< Return to Video

The intriguing sound of marine mammals

  • 0:00 - 0:03
    Thank you so much. I'm going to try to take you
  • 0:03 - 0:06
    on a journey of the underwater acoustic world
  • 0:06 - 0:08
    of whales and dolphins.
  • 0:08 - 0:10
    Since we are such a visual species,
  • 0:10 - 0:12
    it's hard for us to really understand this,
  • 0:12 - 0:14
    so I'll use a mixture of figures and sounds
  • 0:14 - 0:16
    and hope this can communicate it.
  • 0:16 - 0:19
    But let's also think, as a visual species,
  • 0:19 - 0:21
    what it's like when we go snorkeling or diving
  • 0:21 - 0:23
    and try to look underwater.
  • 0:23 - 0:25
    We really can't see very far.
  • 0:25 - 0:27
    Our vision, which works so well in air,
  • 0:27 - 0:30
    all of a sudden is very restricted and claustrophobic.
  • 0:30 - 0:32
    And what marine mammals have evolved
  • 0:32 - 0:35
    over the last tens of millions of years
  • 0:35 - 0:37
    is ways to depend on sound
  • 0:37 - 0:39
    to both explore their world
  • 0:39 - 0:41
    and also to stay in touch with one another.
  • 0:41 - 0:43
    Dolphins and toothed whales use echolocation.
  • 0:43 - 0:45
    They can produce loud clicks
  • 0:45 - 0:48
    and listen for echoes from the sea floor in order to orient.
  • 0:48 - 0:50
    They can listen for echoes from prey
  • 0:50 - 0:52
    in order to decide where food is
  • 0:52 - 0:55
    and to decide which one they want to eat.
  • 0:55 - 0:57
    All marine mammals use sound for communication to stay in touch.
  • 0:57 - 0:59
    So the large baleen whales
  • 0:59 - 1:02
    will produce long, beautiful songs,
  • 1:02 - 1:04
    which are used in reproductive advertisement
  • 1:04 - 1:06
    for male and females, both to find one another
  • 1:06 - 1:08
    and to select a mate.
  • 1:08 - 1:10
    And mother and young and closely bonded animals
  • 1:10 - 1:13
    use calls to stay in touch with one another,
  • 1:13 - 1:15
    so sound is really critical for their lives.
  • 1:15 - 1:17
    The first thing that got me interested in the sounds
  • 1:17 - 1:19
    of these underwater animals,
  • 1:19 - 1:21
    whose world was so foreign to me,
  • 1:21 - 1:23
    was evidence from captive dolphins
  • 1:23 - 1:26
    that captive dolphins could imitate human sounds.
  • 1:26 - 1:28
    And I mentioned I'll use
  • 1:28 - 1:30
    some visual representations of sounds.
  • 1:30 - 1:32
    Here's the first example.
  • 1:32 - 1:34
    This is a plot of frequency against time --
  • 1:34 - 1:36
    sort of like musical notation,
  • 1:36 - 1:39
    where the higher notes are up higher and the lower notes are lower,
  • 1:39 - 1:41
    and time goes this way.
  • 1:41 - 1:43
    This is a picture of a trainer's whistle,
  • 1:43 - 1:45
    a whistle a trainer will blow to tell a dolphin
  • 1:45 - 1:47
    it's done the right thing and can come get a fish.
  • 1:47 - 1:50
    It sounds sort of like "tweeeeeet." Like that.
  • 1:50 - 1:52
    This is a calf in captivity
  • 1:52 - 1:54
    making an imitation
  • 1:54 - 1:56
    of that trainer's whistle.
  • 1:56 - 1:58
    If you hummed this tune to your dog or cat
  • 1:58 - 2:00
    and it hummed it back to you,
  • 2:00 - 2:02
    you ought to be pretty surprised.
  • 2:02 - 2:04
    Very few nonhuman mammals
  • 2:04 - 2:06
    can imitate sounds.
  • 2:06 - 2:08
    It's really important for our music and our language.
