-
It's a real pleasure to be here
-
and to present the work I've been doing
-
over the last five years or so since my PhD.
-
Genuinely, I want you to try and understand
-
as much as you can
-
So it's not the easiest material,
-
And especially when you don't have a background in neuroscience.
-
It's not always that easy to get a handle on these processes.
-
But I'm going to do my absolute best
-
to try and help you understand, and
-
I'll provide some metaphors to try and
-
break things down to make things more accessible.
-
Also it would be useful to keep
-
some pens out because there will be
-
some references, so if you genuinely you do
-
want to understand how these drugs
-
work in the brain, then it does require
-
a bit of work on your parts as well, unfortunately.
-
So you'd have to go away and look up
-
some of these references and do some
-
background reading.
-
So I just want to start by saying
-
that this work is part of the
-
Beckley-Imperial psychedelic research programme,
-
which is an initiative between David Kotts
-
and Amanda Fielding of the Beckley Foundation.
-
Amanda is a key collaborative partner in this work,
-
and David Nutts, the principal investigator on it.
-
So we'll start with the science.
-
We know that psilocybin is an ingredient
-
in magic mushrooms.
-
Now, psilocybin is the [pro-drug] of psilocin,
-
which is remarkably similar in its molecular structure
-
to the endogenous neurotransmitter
-
which is found throughout the brain, serotonin.
-
So it is really quite striking how similar
-
it is in its molecular structure.
-
Just a subtle change in its structure
-
confers such profound effects of consciousness.
-
Not Synced
So this already
-
Not Synced
is a matter of great intrigue
-
Not Synced
about how these drugs work in the brain.
-
Not Synced
So what was found in the mid-1980s
-
Not Synced
was a strong positive correlation
-
Not Synced
between a psychedelic drug's affinity
-
Not Synced
for the serotonin 2A receptor,
-
Not Synced
a particular subtype of the serotonin receptor,
-
Not Synced
and the drug's potency.
-
Not Synced
So a good example to help illustrate
-
Not Synced
it's principle is, LSD has a very high affinity
-
Not Synced
for the serotonin 2A receptor --
-
Not Synced
it's very sticky, and it's also incredibly potent.
-
Not Synced
So that helps you understand.
-
Not Synced
Also, Franz Vollenweider did an excellent study
-
Not Synced
blocking the serotinin 2A receptor
-
Not Synced
with ketanserin, a relatively selective
-
Not Synced
serotonin 2A receptor blocker,
-
Not Synced
and he found that pre-treatment
-
Not Synced
with this drug blocked the psychedelic effects
-
Not Synced
of psilocybin. So there's good evidence
-
Not Synced
that these drugs trigger their effects
-
Not Synced
on consciousness by initial effect on
-
Not Synced
the serotonin 2A receptor.
-
Not Synced
So already we have an important
-
Not Synced
fundamental relationship that's been discovered
-
Not Synced
between the serotonin system and how these drugs work
-
Not Synced
in the brain.
-
Not Synced
So where is the serotonin 2A receptor in the brain?
-
Not Synced
Well, this is the largest serotonin 2A binding study
-
Not Synced
that's been done by a colleague of ours,
-
Not Synced
David Erritzoe.
-
Not Synced
He used a radioactive tracer, or ligand,
-
Not Synced
that sticks to serotonin 2A receptors in the brain.
-
Not Synced
And then you can detect the signal
-
Not Synced
where the ligand is stuck
-
Not Synced
And so doing this, he found that
-
Not Synced
the serotonin 2A receptor is
-
Not Synced
very much a cortical receptor.
-
Not Synced
So the outer layer of the brain, the cortex,
-
Not Synced
(it's referred to as kind of like the bark of a tree)
-
Not Synced
so this outer layer of the brain, that's where you find
-
Not Synced
the serotonin 2A receptor.
-
Not Synced
And it's especially prevalent,
-
Not Synced
especially densely expressed,
-
Not Synced
in high-level cortical regions.
-
Not Synced
So these are regions that
-
Not Synced
don't have a specific sensory function, like,
-
Not Synced
for instance, the visual cortex, which is concerned
-
Not Synced
with visual processing.
-
Not Synced
The heteromodal regions so they have a more
-
Not Synced
kind of divergent, and high-level, function
-
Not Synced
and so the serotonin 2A receptor
-
Not Synced
is especially densely expressed
-
Not Synced
in these high-level cortical regions,
-
Not Synced
such as the posterior cingulate cortex.
-
Not Synced
You will hear this term referred to again
-
Not Synced
throughout my talk
-
Not Synced
because it's a key region of the brain,
-
Not Synced
a very high-level region of the brain,
-
Not Synced
and it seems to be especially implicated in
-
Not Synced
the mechanistic action of these drugs,
-
Not Synced
how they work in the brain.
-
Not Synced
We also know that the serotonin 2A receptor
-
Not Synced
is especially densely expressed
-
Not Synced
in the particular layer of the cortex.
-
Not Synced
So the cortex is organized in a kind of laminar
-
Not Synced
way, and there's some large,
-
Not Synced
what are referred to as pyramidal neurons
-
Not Synced
in layer 5 of the cortex.
-
Not Synced
That is... (pause)
-
Not Synced
This is layer 5 here.
-
Not Synced
So there's some large neurons there,
-
Not Synced
and these are the principal output layer
-
Not Synced
of the cortex,
-
Not Synced
and there's also something else,
-
Not Synced
which is especially important about
-
Not Synced
this cellular group,
-
Not Synced
in terms of how this drug works in the brain.
-
Not Synced
So we know the serotonin 2A receptor is very important,
-
Not Synced
We know where it is,
-
Not Synced
in terms of its spatial distribution in the brain
-
Not Synced
and also within the cortex itself,
-
Not Synced
within its laminae organization. It's dense in the
-
Not Synced
deep pyramidal cells in layer 5.
-
Not Synced
We also know that if you stimulate
-
Not Synced
the serotonin 2A receptor,
-
Not Synced
you have an excitatory effect on the host cells.
-
Not Synced
So the cell that expresses the receptor,
-
Not Synced
if you stimulate it, then you're gonna make that cell
-
Not Synced
more excitable.
-
Not Synced
So these are all important principles that we know.
-
Not Synced
These are kind of the bedrock findings so far
-
Not Synced
about how psychedelics work in the brain.
-
Not Synced
But these are all quite low level;
-
Not Synced
my brain imaging work has been looking at
-
Not Synced
a higher level,
-
Not Synced
what's referred to as a macroscopic level.
-
Not Synced
So, the level you can look at and see
-
Not Synced
on a large scale in terms of
-
Not Synced
brain networks, for instance,
-
Not Synced
and regional brain activity.
-
Not Synced
So let's start with our first study,
-
Not Synced
our first fMRI study with psilocybin.
-
Not Synced
This used the modality referred to as
-
Not Synced
arterial spin labelling, which is
-
Not Synced
a method which measures changes
-
Not Synced
in blood flow in the brain.
-
Not Synced
And generally there's quite a reliable relationship
-
Not Synced
between blood flow in the brain and brain activity.
-
Not Synced
So if blood flow increases,
-
Not Synced
we generally infer
-
Not Synced
that brain activity has increased.
-
Not Synced
So this study had fifteen healthy volunteers,
-
Not Synced
mean age of 34,
-
Not Synced
the scans were 18 minutes in duration,
-
Not Synced
there was a six-minute baseline,
-
Not Synced
and then we looked at changes in blood flow
-
Not Synced
after that baseline.
-
Not Synced
There were two scans:
-
Not Synced
a placebo scan followed by a drug scan.
-
Not Synced
And volunteers just lay in the scanner.
-
Not Synced
They were presented with a fixation cross:
-
Not Synced
just a simple green cross that they looked at
-
Not Synced
on a screen, and they just relaxed
-
Not Synced
and were instructed really just to let their minds wander.
-
Not Synced
And then we looked at how
-
Not Synced
the drug affected blood flow
-
Not Synced
during these conditions.
-
Not Synced
We gave a dose of 2 mg of psilocybin--
-
Not Synced
that compares to about 50 mg when given orally.
-
Not Synced
We gave the drug intravenously,
-
Not Synced
so 2 mg is equivalent to about 50 mg orally,
-
Not Synced
so it's a moderate dose.
-
Not Synced
Here's the design. Here's our 6-minute baseline.
-
Not Synced
Infusion was given over 60 seconds.
-
Not Synced
So it's a relatively rapid infusion.
-
Not Synced
And then the onset of the effects is also rapid,
-
Not Synced
so when the drug is given intravenously,
-
Not Synced
really there is very little delay between
-
Not Synced
delivery of the drug and the onset
-
Not Synced
of the subjective effects.
-
Not Synced
So the subjective effects actually begin,
-
Not Synced
really, before the end of the 60-second adminstration,
-
Not Synced
so the drug seems to really get in the brain
-
Not Synced
very quickly and to change consciousness
-
Not Synced
profoundly very quickly.
-
Not Synced
So what was the first observation?
-
Not Synced
Well, the first thing that we get
-
Not Synced
before we analyze the results are people's
-
Not Synced
descriptions of their experiences.
-
Not Synced
So here's one of them:
-
Not Synced
This volunteer said that "there was a definite
-
Not Synced
sense of lubrication,
-
Not Synced
of freedom,
-
Not Synced
of the cogs being loosened and firing off
-
Not Synced
in all sorts of unexpected directions."
-
Not Synced
Now these subjective reports are really useful
-
Not Synced
because they give you a sense of the
-
Not Synced
mechanics that are going on in the brain,
-
Not Synced
and the changes in the mechanics,
-
Not Synced
which confer the subjective effect --
-
Not Synced
what's going on in the brain on a mechanical level
-
Not Synced
to produce the profound changes in consciousness.
-
Not Synced
This volunteer said, "Everything became
-
Not Synced
fragmented; things were all in bits
-
Not Synced
and it was very hard to hold it all together
-
Not Synced
in a coherent stream."
-
Not Synced
So it's like I said, this stuff is really useful
-
Not Synced
for understanding what is going on
-
Not Synced
on a systems-level in the brain
-
Not Synced
to produce these subjective effects.
-
Not Synced
Now, the default mode network
-
Not Synced
you've heard quite a lot about
-
Not Synced
over the last two days.
-
Not Synced
It is an incredibly important
-
Not Synced
system that's been discovered in the brain.
-
Not Synced
And one of its properties is that
-
Not Synced
it has very dense connectivity,
-
Not Synced
so if you look at the white matter
-
Not Synced
tracks in the brain,
-
Not Synced
so these are the fibers that connect
-
Not Synced
different brain regions,
-
Not Synced
then you'll find that there's a very dense
-
Not Synced
coming together of connections within
-
Not Synced
the default mode notwork.
-
Not Synced
So there seems to be
-
Not Synced
an incredibly important transit hub,
-
Not Synced
a place where different regions
-
Not Synced
can connect via,
-
Not Synced
and information can be projected from,
-
Not Synced
and also a very important integration center.
-
Not Synced
So, to integrate brain function,
-
Not Synced
information comes together at this common
-
Not Synced
convergence zone, and then
-
Not Synced
that gives a coherence to cognition, essentially.
-
Not Synced
That's how it's understood so far.
-
Not Synced
What else about the default mode network?
-
Not Synced
So here's a metaphor to help you try and understand
-
Not Synced
what people are thinking about its function.
-
Not Synced
So a metaphor that could be used to explain
-
Not Synced
what it does is a capital city in a country.
-
Not Synced
It's a place where people come together,
-
Not Synced
things come together,
-
Not Synced
business gets done.
-
Not Synced
And it's an incredibly important hub,
-
Not Synced
and if ever
-
Not Synced
God forbid, something were to happen to
-
Not Synced
London, then, the country as a whole
-
Not Synced
would be seriously effected,
-
Not Synced
and not just Britain.
-
Not Synced
So it's incredibly important
-
Not Synced
integration hub, the default mode network.
-
Not Synced
What else do we know about
-
Not Synced
the default mode network?
-
Not Synced
There's some evidences here.
-
Not Synced
The default mode network undergoes
-
Not Synced
significant ontogenetic development
-
Not Synced
from infancy to adulthood.
-
Not Synced
It undergoes maturation as cognition matures.
-
Not Synced
It has also undergone significant
-
Not Synced
evolutionary expansion
-
Not Synced
so these regions have increased
-
Not Synced
more than other regions
-
Not Synced
from primates to humans.
-
Not Synced
It's more metabolically active
-
Not Synced
than elsewhere in the brain,
-
Not Synced
so the posterior cingulate cortex,
-
Not Synced
which is the region which is circled there,
-
Not Synced
it actually accounts for 40% more
-
Not Synced
blood flow than anywhere else
-
Not Synced
in the brain.
-
Not Synced
So it is a very metabolically hungry system
-
Not Synced
and these regions that are part of it
-
Not Synced
are incredibly metabolically hungry.
-
Not Synced
It's doing something important.
-
Not Synced
Now, a matter of intrigue in neuroscience
-
Not Synced
is that people don't have a really good handle
-
Not Synced
on what the default mode is
-
Not Synced
and what it does,
-
Not Synced
but of course, they enjoy speculating,
-
Not Synced
and the researcher who really discovered
-
Not Synced
the default mode network has referred
-
Not Synced
to its very high energy levels
-
Not Synced
as being like the brain's dark energy.
-
Not Synced
So similar to dark energy in cosmology
-
Not Synced
it is something that we know is there
-
Not Synced
but we don't really know what it does.
-
Not Synced
Really, we make inferences about it,
-
Not Synced
based on its relative decrease in activity.
-
Not Synced
So when you engage in a task,
-
Not Synced
you see a decrease in activity
-
Not Synced
in the default mode network,
-
Not Synced
whereas otherwise
-
Not Synced
it is incredibly active.
-
Not Synced
So it's a mater of intrigue.
-
Not Synced
What's going on here?
-
Not Synced
What's all this energy for, and why
-
Not Synced
is it consuming so much?
-
Not Synced
We know that the default mode network
-
Not Synced
is engaged during self-reflection,
-
Not Synced
so that is a very staple finding.
-
Not Synced
We also know that during
-
Not Synced
complex mental imagery, such as
-
Not Synced
spatial navigation or imagination,
-
Not Synced
fantasy in one's mind eye,
-
Not Synced
you'll also see increased activity
-
Not Synced
in the default mode network.
-
Not Synced
Mental time travel -- so that's being
-
Not Synced
outside of the moment and
-
Not Synced
daydreaming about future events
-
Not Synced
or past biographical experiences.
-
Not Synced
So whenever you come out of the moment
-
Not Synced
and you daydream in this way,
-
Not Synced
you see increased activity and connectivity
-
Not Synced
in the default mode network.
-
Not Synced
Also, theory of mind -- which is
-
Not Synced
putting someone oneself
-
Not Synced
in somebody else's shoes.
-
Not Synced
You will also see increased activity
-
Not Synced
in the default mode network during that function.
-
Not Synced
And metacognition -- which thinking about thinking,
-
Not Synced
that's also linked with
-
Not Synced
default mode network activity.
-
Not Synced
So Raichle, who I said is the guy who
-
Not Synced
really discovered this network
-
Not Synced
has also referred to it as
-
Not Synced
the orchestrator of the self.
-
Not Synced
So all these things
-
Not Synced
led me to start thinking
-
Not Synced
having a background in psychoanalysis
-
Not Synced
and being interested in
-
Not Synced
especially Freudian metapsychology
-
Not Synced
instead of the more mechanistic
-
Not Synced
ideas of Freud. There is remarkable overlap
-
Not Synced
between his descriptions of the ego
-
Not Synced
and the relationship between the ego
-
Not Synced
and the unconscious mind, or the id,
-
Not Synced
and what we're discovering in
-
Not Synced
the default mode network.
-
Not Synced
So in this paper, with Carhart-Harris & Friston,
-
Not Synced
I submitted the idea that
-
Not Synced
the default mode network is essentially
-
Not Synced
the neural substrate source of the ego,
-
Not Synced
which is an idea to be shot down
-
Not Synced
if people find otherwise.
-
Not Synced
But that's science. That's how we work.
-
Not Synced
So what else is there about
-
Not Synced
the default mode network?
-
Not Synced
Well, what's it's relationship to depression?
-
Not Synced
There's a very interesting relationship
-
Not Synced
between default mode network parameters
-
Not Synced
and depressive symptomatology.
-
Not Synced
So we know that connectivity between
-
Not Synced
the medial pre-frontal cortex
-
Not Synced
of the default mode network
-
Not Synced
and the posterior cingulate cortex
-
Not Synced
which are
-
Not Synced
(pause)
-
Not Synced
the front bits and the back bits.
-
Not Synced
So when connectivity between these regions
-
Not Synced
is high,
-
Not Synced
then scores in patients with depression
-
Not Synced
on rumination (so these are scores in rumination.
-
Not Synced
They are thinking over problems
-
Not Synced
and ruminating on negative things),
-
Not Synced
when this is high, connectivity
-
Not Synced
betwen these regions is especially
-
Not Synced
high. And we think this system,
-
Not Synced
this overconnectivity, is really causing people to
-
Not Synced
have a kind of stereotype style of thinking.
-
Not Synced
So they are stuck in this system,
-
Not Synced
they are stuck in their own heads,
-
Not Synced
they are stuck on their sense of self.
-
Not Synced
They are usually thinking very critically
-
Not Synced
about themselves, and going over and over
-
Not Synced
about how terrible they are.
-
Not Synced
So this is a relationship we seem to be discovering
-
Not Synced
about the default mode network
-
Not Synced
and depressive symptomatology.
-
Not Synced
This provides a useful background
-
Not Synced
for what we're finding in terms of how
-
Not Synced
psilocybin is affecting
-
Not Synced
brain networks and brain systems.
-
Not Synced
So in our ASL study, we found
-
Not Synced
it was really quite surprising findings for us,
-
Not Synced
given descriptions of consciousness
-
Not Synced
being expanded by psychedelic drugs.
-
Not Synced
We are given some previous work,
-
Not Synced
for instance, by Franz Vollenweider,
-
Not Synced
we were thinking we were going to be seeing
-
Not Synced
increases in brain activity or brain blood flow
-
Not Synced
with psilocybin.
-
Not Synced
And despite dropping the threshold
-
Not Synced
and a number of different things,
-
Not Synced
we really didn't see this.
-
Not Synced
All we were really seeing was
-
Not Synced
the same pattern again and again,
-
Not Synced
which was decreased blood flow
-
Not Synced
in certain regions of the brain.
-
Not Synced
What was intriguing was that
-
Not Synced
the decreases that we were seeing were
-
Not Synced
in these very same important hub strutures
-
Not Synced
of the brain.
-
Not Synced
So, for instance, the posterior cingulate cortex,
-
Not Synced
this bit at the back,
-
Not Synced
the thalamus,
-
Not Synced
and the medial pre-frontal cortex --
-
Not Synced
so these very reliably
-
Not Synced
were coming up as being
-
Not Synced
decreased under psilocybin.
-
Not Synced
Here's just showing you again,
-
Not Synced
the default mode network is this kind of
-
Not Synced
hub, this connectivity hub, in the brain.
-
Not Synced
We also found that
-
Not Synced
there was a relationship between
-
Not Synced
the magnitude of the decrease in blood flow
-
Not Synced
and ratings of the intensity of the experience.
-
Not Synced
So, the larger the decrease in blood flow,
-
Not Synced
particularly in the anterior cingulate cortex,
-
Not Synced
the more intense people were describing
-
Not Synced
their experiences.
-
Not Synced
So whenever you find these relationships
-
Not Synced
it kind of reinforces your inferences, really,
-
Not Synced
and provides some consolidation
-
Not Synced
for what you are finding
-
Not Synced
and supports its functional meaning.
-
Not Synced
So since our ASL study
-
Not Synced
we did a [bold] study.
-
Not Synced
This is kind of the classic [signal]
-
Not Synced
of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI).
-
Not Synced
We really repeated the same protocol:
-
Not Synced
15 healthy volunteers,
-
Not Synced
infused with a drug over 60 seconds at 2 mg,
-
Not Synced
and we found exactly the same thing.
-
Not Synced
So this was really nice reinforcement
-
Not Synced
for the initial finding that we had found
-
Not Synced
with ASL.
-
Not Synced
And these regions where there was a common
-
Not Synced
overlap between the decreases in blood flow
-
Not Synced
with ASL and the decreases in,
-
Not Synced
essentially venous oxygenation,
-
Not Synced
or oxygenated blood with the BOLD signal
-
Not Synced
of fMRI.
-
Not Synced
So one of the merits of BOLD fMRI is
-
Not Synced
that it allows you to do these
-
Not Synced
functional connectivity analyses.
-
Not Synced
So just to give you a feel for what that is,
-
Not Synced
here is an image which shows
-
Not Synced
the default mode network in orange
-
Not Synced
and let's concentrate on the default
-
Not Synced
mode network for a moment.
-
Not Synced
So you can see that there are two regions in it,
-
Not Synced
and there is one that has has yellow text,
-
Not Synced
the PCC? and then you can see there is this
-
Not Synced
time series underneath.
-
Not Synced
So the PCC time series is in yellow
-
Not Synced
and you can see that it overlaps with
-
Not Synced
anothertime series, and that is
-
Not Synced
the medial prefontal cortex.
-
Not Synced
So it is by looking at correlations between
-
Not Synced
fluctuations in the BOLD signal
-
Not Synced
that we identify functionally coherent
-
Not Synced
brain networks. We know these regions
-
Not Synced
work together as a common network
-
Not Synced
doing a common function.