-
In a study in the 1990s,
-
participants recalled getting lost in a
shopping mall as children.
-
Some shared these memories
in vivid detail—
-
one even remembered that the old man
who rescued him
-
was wearing a flannel shirt.
-
But none of these people had actually
gotten lost in a mall.
-
They produced these false memories
-
when the psychologists conducting the
study told them they’d gotten lost,
-
and although they might not remember
the incident,
-
their parents had confirmed it.
-
And it wasn’t just one or two people
who thought they remembered getting lost—
-
a quarter of the participants did.
-
These findings may sound unbelievable,
-
but they actually reflect a very
common experience.
-
Our memories are sometimes unreliable.
-
And though we still don’t know precisely
what causes this fallibility
-
on a neurological level,
-
research has highlighted some of the
most common ways our memories
-
diverge from what actually happened.
-
The mall study highlights how we can
incorporate information
-
from outside sources,
-
like other people or the news,
-
into our personal recollections
without realizing it.
-
This kind of suggestibility is just
one influence on our memories.
-
Take another study,
-
in which researchers briefly showed a
random collection of photographs
-
to a group of participants,
-
including images of a university campus
none of them had ever visited.
-
When shown the images three weeks later,
-
a majority of participants said that
they had probably or definitely
-
visited the campus in the past.
-
The participants misattributed information
from one context––an image they’d seen—
-
onto another––a memory of something
they believed they actually experienced.
-
In another experiment, people were shown
an image of a magnifying glass,
-
and then told to imagine a lollipop.
-
They frequently recalled that they saw
the magnifying glass and the lollipop.
-
They struggled to link the objects to
the correct context––
-
whether they actually saw them,
or simply imagined them.
-
Another study, where a psychologist
questioned over 2,000 people
-
on their views about the legalization
of marijuana,
-
highlights yet another kind of influence
on memory.
-
Participants answered questions in 1973
and 1982.
-
Those who said they had supported
marijuana legalization in 1973,
-
but reported they were against it in 1982,
-
were more likely to recall that they were
actually against legalization in 1973––
-
bringing their old views in line
with their current ones.
-
Our current opinions,
feelings, and experiences
-
can bias our memories of
how we felt in the past.
-
In another study,
-
researchers gave two groups
of participants background information
-
on a historical war and asked them to rate
the likelihood that each side would win.
-
They gave each group the same information,
-
except that they only told one group
who had actually won the war—
-
the other group didn’t know the
real world outcome.
-
In theory, both groups’ answers should
be similar,
-
because the likelihood
of each side winning
-
isn’t effected by who actually won—
-
if there’s a 20% chance of thunderstorms,
and a thunderstorm happens,
-
the chance of thunderstorms
doesn’t retroactively go up to 100%.
-
Still, the group that knew how the war
ended
-
rated the winning side as more likely
to win than the group who did not.
-
All of these fallibilities of memory
can have real-world impacts.
-
If police interrogations use leading
questions with eye witnesses or suspects,
-
suggestibility could result in incorrect
identifications or unreliable confessions.
-
Even in the absence of leading questions,
-
misattribution can lead to inaccurate
eyewitness testimony.
-
In a courtroom,
-
if a judge rules a piece of evidence
inadmissible
-
and tells jurors to disregard it,
they may not be able to do so.
-
In a medical setting, if a patient
seeks a second opinion
-
and the second physician is aware of the
first one’s diagnosis,
-
that knowledge may bias their conclusion.
-
Our memories are not ironclad
representations of reality,
-
but subjective perceptions.
-
And there’s not necessarily
anything wrong with that—
-
the problems arise when we treat
memory as fact,
-
rather than accepting
this fundamental truth
-
about the nature of our recollections.