  • 2:08 - 2:11
    So it's a puzzle: The few other mammal groups that do this,
  • 2:11 - 2:13
    why do they do it?
  • 2:13 - 2:15
    A lot of my career has been devoted
  • 2:15 - 2:17
    to trying to understand
  • 2:17 - 2:19
    how these animals use their learning,
  • 2:19 - 2:21
    use the ability to change what you say
  • 2:21 - 2:23
    based on what you hear
  • 2:23 - 2:25
    in their own communication systems.
  • 2:25 - 2:28
    So let's start with calls of a nonhuman primate.
  • 2:28 - 2:30
    Many mammals have to produce contact calls
  • 2:30 - 2:33
    when, say, a mother and calf are apart.
  • 2:33 - 2:36
    This is an example of a call produced by squirrel monkeys
  • 2:36 - 2:38
    when they're isolated from another one.
  • 2:38 - 2:40
    And you can see, there's not much
  • 2:40 - 2:42
    variability in these calls.
  • 2:42 - 2:44
    By contrast, the signature whistle
  • 2:44 - 2:46
    which dolphins use to stay in touch,
  • 2:46 - 2:49
    each individual here has a radically different call.
  • 2:49 - 2:52
    They can use this ability to learn calls
  • 2:52 - 2:55
    in order to develop more complicated and more distinctive calls
  • 2:55 - 2:57
    to identify individuals.
  • 2:58 - 3:01
    How about the setting in which animals need to use this call?
  • 3:01 - 3:03
    Well let's look at mothers and calves.
  • 3:03 - 3:05
    In normal life for mother and calf dolphin,
  • 3:05 - 3:08
    they'll often drift apart or swim apart if Mom is chasing a fish,
  • 3:08 - 3:10
    and when they separate
  • 3:10 - 3:12
    they have to get back together again.
  • 3:12 - 3:15
    What this figure shows is the percentage of the separations
  • 3:15 - 3:17
    in which dolphins whistle,
  • 3:17 - 3:19
    against the maximum distance.
  • 3:19 - 3:21
    So when dolphins are separating by less than 20 meters,
  • 3:21 - 3:23
    less than half the time they need to use whistles.
  • 3:23 - 3:25
    Most of the time they can just find each other
  • 3:25 - 3:27
    just by swimming around.
  • 3:27 - 3:30
    But all of the time when they separate by more than 100 meters,
  • 3:30 - 3:33
    they need to use these individually distinctive whistles
  • 3:33 - 3:36
    to come back together again.
  • 3:36 - 3:38
    Most of these distinctive signature whistles
  • 3:38 - 3:40
    are quite stereotyped and stable
  • 3:40 - 3:42
    through the life of a dolphin.
  • 3:42 - 3:44
    But there are some exceptions.
  • 3:44 - 3:46
    When a male dolphin leaves Mom,
  • 3:46 - 3:48
    it will often join up with another male
  • 3:48 - 3:51
    and form an alliance, which may last for decades.
  • 3:51 - 3:54
    As these two animals form a social bond,
  • 3:54 - 3:56
    their distinctive whistles actually converge
  • 3:56 - 3:58
    and become very similar.
  • 3:58 - 4:01
    This plot shows two members of a pair.
  • 4:01 - 4:03
    As you can see at the top here,
  • 4:03 - 4:05
    they share an up-sweep, like "woop, woop, woop."
  • 4:05 - 4:07
    They both have that kind of up-sweep.
  • 4:07 - 4:10
    Whereas these members of a pair go "wo-ot, wo-ot, wo-ot."
  • 4:10 - 4:12
    And what's happened is
  • 4:12 - 4:14
    they've used this learning process
  • 4:14 - 4:17
    to develop a new sign that identifies this new social group.
  • 4:17 - 4:19
    It's a very interesting way that they can
  • 4:19 - 4:21
    form a new identifier
  • 4:21 - 4:23
    for the new social group that they've had.
  • 4:23 - 4:25
    Let's now take a step back
  • 4:25 - 4:27
    and see what this message can tell us
  • 4:27 - 4:29
    about protecting dolphins
  • 4:29 - 4:31
    from human disturbance.
  • 4:31 - 4:33
    Anybody looking at this picture
  • 4:33 - 4:35
    will know this dolphin is surrounded,
  • 4:35 - 4:38
    and clearly his behavior is being disrupted.
  • 4:38 - 4:40
    This is a bad situation.
  • 4:40 - 4:42
    But it turns out that when just a single boat
  • 4:42 - 4:44
    is approaching a group of dolphins
  • 4:44 - 4:46
    at a couple hundred meters away,
  • 4:46 - 4:48
    the dolphins will start whistling,
  • 4:48 - 4:50
    they'll change what they're doing, they'll have a more cohesive group,
  • 4:50 - 4:52
    wait for the boat to go by,
  • 4:52 - 4:54
    and then they'll get back to normal business.
  • 4:54 - 4:56
    Well, in a place like Sarasota, Florida,
  • 4:56 - 4:58
    the average interval between times
  • 4:58 - 5:01
    that a boat is passing within a hundred meters of a dolphin group
  • 5:01 - 5:03
    is six minutes.
  • 5:03 - 5:06
    So even in the situation that doesn't look as bad as this,
  • 5:06 - 5:08
    it's still affecting the amount of time these animals have
  • 5:08 - 5:10
    to do their normal work.
  • 5:10 - 5:13
    And if we look at a very pristine environment like western Australia,
  • 5:13 - 5:15
    Lars Bider has done work
  • 5:15 - 5:18
    comparing dolphin behavior and distribution
  • 5:18 - 5:21
    before there were dolphin-watching boats.
  • 5:21 - 5:24
    When there was one boat, not much of an impact.
  • 5:24 - 5:27
    And two boats: When the second boat was added,
  • 5:27 - 5:29
    what happened was that some of the dolphins
  • 5:29 - 5:31
    left the area completely.
  • 5:31 - 5:34
    Of the ones that stayed, their reproductive rate declined.
  • 5:34 - 5:37
    So it could have a negative impact on the whole population.
  • 5:37 - 5:40
    When we think of marine-protected areas for animals like dolphins,
  • 5:40 - 5:42
    this means that we have to be
  • 5:42 - 5:45
    quite conscious about activities that we thought were benign.
  • 5:45 - 5:47
    We may need to regulate the intensity
  • 5:47 - 5:50
    of recreational boating and actual whale watching
  • 5:50 - 5:53
    in order to prevent these kinds of problems.
  • 5:53 - 5:55
    I'd also like to point out that sound
  • 5:55 - 5:57
    doesn't obey boundaries.
  • 5:57 - 6:00
    So you can draw a line to try to protect an area,
  • 6:00 - 6:02
    but chemical pollution and noise pollution
  • 6:02 - 6:04
    will continue to move through the area.
  • 6:04 - 6:06
    And I'd like to switch now from this local,
  • 6:06 - 6:09
    familiar, coastal environment
  • 6:09 - 6:12
    to a much broader world of the baleen whales and the open ocean.
  • 6:12 - 6:15
    This is a kind of map we've all been looking at.
  • 6:15 - 6:17
    The world is mostly blue.
  • 6:17 - 6:19
    But I'd also like to point out that the oceans
  • 6:19 - 6:21
    are much more connected than we think.
  • 6:21 - 6:24
    Notice how few barriers there are to movement
  • 6:24 - 6:26
    across all of the oceans compared to land.
  • 6:26 - 6:28
    To me, the most mind-bending example
  • 6:28 - 6:30
    of the interconnectedness of the ocean
  • 6:30 - 6:32
    comes from an acoustic experiment
  • 6:32 - 6:34
    where oceanographers
  • 6:34 - 6:37
    took a ship to the southern Indian Ocean,
  • 6:37 - 6:39
    deployed an underwater loudspeaker
  • 6:39 - 6:41
    and played back a sound.
  • 6:41 - 6:43
    That same sound
  • 6:43 - 6:46
    traveled to the west, and could be heard in Bermuda,
  • 6:46 - 6:49
    and traveled to the east, and could be heard in Monterey --
  • 6:49 - 6:51
    the same sound.
  • 6:51 - 6:53
    So we live in a world of satellite communication,
  • 6:53 - 6:55
    are used to global communication,
  • 6:55 - 6:57
    but it's still amazing to me.
  • 6:57 - 6:59
    The ocean has properties
  • 6:59 - 7:01
    that allow low-frequency sound
  • 7:01 - 7:03
    to basically move globally.
  • 7:03 - 7:06
    The acoustic transit time for each of these paths is about three hours.
  • 7:06 - 7:09
    It's nearly halfway around the globe.
  • 7:09 - 7:11
    In the early '70s,
  • 7:11 - 7:13
    Roger Payne and an ocean acoustician
  • 7:13 - 7:15
    published a theoretical paper
  • 7:15 - 7:17
    pointing out that it was possible
  • 7:17 - 7:20
    that sound could transmit over these large areas,
  • 7:20 - 7:23
    but very few biologists believed it.
  • 7:23 - 7:25
    It actually turns out, though,
  • 7:25 - 7:28
    even though we've only known of long-range propagation for a few decades,
  • 7:28 - 7:31
    the whales clearly have evolved,
  • 7:31 - 7:33
    over tens of millions of years,
  • 7:33 - 7:36
    a way to exploit this amazing property of the ocean.
  • 7:36 - 7:38
    So blue whales and fin whales
  • 7:38 - 7:40
    produce very low-frequency sounds
  • 7:40 - 7:42
    that can travel over very long ranges.
  • 7:42 - 7:44
    The top plot here shows
  • 7:44 - 7:46
    a complicated series of calls
  • 7:46 - 7:48
    that are repeated by males.
  • 7:48 - 7:51
    They form songs, and they appear to play a role in reproduction,
  • 7:51 - 7:53
    sort of like that of song birds.
  • 7:53 - 7:56
    Down below here, we see calls made by both males and females
  • 7:56 - 7:59
    that also carry over very long ranges.
  • 8:00 - 8:02
    The biologists continued to be skeptical
  • 8:02 - 8:04
    of the long-range communication issue
  • 8:04 - 8:06
    well past the '70s,
  • 8:06 - 8:08
    until the end of the Cold War.
  • 8:08 - 8:10
    What happened was, during the Cold War,
  • 8:10 - 8:13
    the U.S. Navy had a system that was secret at the time,
  • 8:13 - 8:16
    that they used to track Russian submarines.
  • 8:16 - 8:18
    It had deep underwater microphones, or hydrophones,
  • 8:18 - 8:20
    cabled to shore,
  • 8:20 - 8:22
    all wired back to a central place that could listen
  • 8:22 - 8:24
    to sounds over the whole North Atlantic.
  • 8:24 - 8:27
    And after the Berlin Wall fell, the Navy made these systems available
  • 8:27 - 8:29
    to whale bio-acousticians
  • 8:29 - 8:31
    to see what they could hear.
  • 8:31 - 8:33
    This is a plot from Christopher Clark
  • 8:33 - 8:36
    who tracked one individual blue whale
  • 8:36 - 8:38
    as it passed by Bermuda,
  • 8:38 - 8:41
    went down to the latitude of Miami and came back again.
  • 8:41 - 8:43
    It was tracked for 43 days,
  • 8:43 - 8:45
    swimming 1,700 kilometers,
  • 8:45 - 8:47
    or more than 1,000 miles.
  • 8:47 - 8:49
    This shows us both that the calls
  • 8:49 - 8:51
    are detectable over hundreds of miles
  • 8:51 - 8:53
    and that whales routinely swim hundreds of miles.
  • 8:53 - 8:55
    They're ocean-based and scale animals
  • 8:55 - 8:57
    who are communicating over much longer ranges
  • 8:57 - 8:59
    than we had anticipated.
  • 8:59 - 9:01
    Unlike fins and blues, which
  • 9:01 - 9:03
    disperse into the temperate and tropical oceans,
  • 9:03 - 9:05
    the humpbacked whales congregate
  • 9:05 - 9:08
    in local traditional breeding grounds,
  • 9:08 - 9:11
    so they can make a sound that's a little higher in frequency,
  • 9:11 - 9:13
    broader-band and more complicated.
  • 9:13 - 9:15
    So you're listening to the complicated song
  • 9:15 - 9:17
    produced by humpbacks here.
  • 9:17 - 9:19
    Humpbacks, when they develop
  • 9:19 - 9:21
    the ability to sing this song,
  • 9:21 - 9:23
    they're listening to other whales
  • 9:23 - 9:26
    and modifying what they sing based on what they're hearing,
  • 9:26 - 9:29
    just like song birds or the dolphin whistles I described.
  • 9:29 - 9:31
    This means that humpback song
  • 9:31 - 9:33
    is a form of animal culture,
  • 9:33 - 9:35
    just like music for humans would be.
  • 9:35 - 9:38
    I think one of the most interesting examples of this
  • 9:38 - 9:40
    comes from Australia.
  • 9:40 - 9:42
    Biologists on the east coast of Australia
  • 9:42 - 9:45
    were recording the songs of humpbacks in that area.
  • 9:45 - 9:48
    And this orange line here marks the typical songs
  • 9:48 - 9:50
    of east coast humpbacks.
  • 9:50 - 9:52
    In '95 they all sang the normal song.
  • 9:52 - 9:54
    But in '96 they heard a few weird songs,
  • 9:54 - 9:57
    and it turned out that these strange songs
  • 9:57 - 9:59
    were typical of west coast whales.
  • 9:59 - 10:02
    The west coast calls became more and more popular,
  • 10:02 - 10:04
    until by 1998,
  • 10:04 - 10:07
    none of the whales sang the east coast song; it was completely gone.
  • 10:07 - 10:09
    They just sang the cool new west coast song.
  • 10:09 - 10:11
    It's as if some new hit style
  • 10:11 - 10:13
    had completely wiped out
  • 10:13 - 10:15
    the old-fashioned style before,
  • 10:15 - 10:17
    and with no golden oldies stations.
  • 10:17 - 10:20
    Nobody sang the old ones.
  • 10:20 - 10:23
    I'd like to briefly just show what the ocean does to these calls.
  • 10:23 - 10:26
    Now you are listening to a recording made by Chris Clark,
  • 10:26 - 10:29
    0.2 miles away from a humpback.
  • 10:29 - 10:32
    You can hear the full frequency range. It's quite loud.
  • 10:32 - 10:34
    You sound very nearby.
  • 10:34 - 10:36
    The next recording you're going to hear
  • 10:36 - 10:38
    was made of the same humpback song
  • 10:38 - 10:40
    50 miles away.
  • 10:40 - 10:42
    That's shown down here.
  • 10:42 - 10:44
    You only hear the low frequencies.
  • 10:44 - 10:46
    You hear the reverberation
  • 10:46 - 10:48
    as the sound travels over long-range in the ocean
  • 10:48 - 10:51
    and is not quite as loud.
  • 10:51 - 10:54
    Now after I play back these humpback calls,
  • 10:54 - 10:57
    I'll play blue whale calls, but they have to be sped up
  • 10:57 - 10:59
    because they're so low in frequency
  • 10:59 - 11:01
    that you wouldn't be able to hear it otherwise.
  • 11:01 - 11:03
    Here's a blue whale call at 50 miles,
  • 11:03 - 11:05
    which was distant for the humpback.
  • 11:05 - 11:08
    It's loud, clear -- you can hear it very clearly.
  • 11:08 - 11:11
    Here's the same call recorded from a hydrophone
  • 11:11 - 11:13
    500 miles away.
  • 11:13 - 11:16
    There's a lot of noise, which is mostly other whales.
  • 11:16 - 11:19
    But you can still hear that faint call.
  • 11:19 - 11:21
    Let's now switch and think about
  • 11:21 - 11:23
    a potential for human impacts.
  • 11:23 - 11:26
    The most dominant sound that humans put into the ocean
  • 11:26 - 11:28
    comes from shipping.
  • 11:28 - 11:30
    This is the sound of a ship,
  • 11:30 - 11:32
    and I'm having to talk a little louder to talk over it.
  • 11:32 - 11:35
    Imagine that whale listening from 500 miles.
  • 11:35 - 11:37
    There's a potential problem that maybe
  • 11:37 - 11:39
    this kind of shipping noise would prevent whales
  • 11:39 - 11:41
    from being able to hear each other.
  • 11:41 - 11:43
    Now this is something that's been known for quite a while.
  • 11:43 - 11:46
    This is a figure from a textbook on underwater sound.
  • 11:46 - 11:48
    And on the y-axis
  • 11:48 - 11:51
    is the loudness of average ambient noise in the deep ocean
  • 11:51 - 11:53
    by frequency.
  • 11:53 - 11:56
    In the low frequencies, this line indicates
  • 11:56 - 11:59
    sound that comes from seismic activity of the earth.
  • 11:59 - 12:01
    Up high, these variable lines
  • 12:01 - 12:04
    indicate increasing noise in this frequency range
  • 12:04 - 12:06
    from higher wind and wave.
  • 12:06 - 12:09
    But right in the middle here where there's a sweet spot,
  • 12:09 - 12:11
    the noise is dominated by human ships.
  • 12:11 - 12:13
    Now think about it. This is an amazing thing:
  • 12:13 - 12:16
    That in this frequency range where whales communicate,
  • 12:16 - 12:19
    the main source globally, on our planet, for the noise
  • 12:19 - 12:21
    comes from human ships,
  • 12:21 - 12:24
    thousands of human ships, distant, far away,
  • 12:24 - 12:26
    just all aggregating.
  • 12:26 - 12:29
    The next slide will show what the impact this may have
  • 12:29 - 12:31
    on the range at which whales can communicate.
  • 12:31 - 12:34
    So here we have the loudness of a call at the whale.
  • 12:34 - 12:36
    And as we get farther away,
  • 12:36 - 12:38
    the sound gets fainter and fainter.
  • 12:38 - 12:41
    Now in the pre-industrial ocean, as we were mentioning,
  • 12:41 - 12:43
    this whale call could be easily detected.
  • 12:43 - 12:45
    It's louder than noise
  • 12:45 - 12:47
    at a range of a thousand kilometers.
  • 12:47 - 12:50
    Let's now take that additional increase in noise
  • 12:50 - 12:52
    that we saw comes from shipping.
  • 12:52 - 12:54
    All of a sudden, the effective range of communication
  • 12:54 - 12:57
    goes from a thousand kilometers to 10 kilometers.
  • 12:57 - 12:59
    Now if this signal is used for males and females
  • 12:59 - 13:02
    to find each other for mating and they're dispersed,
  • 13:02 - 13:04
    imagine the impact this could have
  • 13:04 - 13:07
    on the recovery of endangered populations.
  • 13:07 - 13:09
    Whales also have contact calls
  • 13:09 - 13:12
    like I described for the dolphins.
  • 13:12 - 13:14
    I'll play the sound of a contact call used
  • 13:14 - 13:16
    by right whales to stay in touch.
  • 13:16 - 13:18
    And this is the kind of call that is used by,
  • 13:18 - 13:20
    say, right whale mothers and calves
  • 13:20 - 13:22
    as they separate to come back again.
  • 13:22 - 13:24
    Now imagine -- let's put the ship noise in the picture.
  • 13:24 - 13:26
    What's a mother to do
  • 13:26 - 13:28
    if the ship comes by and her calf isn't there?
  • 13:28 - 13:31
    I'll describe a couple strategies.
  • 13:31 - 13:33
    One strategy is if your call's down here,
  • 13:33 - 13:35
    and the noise is in this band,
  • 13:35 - 13:38
    you could shift the frequency of your call out of the noise band
  • 13:38 - 13:40
    and communicate better.
  • 13:40 - 13:43
    Susan Parks of Penn State has actually studied this.
  • 13:43 - 13:46
    She's looked in the Atlantic. Here's data from the South Atlantic.
  • 13:46 - 13:49
    Here's a typical South Atlantic contact call from the '70s.
  • 13:49 - 13:52
    Look what happened by 2000 to the average call.
  • 13:52 - 13:54
    Same thing in the North Atlantic,
  • 13:54 - 13:56
    in the '50s versus 2000.
  • 13:56 - 13:58
    Over the last 50 years,
  • 13:58 - 14:00
    as we've put more noise into the oceans,
  • 14:00 - 14:02
    these whales have had to shift.
  • 14:02 - 14:04
    It's as if the whole population had to shift
  • 14:04 - 14:07
    from being basses to singing as a tenor.
  • 14:07 - 14:09
    It's an amazing shift, induced by humans
  • 14:09 - 14:11
    over this large scale,
  • 14:11 - 14:13
    in both time and space.
  • 14:13 - 14:15
    And we now know that whales can compensate for noise
  • 14:15 - 14:18
    by calling louder, like I did when that ship was playing,
  • 14:18 - 14:20
    by waiting for silence
  • 14:20 - 14:23
    and by shifting their call out of the noise band.
  • 14:23 - 14:25
    Now there's probably costs to calling louder
  • 14:25 - 14:27
    or shifting the frequency away from where you want to be,
  • 14:27 - 14:29
    and there's probably lost opportunities.
  • 14:29 - 14:31
    If we also have to wait for silence,
  • 14:31 - 14:34
    they may miss a critical opportunity to communicate.
  • 14:34 - 14:36
    So we have to be very concerned
  • 14:36 - 14:38
    about when the noise in habitats
  • 14:38 - 14:40
    degrades the habitat enough
  • 14:40 - 14:43
    that the animals either have to pay too much to be able to communicate,
  • 14:43 - 14:45
    or are not able to perform critical functions.
  • 14:45 - 14:48
    It's a really important problem.
  • 14:48 - 14:50
    And I'm happy to say that there are several
  • 14:50 - 14:53
    very promising developments in this area,
  • 14:53 - 14:56
    looking at the impact of shipping on whales.
  • 14:56 - 14:58
    In terms of the shipping noise,
  • 14:58 - 15:01
    the International Maritime Organization of the United Nations
  • 15:01 - 15:04
    has formed a group whose job is to establish
  • 15:04 - 15:06
    guidelines for quieting ships,
  • 15:06 - 15:08
    to tell the industry how you could quiet ships.
  • 15:08 - 15:10
    And they've already found
  • 15:10 - 15:13
    that by being more intelligent about better propeller design,
  • 15:13 - 15:16
    you can reduce that noise by 90 percent.
  • 15:16 - 15:19
    If you actually insulate and isolate
  • 15:19 - 15:21
    the machinery of the ship from the hull,
  • 15:21 - 15:24
    you can reduce that noise by 99 percent.
  • 15:24 - 15:27
    So at this point, it's primarily an issue of cost and standards.
  • 15:27 - 15:29
    If this group can establish standards,
  • 15:29 - 15:32
    and if the shipbuilding industry adopts them for building new ships,
  • 15:32 - 15:34
    we can now see a gradual decline
  • 15:34 - 15:36
    in this potential problem.
  • 15:36 - 15:39
    But there's also another problem from ships that I'm illustrating here,
  • 15:39 - 15:41
    and that's the problem of collision.
  • 15:41 - 15:44
    This is a whale that just squeaked by
  • 15:44 - 15:47
    a rapidly moving container ship and avoided collision.
  • 15:47 - 15:49
    But collision is a serious problem.
  • 15:49 - 15:52
    Endangered whales are killed every year by ship collision,
  • 15:52 - 15:55
    and it's very important to try to reduce this.
  • 15:55 - 15:58
    I'll discuss two very promising approaches.
  • 15:58 - 16:00
    The first case comes from the Bay of Fundy.
  • 16:00 - 16:02
    These black lines mark shipping lanes
  • 16:02 - 16:04
    in and out of the Bay of Fundy.
  • 16:04 - 16:06
    The colorized area
  • 16:06 - 16:09
    shows the risk of collision for endangered right whales
  • 16:09 - 16:11
    because of the ships moving in this lane.
  • 16:11 - 16:14
    It turns out that this lane here
  • 16:14 - 16:17
    goes right through a major feeding area of right whales in the summer time,
  • 16:17 - 16:20
    and it makes an area of a significant risk of collision.
  • 16:20 - 16:22
    Well, biologists
  • 16:22 - 16:24
    who couldn't take no for an answer
  • 16:24 - 16:26
    went to the International Maritime Organization
  • 16:26 - 16:28
    and petitioned them to say,
  • 16:28 - 16:30
    "Can't you move that lane? Those are just lines on the ground.
  • 16:30 - 16:32
    Can't you move them over to a place
  • 16:32 - 16:34
    where there's less of a risk?"
  • 16:34 - 16:36
    And the International Maritime Organization responded very strongly,
  • 16:36 - 16:38
    "These are the new lanes."
  • 16:38 - 16:40
    The shipping lanes have been moved.
  • 16:40 - 16:43
    And as you can see, the risk of collision is much lower.
  • 16:43 - 16:45
    So it's very promising, actually.
  • 16:45 - 16:47
    We can be very creative about thinking
  • 16:47 - 16:49
    of different ways to reduce these risks.
  • 16:49 - 16:51
    Another action which was just taken independently
  • 16:51 - 16:54
    by a shipping company itself
  • 16:54 - 16:57
    was initiated because of concerns the shipping company had
  • 16:57 - 17:00
    about greenhouse gas emissions with global warming.
  • 17:00 - 17:03
    The Maersk Line looked at their competition
  • 17:03 - 17:06
    and saw that everybody who is in shipping thinks time is money.
  • 17:06 - 17:08
    They rush as fast as they can to get to their port.
  • 17:08 - 17:10
    But then they often wait there.
  • 17:10 - 17:12
    What Maersk did is they worked ways to slow down.
  • 17:12 - 17:15
    They could slow down by about 50 percent.
  • 17:15 - 17:18
    This reduced their fuel consumption by about 30 percent,
  • 17:18 - 17:20
    which saved them money,
  • 17:20 - 17:23
    and at the same time, it had a significant benefit for whales.
  • 17:23 - 17:26
    It you slow down, you reduce the amount of noise you make
  • 17:26 - 17:28
    and you reduce the risk of collision.
  • 17:28 - 17:30
    So to conclude, I'd just like to point out,
  • 17:30 - 17:32
    you know, the whales live in
  • 17:32 - 17:34
    an amazing acoustic environment.
  • 17:34 - 17:36
    They've evolved over tens of millions of years
  • 17:36 - 17:38
    to take advantage of this.
  • 17:38 - 17:41
    And we need to be very attentive and vigilant
  • 17:41 - 17:43
    to thinking about where things that we do
  • 17:43 - 17:45
    may unintentionally prevent them
  • 17:45 - 17:48
    from being able to achieve their important activities.
  • 17:48 - 17:50
    At the same time, we need to be really creative
  • 17:50 - 17:53
    in thinking of solutions to be able to help reduce these problems.
  • 17:53 - 17:55
    I hope these examples have shown
  • 17:55 - 17:57
    some of the different directions we can take
  • 17:57 - 17:59
    in addition to protected areas
  • 17:59 - 18:02
    to be able to keep the ocean safe for whales to be able to continue to communicate.
  • 18:02 - 18:04
    Thank you very much.
  • 18:04 - 18:06
    (Applause)
Title:
The intriguing sound of marine mammals
Speaker:
Peter Tyack
Description:

Peter Tyack of Woods Hole talks about a hidden wonder of the sea: underwater sound. Onstage at Mission Blue, he explains the amazing ways whales use sound and song to communicate across hundreds of miles of ocean.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
18:07
TED edited English subtitles for The intriguing sound of marine mammals
TED added a translation

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions