate
    and create bonds,
  • 2:05 - 2:08
    and at the personal
    and interpersonal levels --
  • 2:08 - 2:12
    with our voice, we share much more
    than words and data,
  • 2:12 - 2:14
    we share basically who we are.
  • 2:14 - 2:18
    And our voice is indistinguishable
    from how other people see us.
  • 2:18 - 2:20
    It is a mask that we wear in society.
  • 2:22 - 2:25
    But our relationship with our own voice
    is far from obvious.
  • 2:26 - 2:31
    We rarely use our voice for ourselves;
    we use it as a gift to give to others.
  • 2:31 - 2:34
    It is how we touch each other.
  • 2:34 - 2:36
    It's a dialectical grooming.
  • 2:36 - 2:38
    But what do we think about our own voice?
  • 2:38 - 2:40
    So please raise your hand
  • 2:40 - 2:44
    if you don't like the sound of your voice
    when you hear it on a recording machine.
  • 2:44 - 2:45
    (Laughter)
  • 2:45 - 2:46
    Yeah, thank you, indeed,
  • 2:47 - 2:50
    most people report not liking
    the sound of their voice recording.
  • 2:50 - 2:52
    So what does that mean?
  • 2:52 - 2:54
    Let's try to understand that
    in the next 10 minutes.
  • 2:54 - 2:57
    I'm a researcher at the MIT Media Lab,
  • 2:58 - 2:59
    part of the Opera of the Future group,
  • 3:00 - 3:03
    and my research
    focuses on the relationship
  • 3:03 - 3:06
    people have with their own voice
    and with the voices of others.
  • 3:08 - 3:12
    I study what we can learn
    from listening to voices,
  • 3:12 - 3:13
    from the various fields,
  • 3:13 - 3:17
    from neurology to biology,
    cognitive sciences, linguistics.
  • 3:19 - 3:22
    In our group we create
    tools and experiences
  • 3:22 - 3:26
    to help people gain a better
    applied understanding of their voice
  • 3:27 - 3:30
    in order to reduce the biases,
  • 3:30 - 3:32
    to become better listeners,
  • 3:32 - 3:35
    to create more healthy relationships
  • 3:35 - 3:37
    or just to understand themselves better.
  • 3:38 - 3:43
    And this really has to come
    with a holistic approach on the voice.
  • 3:44 - 3:47
    Because, think about all
    the applications and implications
  • 3:47 - 3:50
    that the voice may have,
    as we discover more about it.
  • 3:51 - 3:54
    Your voice is a very complex phenomenon.
  • 3:54 - 3:58
    It requires a synchronization
    of more than 100 muscles in your body.
  • 3:58 - 4:01
    And by listening to the voice,
  • 4:01 - 4:06
    we can understand possible failures
    of what happens inside.
  • 4:06 - 4:07
    For example:
  • 4:08 - 4:11
    listening to very specific
    types of turbulences
  • 4:11 - 4:14
    and nonlinearity of the voice
  • 4:14 - 4:17
    can help predict
    very early stages of Parkinson's,
  • 4:17 - 4:19
    just through a phone call.
  • 4:20 - 4:24
    Listening to the breathness of the voice
    can help detect heart disease.
  • 4:26 - 4:30
    And we also know that the changes of tempo
    inside individual words
  • 4:31 - 4:33
    is a very good marker of depression.
  • 4:34 - 4:37
    Your voice is also very linked
    with your hormone levels.
  • 4:37 - 4:40
    Third parties listening to female voices
  • 4:40 - 4:43
    were able to very accurately
    place the speaker
  • 4:43 - 4:44
    on their menstrual cycle.
  • 4:46 - 4:47
    Just with acoustic information.
  • 4:49 - 4:52
    And now with technology
    listening to us all the time,
  • 4:52 - 4:55
    Alexa from Amazon Echo
  • 4:55 - 4:58
    might be able to predict
    if you're pregnant
  • 4:58 - 5:00
    even before you know it.
  • 5:00 - 5:01
    So think about --
  • 5:01 - 5:02
    (Laughter)
  • 5:02 - 5:04
    Think about the ethical
    application of that.
  • 5:06 - 5:09
    Your voice is also very linked
    to how you create relationships.
  • 5:09 - 5:12
    You have a different voice
    for every person you talk to.
  • 5:12 - 5:16
    If I take a little snippet
    of your voice and I analyze it,
  • 5:16 - 5:19
    I can know whether you're talking
    to your mother, to your brother,
  • 5:19 - 5:21
    your friend or your boss.
  • 5:21 - 5:26
    We can also use, as a predictor,
    the vocal posture.
  • 5:26 - 5:30
    Meaning, how you decide to place
    your voice when you talk to someone.
  • 5:30 - 5:33
    And you vocal posture,
    when you talk to your spouse,
  • 5:33 - 5:37
    can help predict not only if,
    but also when you will divorce.
  • 5:39 - 5:41
    So there is a lot to learn
    from listening to voices.
  • 5:42 - 5:44
    And I believe this has to start
    with understanding
  • 5:44 - 5:46
    that we have more than one voice.
  • 5:47 - 5:50
    So, I'm going to talk
    about three voices that most of us posses,
  • 5:50 - 5:53
    in a model of what I call the mask.
  • 5:54 - 5:55
    So when you look at the mask,
  • 5:56 - 5:58
    what you see is a projection
    of a character.
  • 5:58 - 6:00
    Let's call that your outward voice.
  • 6:01 - 6:03
    This is also the most classic way
    to think about the voice,
  • 6:03 - 6:06
    it's a way of projecting
    yourself in the world.
  • 6:06 - 6:10
    The mechanism for this projection
    is well understood.
  • 6:10 - 6:12
    Your lungs contract your diaphragm
  • 6:12 - 6:15
    and that creates a self-sustained
    vibration of your vocal fold,
  • 6:15 - 6:17
    that creates a sound.
  • 6:17 - 6:20
    And then the way you open and close
    the cavities in you mouth,
  • 6:20 - 6:23
    your vocal tract is going to
    transform the sound.
  • 6:23 - 6:25
    So everyone has the same mechanism.
  • 6:25 - 6:27
    But voices are quite unique.
  • 6:27 - 6:32
    It's because very subtle differences
    in size, physiology, in hormone levels
  • 6:32 - 6:36
    are going to make very subtle
    differences in your outward voice.
  • 6:37 - 6:39
    And your brain is very good
  • 6:39 - 6:43
    at picking up those subtle differences
    from other people's outward voices.
  • 6:44 - 6:47
    In our lab, we are working
    on teaching machines
  • 6:47 - 6:49
    to understand those subtle differences.
  • 6:49 - 6:55
    And we use deep learning to create
    a real-time speaker identification system
  • 6:55 - 7:00
    to help raise awareness
    on the use of the shared vocal space --
  • 7:00 - 7:03
    so who talks and who never
    talks during meetings --
  • 7:03 - 7:05
    to increase group intelligence.
  • 7:06 - 7:10
    And one of the difficulties with that
    is that your voice is also not static.
  • 7:11 - 7:14
    We already said that it changes
    with every person you talk to
  • 7:14 - 7:17
    but it also changes generally
    throughout your life.
  • 7:17 - 7:19
    At the beginning
    and at the end of the journey,
  • 7:19 - 7:22
    male and female voices are very similar.
  • 7:22 - 7:24
    It's very hard to distinguish
  • 7:24 - 7:27
    the voice of a very your girl
    from the voice of a very young boy.
  • 7:28 - 7:33
    But in between, your voice
    becomes a marker of your fluid identity.
  • 7:33 - 7:37
    Generally, for male voices
    there's a big change at puberty.
  • 7:37 - 7:39
    And then for female voices,
  • 7:39 - 7:42
    there is a change at each pregnancy
    and a big change at menopause.
  • 7:43 - 7:47
    So all of that is the voice
    other people hear when you talk.
  • 7:47 - 7:51
    So why is it that we're so
    unfamiliar with it?
  • 7:52 - 7:55
    Why is it that it's not
    the voice that we hear?
  • 7:55 - 7:56
    So, let's think about it.
  • 7:56 - 8:00
    When you wear a mask,
    you actually don't see the mask.
  • 8:01 - 8:05
    And when you try to observe it,
    what you will see is inside of the mask.
  • 8:05 - 8:07
    And that's your inward voice.
  • 8:09 - 8:11
    So to understand why it's different,
  • 8:11 - 8:14
    let's try to understand the mechanism
    of perception of this inward voice.
  • 8:16 - 8:18
    Because your body has many ways
    of filtering it differently
  • 8:19 - 8:21
    from the outward voice.
  • 8:21 - 8:24
    So to perceive this voice,
    it first has to travel to your ears.
  • 8:24 - 8:27
    And your outward voice
    travels through the air
  • 8:27 - 8:30
    while your inward voice
    travels through your bones.
  • 8:30 - 8:32
    This is called bone conduction.
  • 8:33 - 8:38
    Because of this, your inward voice
    is going to sound in a lower register
  • 8:38 - 8:42
    and also more musically harmonical
    than your outward voice.
  • 8:43 - 8:47
    Once it travels there,
    it has to access your inner ear.
  • 8:47 - 8:50
    And there's this other mechanism
    taking place here.
  • 8:50 - 8:52
    It's a mechanical filter,
  • 8:52 - 8:55
    it's a little partition
    that comes and protects your inner ear
  • 8:55 - 8:58
    each time you produce a sound.
  • 8:58 - 9:00
    So it also reduces what you hear.
  • 9:01 - 9:04
    And then there is a third filter,
    it's a biological filter.
  • 9:05 - 9:09
    Your cochlea -- it's a part of your
    inner ear that processes the sound --
  • 9:09 - 9:11
    is made out of living cells.
  • 9:11 - 9:14
    And those living cells
    are going to trigger differently
  • 9:14 - 9:17
    according to how often
    they hear the sound.
  • 9:17 - 9:18
    It's a habituation effect.
  • 9:19 - 9:21
    So because of this,
  • 9:21 - 9:24
    as your voice is the sound
    you hear the most in your life,
  • 9:24 - 9:27
    you actually hear it less
    than other sounds.
  • 9:27 - 9:29
    Finally, we have a fourth filter.
  • 9:29 - 9:31
    It's a neurological filter.
  • 9:32 - 9:34
    Neurologists found out recently
  • 9:34 - 9:37
    that when you open your mouth
    to create a sound,
  • 9:37 - 9:40
    your own auditory cortex shuts down.
  • 9:42 - 9:45
    So you hear your voice
  • 9:45 - 9:50
    but your brain actually never listens
    to the sound of your voice.
  • 9:52 - 9:55
    Well, evolutionarily
    that might make sense,
  • 9:55 - 9:58
    because we know cognitively
    what we are going to sound like
  • 9:58 - 10:01
    so maybe we don't need
    to spend energy analyzing the signal.
  • 10:02 - 10:05
    And this is called a corollary discharge
  • 10:05 - 10:08
    and it happens for every motion
    that your body does.
  • 10:08 - 10:10
    The exact definition
    of a corollary discharge
  • 10:10 - 10:15
    is a copy of a motor command
    that is sent by the brain.
  • 10:15 - 10:18
    This copy doesn't create any motion itself
  • 10:18 - 10:22
    but instead is sent
    to other regions of the brain
  • 10:22 - 10:24
    to inform them of the impending motion.
  • 10:26 - 10:30
    And for the voice, this corollary
    discharge also has a different name.
  • 10:30 - 10:33
    It is your inner voice.
  • 10:33 - 10:34
    So let's recapitulate.
  • 10:34 - 10:36
    We have the mask, the outward voice,
  • 10:37 - 10:40
    the inside of the mask, your inward voice,
  • 10:40 - 10:42
    and then you have your inner voice.
  • 10:42 - 10:44
    And I like to see this one
    as the puppeteer
  • 10:44 - 10:46
    that holds the strings
    of the whole system.
  • 10:47 - 10:49
    Your inner voice is
  • 10:49 - 10:52
    the one you hear
    when you read a text silently,
  • 10:53 - 10:56
    when you rehearse
    for an important conversation.
  • 10:57 - 10:58
    Sometimes is hard to turn it off,
  • 10:58 - 11:02
    it's really hard to look at the text
    written in your native language,
  • 11:02 - 11:05
    without having this inner voice read it.
  • 11:06 - 11:08
    It's also the voice
    that refuse to stop singing
  • 11:08 - 11:10
    the stupid song you have in your head.
  • 11:10 - 11:11
    (Laughter)
  • 11:13 - 11:17
    And for some people
    it's actually impossible to control it.
  • 11:17 - 11:20
    And that's the case
    of schizophrenic patients,
  • 11:20 - 11:22
    who have auditory hallucinations.
  • 11:22 - 11:25
    Who can't distinguish at all
    between voices coming from inside
  • 11:25 - 11:27
    and outside their head.
  • 11:27 - 11:30
    So in our lab, we are also
    working on small devices
  • 11:30 - 11:33
    to help those people
    make those distinctions
  • 11:33 - 11:35
    and know if a voice
    is internal or external.
  • 11:37 - 11:41
    You can also think about the inner voice
    as the voice that speaks in your dream.
  • 11:41 - 11:43
    This inner voice can take many forms.
  • 11:43 - 11:47
    And in your dreams, you actually unleash
    the potential of this inner voice.
  • 11:47 - 11:50
    That's another work
    we are doing in our lab:
  • 11:50 - 11:53
    trying to access
    this inner voice in dreams.
  • 11:54 - 11:57
    So even if you can't always control it,
  • 11:57 - 11:59
    the inner voice --
    you can always engage with it
  • 11:59 - 12:01
    through dialogue, through inner dialogues.
  • 12:02 - 12:03
    And you can even see this inner voice
  • 12:03 - 12:06
    as the missing link
    between thought and actions.
  • 12:09 - 12:12
    So I hope I've left you
    with a better appreciation,
  • 12:12 - 12:15
    a new appreciation of all of your voices
  • 12:15 - 12:18
    and the role it plays
    inside and outside of you --
  • 12:18 - 12:22
    as your voice is a very critical
    determinant of what makes you humans
  • 12:22 - 12:25
    and of how you interact with the world.
  • 12:25 - 12:26
    Thank you.
  • 12:26 - 12:29
    (Applause)
Title:
Why you don't like the sound of your own voice
Speaker:
Rébecca Kleinberger
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
12:42

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions