< Return to Video

The smelly mystery of the human pheromone

  • 0:01 - 0:04
    "Pheromone" is a very powerful word.
  • 0:04 - 0:09
    It conjures up sex, abandon, loss of control,
  • 0:09 - 0:12
    and you can see, it's very important as a word.
  • 0:12 - 0:18
    But it's only 50 years old. It was invented in 1959.
  • 0:18 - 0:20
    Now, if you put that word into the web,
  • 0:20 - 0:22
    as you may have done,
  • 0:22 - 0:24
    you'll come up with millions of hits,
  • 0:24 - 0:27
    and almost all of those sites are trying to sell you
  • 0:27 - 0:29
    something to make you irresistible
  • 0:29 - 0:32
    for 10 dollars or more.
  • 0:32 - 0:34
    Now this is a very attractive idea,
  • 0:34 - 0:37
    and the molecules they mention
  • 0:37 - 0:39
    sound really science-y.
  • 0:39 - 0:40
    They've got lots of syllables.
  • 0:40 - 0:43
    It's things like androstenol and drostenome
  • 0:43 - 0:45
    or drostendienome.
  • 0:45 - 0:47
    It gets better and better,
  • 0:47 - 0:50
    and when you combine that with white lab coats,
  • 0:50 - 0:51
    you must imagine that there is
  • 0:51 - 0:54
    fantastic science behind this.
  • 0:54 - 0:58
    But sadly, these are fraudulent claims
  • 0:58 - 1:02
    supported by dodgy science.
  • 1:02 - 1:05
    The problem is that, although there are many
  • 1:05 - 1:08
    good scientists working on what they think
  • 1:08 - 1:09
    are human pheromones,
  • 1:09 - 1:12
    and they're publishing in respectable journals,
  • 1:12 - 1:14
    at the basis of this,
  • 1:14 - 1:17
    despite very sophisticated experiments,
  • 1:17 - 1:19
    there really is no good science behind it,
  • 1:19 - 1:23
    because it's based on a problem,
  • 1:23 - 1:26
    which is, nobody has systematically gone through
  • 1:26 - 1:28
    all the odors that humans produce,
  • 1:28 - 1:31
    and there are thousands of
    molecules that we give off.
  • 1:31 - 1:34
    We're mammals. We produce a lot of smell.
  • 1:34 - 1:36
    Nobody has gone through systematically
  • 1:36 - 1:39
    to work out which molecules really are pheromones.
  • 1:39 - 1:41
    They've just plucked a few,
  • 1:41 - 1:43
    and all these experiments are based on those,
  • 1:43 - 1:46
    but there's no good evidence at all.
  • 1:46 - 1:48
    Now, that's not to say
  • 1:48 - 1:50
    that smell is not important to people.
  • 1:50 - 1:53
    It is, and some people are real enthusiasts,
  • 1:53 - 1:57
    and one of these was Napoleon.
  • 1:57 - 1:59
    And famously, you may remember
  • 1:59 - 2:02
    that out on the campaign trail for war,
  • 2:02 - 2:05
    he wrote to his lover, Empress Josephine,
  • 2:05 - 2:08
    saying, "Don't wash, I'm coming home."
  • 2:08 - 2:10
    (Laughter)
  • 2:10 - 2:12
    So he didn't want to lose any of her richness
  • 2:12 - 2:14
    in the days before he'd get home,
  • 2:14 - 2:16
    and it is still, you'll find websites
  • 2:16 - 2:20
    that offer this as a major quirk.
  • 2:20 - 2:21
    At the same time, though,
  • 2:21 - 2:23
    we spend about as much money
  • 2:23 - 2:25
    taking the smells off us
  • 2:25 - 2:28
    as putting them back on in perfumes,
  • 2:28 - 2:32
    and perfumes are a multi-billion dollar business.
  • 2:32 - 2:35
    So what I want to do in the rest of this talk
  • 2:35 - 2:40
    is tell you about what pheromones really are,
  • 2:40 - 2:43
    tell you why I think we would expect
  • 2:43 - 2:46
    humans to have pheromones,
  • 2:46 - 2:50
    tell you about some of the
    confusions in pheromones,
  • 2:50 - 2:53
    and then finally, I want to end with
  • 2:53 - 2:55
    a promising avenue which shows us
  • 2:55 - 2:59
    the way we ought to be going.
  • 2:59 - 3:03
    So the ancient Greeks knew
  • 3:03 - 3:07
    that dog sent invisible signals between each other.
  • 3:07 - 3:10
    A female dog in heat
  • 3:10 - 3:12
    sent an invisible signal to male dogs
  • 3:12 - 3:13
    for miles around,
  • 3:13 - 3:15
    and it wasn't a sound, it was a smell.
  • 3:15 - 3:17
    You could take the smell from the female dog,
  • 3:17 - 3:20
    and the dogs would chase the cloth.
  • 3:20 - 3:21
    But the problem for everybody
  • 3:21 - 3:24
    who could see this effect
  • 3:24 - 3:25
    was that you couldn't identify the molecules.
  • 3:25 - 3:28
    You couldn't demonstrate it was chemical.
  • 3:28 - 3:29
    The reason for that, of course,
  • 3:29 - 3:31
    is that each of these animals
  • 3:31 - 3:32
    produces tiny quantities,
  • 3:32 - 3:34
    and in the case of the dog,
  • 3:34 - 3:36
    males dogs can smell it, but we can't smell it.
  • 3:36 - 3:41
    And it was only in 1959 that a German team,
  • 3:41 - 3:44
    after spending 20 years in
    search of these molecules,
  • 3:44 - 3:48
    discovered, identified, the first pheromone,
  • 3:48 - 3:51
    and this was the sex pheromone of a silk moth.
  • 3:51 - 3:54
    Now this was an inspired choice by Adolf Butenandt
  • 3:54 - 3:55
    and his team,
  • 3:55 - 3:57
    because he needed half a million moths
  • 3:57 - 4:00
    to get enough material to do the chemical analysis.
  • 4:00 - 4:02
    But he created the model
  • 4:02 - 4:05
    for how you should go about pheromone analysis.
  • 4:05 - 4:08
    He basically went through systematically,
  • 4:08 - 4:10
    showing that only the molecule in question
  • 4:10 - 4:12
    was the one that stimulated the males,
  • 4:12 - 4:14
    not all the others.
  • 4:14 - 4:16
    He analyzed it very carefully.
  • 4:16 - 4:18
    He synthesized the molecule,
  • 4:18 - 4:21
    and then tried the synthesized
    molecule on the males
  • 4:21 - 4:24
    and got them to respond and showed it was,
  • 4:24 - 4:26
    indeed, that molecule.
  • 4:26 - 4:28
    That's closing the circle.
  • 4:28 - 4:31
    That's the thing which has
    never been done with humans:
  • 4:31 - 4:34
    nothing systematic, no real demonstration.
  • 4:34 - 4:36
    With that new concept,
  • 4:36 - 4:38
    we needed a new word,
  • 4:38 - 4:41
    and that was the word "pheromone,"
  • 4:41 - 4:44
    and it's basically transferred excitement,
  • 4:44 - 4:47
    transferred between individuals,
  • 4:47 - 4:49
    and since 1959, pheromones have been found
  • 4:49 - 4:51
    right the way across the animal kingdom,
  • 4:51 - 4:54
    in male animals, in female animals.
  • 4:54 - 4:55
    It works just as well underwater
  • 4:55 - 4:57
    for goldfish and lobsters.
  • 4:57 - 4:59
    And almost every mammal you can think of
  • 4:59 - 5:01
    has had a pheromone identified,
  • 5:01 - 5:05
    and of course, an enormous number of insects.
  • 5:05 - 5:07
    So we know that pheromones exist
  • 5:07 - 5:09
    right the way across the animal kingdom.
  • 5:09 - 5:11
    What about humans?
  • 5:11 - 5:13
    Well, the first thing, of course,
  • 5:13 - 5:15
    is that we're mammals,
  • 5:15 - 5:16
    and mammals are smelly.
  • 5:16 - 5:18
    As any dog owner can tell you,
  • 5:18 - 5:20
    we smell, they smell.
  • 5:20 - 5:22
    But the real reason we might think
  • 5:22 - 5:24
    that humans have pheromones
  • 5:24 - 5:27
    is the change that occurs as we grow up.
  • 5:27 - 5:29
    The smell of a room of teenagers
  • 5:29 - 5:31
    is quite different
  • 5:31 - 5:33
    from the smell of a room of small children.
  • 5:33 - 5:37
    What's changed? And of course, it's puberty.
  • 5:37 - 5:38
    Along with the pubic hair
  • 5:38 - 5:40
    and the hair in the armpits,
  • 5:40 - 5:43
    new glands start to secrete in those places,
  • 5:43 - 5:46
    and that's what's making the change in smell.
  • 5:46 - 5:48
    If we were any other kind of mammal,
  • 5:48 - 5:50
    or any other kind of animal,
  • 5:50 - 5:51
    we would say,
  • 5:51 - 5:52
    "That must be something to do with pheromones,"
  • 5:52 - 5:54
    and we'd start looking properly.
  • 5:54 - 5:57
    But there are some problems, and this is why,
  • 5:57 - 6:00
    I think, people have not looked for
  • 6:00 - 6:03
    pheromones so effectively in humans.
  • 6:03 - 6:05
    There are, indeed, problems.
  • 6:05 - 6:07
    And the first of these
  • 6:07 - 6:08
    is perhaps surprising.
  • 6:08 - 6:10
    It's all about culture.
  • 6:10 - 6:12
    Now moths don't learn a lot
  • 6:12 - 6:16
    about what is good to smell, but humans do,
  • 6:16 - 6:18
    and up to the age of about four,
  • 6:18 - 6:20
    any smell, no matter how rancid,
  • 6:20 - 6:22
    is simply interesting.
  • 6:22 - 6:24
    And I understand that the major role of parents
  • 6:24 - 6:27
    is to stop kids putting their fingers in poo,
  • 6:27 - 6:30
    because it's always something nice to smell.
  • 6:30 - 6:33
    But gradually we learn what's not good,
  • 6:33 - 6:35
    and one of the things we learn
  • 6:35 - 6:37
    at the same time as what is not good
  • 6:37 - 6:39
    is what is good.
  • 6:39 - 6:41
    Now, the cheese behind me
  • 6:41 - 6:45
    is a British, if not an English, delicacy.
  • 6:45 - 6:48
    It's ripe blue Stilton.
  • 6:48 - 6:52
    Liking it is incomprehensible to
    people from other countries.
  • 6:52 - 6:56
    Every culture has its own special food
  • 6:56 - 6:58
    and national delicacy.
  • 6:58 - 7:00
    If you were to come from Iceland,
  • 7:00 - 7:01
    your national dish
  • 7:01 - 7:04
    is deep rotted shark.
  • 7:04 - 7:07
    Now, all of these things are acquired taste,
  • 7:07 - 7:10
    but they form almost a badge of identity.
  • 7:10 - 7:14
    You're part of the in group.
  • 7:14 - 7:18
    The second thing is the sense of smell.
  • 7:18 - 7:22
    Each of us has a unique odor world,
  • 7:22 - 7:24
    in the sense that what we smell.
  • 7:24 - 7:27
    We each smell a completely different world.
  • 7:27 - 7:29
    Now, smell was the hardest
  • 7:29 - 7:30
    of the senses to crack,
  • 7:30 - 7:32
    and the Nobel Prize awarded to
  • 7:32 - 7:34
    Richard Axel and Linda Buck
  • 7:34 - 7:37
    was only awarded in 2004
  • 7:37 - 7:39
    for their discovery of how smell works.
  • 7:39 - 7:41
    It's really hard,
  • 7:41 - 7:43
    but in essence, nerves from the brain
  • 7:43 - 7:45
    go up into the nose
  • 7:45 - 7:48
    and on these nerves, exposed in the nose
  • 7:48 - 7:51
    to the outside air, are receptors,
  • 7:51 - 7:54
    and odor molecules coming in on a sniff
  • 7:54 - 7:56
    interact with these receptors,
  • 7:56 - 8:00
    and if they bond, they send the nerve a signal
  • 8:00 - 8:02
    which goes back into the brain.
  • 8:02 - 8:05
    We don't just have one kind of receptor.
  • 8:05 - 8:07
    If you're a human, you have about 400
  • 8:07 - 8:09
    different kinds of receptors,
  • 8:09 - 8:11
    and the brain knows what you're smelling
  • 8:11 - 8:13
    because of the combination of receptors
  • 8:13 - 8:16
    and nerve cells that they trigger,
  • 8:16 - 8:18
    sending messages up to the brain
  • 8:18 - 8:20
    in a combinatorial fashion.
  • 8:20 - 8:21
    But it's a bit more complicated,
  • 8:21 - 8:23
    because each of those 400
  • 8:23 - 8:26
    comes in various variants,
  • 8:26 - 8:28
    and depending which variant you have,
  • 8:28 - 8:32
    you might smell coriander, or cilantro, that herb,
  • 8:32 - 8:34
    either as something delicious and savory
  • 8:34 - 8:36
    or something like soap.
  • 8:36 - 8:40
    So we each have an individual world of smell,
  • 8:40 - 8:41
    and that complicates anything
  • 8:41 - 8:43
    when we're studying smell.
  • 8:43 - 8:46
    Well, we really ought to talk about armpits,
  • 8:46 - 8:49
    and I have to say that I do
    have particularly good ones.
  • 8:49 - 8:52
    Now, I'm not going to share them with you,
  • 8:52 - 8:53
    but this is the place that most people
  • 8:53 - 8:55
    have looked for pheromones.
  • 8:55 - 8:57
    There is one good reason,
  • 8:57 - 9:00
    which is, the great apes have armpits
  • 9:00 - 9:02
    as their unique characteristic.
  • 9:02 - 9:05
    The other primates have scent glands
  • 9:05 - 9:06
    in other parts of the body.
  • 9:06 - 9:09
    The great apes have these armpits
  • 9:09 - 9:11
    full of secretory glands
  • 9:11 - 9:14
    producing smells all the time,
  • 9:14 - 9:17
    enormous numbers of molecules.
  • 9:17 - 9:19
    When they're secreted from the glands,
  • 9:19 - 9:21
    the molecules are odorless.
  • 9:21 - 9:23
    They have no smell at all,
  • 9:23 - 9:25
    and it's only the wonderful bacteria
  • 9:25 - 9:27
    growing on the rainforest of hair
  • 9:27 - 9:29
    that actually produces the smells
  • 9:29 - 9:31
    that we know and love.
  • 9:31 - 9:32
    And so incidentally, if you want to reduce
  • 9:32 - 9:34
    the amount of smell,
  • 9:34 - 9:36
    clear-cutting your armpits
  • 9:36 - 9:38
    is a very effective way of reducing
  • 9:38 - 9:40
    the habitat for the bacteria,
  • 9:40 - 9:42
    and you'll find they remain less smelly
  • 9:42 - 9:44
    for much longer.
  • 9:44 - 9:46
    But although we've focused on armpits,
  • 9:46 - 9:48
    I think it's partly because they're the least
  • 9:48 - 9:50
    embarrassing place to go and ask people
  • 9:50 - 9:52
    for samples.
  • 9:52 - 9:54
    There is actually another reason why we might not
  • 9:54 - 9:57
    be looking for a universal sex pheromone there,
  • 9:57 - 10:01
    and that's because 20 percent
    of the world's population
  • 10:01 - 10:05
    doesn't have smelly armpits like me.
  • 10:05 - 10:09
    And these are people from China, Japan,
  • 10:09 - 10:11
    Korea, and other parts of northeast Asia.
  • 10:11 - 10:15
    They simply don't secrete those odorless precursors
  • 10:15 - 10:19
    that the bacteria love to use to produce the smells
  • 10:19 - 10:21
    that in an ethnocentric way we always thought of
  • 10:21 - 10:24
    as characteristic of armpits.
  • 10:24 - 10:28
    So it doesn't apply to 20 percent of the world.
  • 10:28 - 10:30
    So what should we be doing
  • 10:30 - 10:34
    in our search for human pheromones?
  • 10:34 - 10:36
    I'm fairly convinced that we do have them.
  • 10:36 - 10:38
    We're mammals, like everybody else
  • 10:38 - 10:42
    who's a mammal, and we probably do have them.
  • 10:42 - 10:44
    But what I think we should do
  • 10:44 - 10:46
    is go right back to the beginning,
  • 10:46 - 10:48
    and basically look all over the body.
  • 10:48 - 10:50
    No matter how embarrassing,
  • 10:50 - 10:53
    we need to search and go for the first time
  • 10:53 - 10:55
    where no one else has dared tread.
  • 10:55 - 10:57
    It's going to be difficult.
  • 10:57 - 11:00
    It's going to be embarrassing, but we need to look.
  • 11:00 - 11:02
    We also need to go back to the ideas
  • 11:02 - 11:06
    that Butenandt used when he
    was studying the silk moth.
  • 11:06 - 11:09
    We need to go back and look systematically
  • 11:09 - 11:11
    at all the molecules that are being produced,
  • 11:11 - 11:14
    and work out which ones are really involved.
  • 11:14 - 11:16
    It isn't good enough simply to pluck a couple
  • 11:16 - 11:18
    and say, "They'll do."
  • 11:18 - 11:19
    We have to actually demonstrate
  • 11:19 - 11:22
    that they really have the effects we claim.
  • 11:22 - 11:25
    There is one team that I'm
    actually very impressed by.
  • 11:25 - 11:28
    They're in France, and their previous success
  • 11:28 - 11:33
    was identifying the rabbit memory pheromone.
  • 11:33 - 11:35
    They've turned their attention now
  • 11:35 - 11:39
    to human babies and mothers.
  • 11:39 - 11:42
    So this is a baby having a drink of milk
  • 11:42 - 11:44
    from its mother's breast.
  • 11:44 - 11:46
    Her nipple is completely hidden
  • 11:46 - 11:48
    by the baby's head,
  • 11:48 - 11:52
    but what you'll notice is a white droplet
  • 11:52 - 11:53
    with an arrow pointing to it,
  • 11:53 - 11:56
    and that's the secretion from the areolar glands.
  • 11:56 - 11:59
    Now, we all have them, men and women,
  • 11:59 - 12:02
    and these are the little bumps around the nipple,
  • 12:02 - 12:04
    and if you're a lactating woman,
  • 12:04 - 12:06
    these start to secrete.
  • 12:06 - 12:09
    It's a very interesting secretion.
  • 12:09 - 12:12
    What Benoist Schaal and his team developed
  • 12:12 - 12:15
    was a simple test to investigate
  • 12:15 - 12:17
    what the effect of this secretion might be,
  • 12:17 - 12:20
    in effect, a simple bioassay.
  • 12:20 - 12:23
    So this is a sleeping baby,
  • 12:23 - 12:28
    and under its nose, we've put a clean glass rod.
  • 12:28 - 12:30
    The baby remains sleeping,
  • 12:30 - 12:32
    showing no interest at all.
  • 12:32 - 12:34
    But if we go to any mother
  • 12:34 - 12:37
    who is secreting from the areolar glands,
  • 12:37 - 12:38
    so it's not about recognition,
  • 12:38 - 12:41
    it can be from any mother,
  • 12:41 - 12:42
    if we take the secretion
  • 12:42 - 12:45
    and now put it under the baby's nose,
  • 12:45 - 12:47
    we get a very different reaction.
  • 12:47 - 12:50
    It's a connoisseur's reaction of delight,
  • 12:50 - 12:52
    and it opens its mouth
  • 12:52 - 12:54
    and sticks out its tongue
  • 12:54 - 12:56
    and starts to suck.
  • 12:56 - 12:58
    Now, since this is from any mother,
  • 12:58 - 13:00
    it could really be a pheromone.
  • 13:00 - 13:02
    It's not about individual recognition.
  • 13:02 - 13:04
    Any mother will do.
  • 13:04 - 13:05
    Now, why is this important,
  • 13:05 - 13:07
    apart from being simply very interesting?
  • 13:07 - 13:09
    It's because women vary
  • 13:09 - 13:12
    in the number of areolar glands that they have,
  • 13:12 - 13:14
    and there is a correlation between the ease
  • 13:14 - 13:17
    with which babies start to suckle
  • 13:17 - 13:19
    and the number of areolar glands she has.
  • 13:19 - 13:22
    It appears that the more secretions she's got,
  • 13:22 - 13:25
    the more likely the baby is to suckle quickly.
  • 13:25 - 13:27
    If you're a mammal,
  • 13:27 - 13:29
    the most dangerous time in life
  • 13:29 - 13:32
    is the first few hours after birth.
  • 13:32 - 13:35
    You have to get that first drink of milk,
  • 13:35 - 13:39
    and if you don't get it, you won't survive.
  • 13:39 - 13:40
    You'll be dead.
  • 13:40 - 13:43
    Since many babies actually find it difficult
  • 13:43 - 13:44
    to take that first meal,
  • 13:44 - 13:47
    because they're not getting the right stimulus,
  • 13:47 - 13:49
    if we could identify what that molecule was,
  • 13:49 - 13:52
    and the French team are being very cautious,
  • 13:52 - 13:54
    but if we could identify the molecule,
  • 13:54 - 13:56
    synthesize it, it would then mean
  • 13:56 - 13:58
    premature babies would be more likely to suckle,
  • 13:58 - 14:01
    and every baby would have a better chance
  • 14:01 - 14:03
    of survival.
  • 14:03 - 14:05
    So what I want to argue is this is one example
  • 14:05 - 14:08
    of where a systematic, really scientific approach
  • 14:08 - 14:11
    can actually bring you a real understanding
  • 14:11 - 14:13
    of pheromones.
  • 14:13 - 14:15
    There could be all sorts of medical interventions.
  • 14:15 - 14:17
    There could be all sorts of things
  • 14:17 - 14:18
    that humans are doing with pheromones
  • 14:18 - 14:20
    that we simply don't know at the moment.
  • 14:20 - 14:22
    What we need to remember is pheromones
  • 14:22 - 14:24
    are not just about sex.
  • 14:24 - 14:25
    They're about all sorts of things to do
  • 14:25 - 14:27
    with a mammal's life.
  • 14:27 - 14:30
    So do go forward and do search for more.
  • 14:30 - 14:31
    There's lots to find.
  • 14:31 - 14:33
    Thank you very much.
  • 14:33 - 14:37
    (Applause)
Title:
The smelly mystery of the human pheromone
Speaker:
Tristram Wyatt
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
14:53

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions