< Return to Video

Why do we believe things that aren't true? | Philip Fernbach | TEDxMileHigh

  • 0:13 - 0:15
    A few months ago,
  • 0:15 - 0:20
    the Internet exploded when a rapper
    named Bobby Ray Simmons,
  • 0:20 - 0:22
    aka B.o.B,
  • 0:22 - 0:27
    started twitting out reasons
    why he thought the world was flat.
  • 0:28 - 0:29
    (Laughter)
  • 0:30 - 0:32
    Now, the story really took off
  • 0:33 - 0:35
    when Neil deGrasse Tyson,
    the astrophysicist,
  • 0:35 - 0:39
    started twitting back at him,
    explaining the apparent discrepancies.
  • 0:39 - 0:41
    But guess what?
  • 0:41 - 0:44
    B.o.B held his ground.
  • 0:44 - 0:46
    (Laughter)
  • 0:47 - 0:48
    He didn't give in.
  • 0:49 - 0:53
    Now, it turns out
    that B.o.B is not the only one.
  • 0:54 - 0:58
    Believe it or not, there's
    actually a flat Earth society,
  • 0:59 - 1:02
    with roots going
    all the way back to the 1800s.
  • 1:03 - 1:06
    Their model is amazing.
  • 1:07 - 1:11
    "We man the guns
    against oppression of thought
  • 1:11 - 1:14
    and the Globularist lies of a new age."
  • 1:14 - 1:15
    (Laughter)
  • 1:15 - 1:18
    When I first read this,
    I thought it said "Globalist lies,"
  • 1:18 - 1:21
    but it's actually "Globularist,"
  • 1:21 - 1:25
    i.e. those nutty folks
    who think the Earth is a sphere.
  • 1:25 - 1:26
    (Laughter)
  • 1:27 - 1:29
    "Standing with reason,
  • 1:29 - 1:32
    we offer a home to those wayward thinkers
  • 1:32 - 1:35
    who march bravely on with reason and truth
  • 1:35 - 1:40
    in recognizing the true
    shape of the Earth."
  • 1:40 - 1:42
    Flat!
  • 1:42 - 1:43
    (Laughter)
  • 1:43 - 1:45
    This is not some elaborate hose.
  • 1:46 - 1:49
    B.o.B and the flat-earthers
    really believe that the Earth is flat,
  • 1:49 - 1:51
    despite all evidence to the contrary.
  • 1:52 - 1:55
    So, why am I showing you this?
  • 1:55 - 1:59
    Because your natural reaction
    to this story is wrong!
  • 1:59 - 2:03
    Your first instinct is to laugh
    at the flat-earthers
  • 2:03 - 2:06
    and assume they must be
    incredibly dense or crazy,
  • 2:06 - 2:10
    but actually, they're not all
    that different than you and I.
  • 2:11 - 2:14
    As human beings,
    false belief is our birth right.
  • 2:14 - 2:17
    It stands from fundamental principles
  • 2:17 - 2:20
    that govern the way our minds work
    and the way we store knowledge.
  • 2:21 - 2:22
    Consider how common it is
  • 2:22 - 2:26
    for groups of people to believe things
    that just aren't true.
  • 2:26 - 2:28
    Right now, in this moment,
  • 2:28 - 2:31
    it feels like we're
    in the midst of an epidemic.
  • 2:31 - 2:34
    The explosion of fake news
  • 2:34 - 2:37
    shows how easy it is to do people
    on the left and the right,
  • 2:37 - 2:39
    and science denial has gone mainstream.
  • 2:40 - 2:42
    Significant proportions of the population
  • 2:42 - 2:45
    maintain beliefs counter
    to the scientific consensus
  • 2:45 - 2:49
    on critical issues
    like vaccination, global warming
  • 2:49 - 2:51
    and the safety of genetically
    modified foods.
  • 2:52 - 2:55
    Public attitudes about these issues
    literally determine
  • 2:55 - 2:58
    whether we can feed ourselves,
    whether we can raise healthy children
  • 2:58 - 3:01
    and whether we can forestall
    a climate disaster.
  • 3:01 - 3:03
    The stakes could not be higher,
  • 3:04 - 3:05
    which is why it's just not good enough
  • 3:05 - 3:08
    to chalk all this up
    to lunacy or stupidity.
  • 3:08 - 3:12
    Simplistic explanations like that
    aren't getting us anywhere.
  • 3:12 - 3:15
    If we really want to improve
    the way we grapple with these challenges,
  • 3:15 - 3:17
    we have to go deeper,
  • 3:17 - 3:20
    we have to understand
    what it is about the way we think
  • 3:20 - 3:24
    that makes us so susceptible
    to believing things that aren't true.
  • 3:24 - 3:30
    And that explanation actually begins
    with a kind of shocking observation.
  • 3:32 - 3:33
    As individuals,
  • 3:34 - 3:39
    we do not know enough
    to justify almost anything we believe.
  • 3:40 - 3:42
    Now, I know that might
    sound crazy to you,
  • 3:42 - 3:45
    but let's think about a couple
    of really obvious facts.
  • 3:45 - 3:48
    We all believe that the Earth
    revolves around the Sun.
  • 3:48 - 3:51
    Of course we do, it's the most
    basic fact in the world.
  • 3:51 - 3:53
    But on what basis?
  • 3:53 - 3:57
    Can you explain the astronomical
    observations that support that belief?
  • 3:57 - 3:59
    I know I can't.
  • 4:00 - 4:01
    What about smoking?
  • 4:01 - 4:04
    We all know it's terrible for us, right?
  • 4:04 - 4:06
    But what's actually
    in cigarette smoke that's bad?
  • 4:06 - 4:09
    And what does it do
    to our bodies and ourselves?
  • 4:09 - 4:12
    What's cancer really?
    How does it even form?
  • 4:13 - 4:16
    These are not isolated examples.
  • 4:16 - 4:20
    Most of what we believe is not based
    on what's in our heads,
  • 4:21 - 4:23
    and there's a good reason for that.
  • 4:23 - 4:25
    There's not much in our heads!
  • 4:25 - 4:27
    (Laughter)
  • 4:27 - 4:29
    As human beings,
  • 4:29 - 4:33
    we are just not made
    to store a lot of detailed information.
  • 4:34 - 4:40
    In the 1980s, a psychologist named
    Thomas Landauer set out to estimate
  • 4:40 - 4:42
    the size of an individual's
    knowledge base in bytes,
  • 4:42 - 4:46
    the same scale that's used
    to measure computer memories.
  • 4:46 - 4:50
    One approach he took was to analyze
    the result of memory experiments
  • 4:50 - 4:54
    where people were asked to study
    some pictures, or words, or bits of music,
  • 4:54 - 4:57
    and then later test it
    to see if they recognized them.
  • 4:57 - 5:02
    Using the data, he was able to estimate
    the rate at which we can acquire knowledge
  • 5:02 - 5:06
    and also the rate at which
    we forget what we learn.
  • 5:06 - 5:10
    And then, he extrapolated
    to a 70-year lifespan.
  • 5:10 - 5:13
    So, how much do you know?
  • 5:14 - 5:16
    Landauer's estimate:
  • 5:16 - 5:18
    1 gigabyte.
  • 5:19 - 5:20
    (Laughter)
  • 5:21 - 5:24
    I think this is an amazing result,
  • 5:24 - 5:26
    mind-blowing really.
  • 5:26 - 5:28
    One gigabyte is a tiny amount!
  • 5:29 - 5:30
    By comparison,
  • 5:30 - 5:35
    you can buy a thumb drive
    on Amazon.com for less than 18 bucks,
  • 5:35 - 5:38
    that holds 64 gigabytes.
  • 5:38 - 5:39
    (Laughter)
  • 5:40 - 5:44
    Now, at this point, some of you guys
    might be freaking out a little bit,
  • 5:44 - 5:46
    feeling a little bit concerned.
  • 5:46 - 5:50
    After all, we all think it's the most
    important thing in the world
  • 5:50 - 5:53
    to know a lot and have great memories.
  • 5:53 - 5:56
    But really, this is a misconception.
  • 5:56 - 5:58
    We do not have to know a lot
  • 5:58 - 6:01
    because we're not made
    to think on our own.
  • 6:02 - 6:06
    It's natural to think about thinking
    as what happens between your ears,
  • 6:06 - 6:09
    but that's not where
    the magic really happens.
  • 6:09 - 6:14
    This video comes from a psychologist
    by the name of Michael Tomasello
  • 6:14 - 6:16
    and his colleagues.
  • 6:16 - 6:20
    They study human children's
    cognitive abilities
  • 6:20 - 6:23
    in comparison to other animals
    like chimpanzees.
  • 6:24 - 6:28
    The goal is to understand
    what really makes us special.
  • 6:28 - 6:33
    What abilities do we excel at
    that other animals just cannot master?
  • 6:33 - 6:37
    You see how easily this young child
    reads the mind of the experimenter
  • 6:37 - 6:41
    and then figures out how to coordinate
    his behavior to achieve the goal.
  • 6:41 - 6:43
    (Laughter)
  • 6:43 - 6:47
    He even makes eye contact at the end
    as if to say, "I've got your back, man!"
  • 6:47 - 6:48
    (Laughter)
  • 6:48 - 6:51
    This is so natural to us
    that it seems like nothing,
  • 6:51 - 6:54
    but it's actually incredibly difficult
  • 6:54 - 6:58
    to design a cognitive system
    that's capable of collaboration.
  • 6:58 - 7:00
    This is really the secret to our success,
  • 7:00 - 7:04
    it's what separates us
    from all other thinking creatures.
  • 7:04 - 7:08
    Chimpanzees routinely fail at tasks
  • 7:08 - 7:11
    that require sharing knowledge
    and working together to pursue goals,
  • 7:11 - 7:16
    tasks that young children
    master with ease.
  • 7:16 - 7:18
    Now, for me,
  • 7:20 - 7:22
    this realization was a major wake-up call.
  • 7:22 - 7:26
    It really changed my perspective
    on the nature of the mind.
  • 7:26 - 7:28
    I'm a cognitive scientist.
  • 7:29 - 7:34
    I'm used to studying how individuals make
    decisions or solve problems in isolation.
  • 7:35 - 7:38
    But thinking is a social process.
  • 7:38 - 7:40
    Rather than happening inside your head,
  • 7:40 - 7:44
    it emerges from your interactions
    with those around you.
  • 7:45 - 7:49
    People are a little more like bees
    than we often realize.
  • 7:49 - 7:52
    In a beehive, you have an incredibly
    complex cluster of behaviors
  • 7:52 - 7:57
    that is achieved despite no individual
    being responsible for it all.
  • 7:57 - 7:59
    Food is collected and stored,
  • 7:59 - 8:01
    the hive is protected from intruders,
  • 8:01 - 8:03
    genetic diversity is introduced.
  • 8:03 - 8:05
    The key is specialization:
  • 8:05 - 8:09
    each individual does its own little part,
    and the complexity emerges.
  • 8:10 - 8:12
    The same is true of people.
  • 8:12 - 8:15
    On our own, none of us
    knows all that much,
  • 8:15 - 8:17
    we don't have to.
  • 8:17 - 8:20
    We each have our own
    little slice of expertise,
  • 8:20 - 8:24
    and our minds are built to collaborate
    and to share knowledge,
  • 8:24 - 8:28
    which allows us to pursue
    incredibly complex goals
  • 8:28 - 8:32
    when none of us has anything approaching
    the knowledge to understand it all.
  • 8:33 - 8:35
    This is the Milan Cathedral.
  • 8:36 - 8:39
    It's one of humanity's great works.
  • 8:40 - 8:42
    Construction began in 1386,
  • 8:43 - 8:49
    and the facade was completed,
    get this, under Napoleon, in the 1800s.
  • 8:50 - 8:53
    It turns out that cathedrals
    have a punch list,
  • 8:53 - 8:55
    like a home renovation.
  • 8:56 - 8:58
    The punch list was completed
  • 8:59 - 9:02
    when they consecrated
    the final gate in the 1960s.
  • 9:02 - 9:05
    Six hundred years.
  • 9:05 - 9:06
    In that time,
  • 9:06 - 9:11
    there were 75 chief engineers
    responsible for the project
  • 9:11 - 9:14
    and thousands upon thousands
    of people involved.
  • 9:14 - 9:16
    None of those people had anything
  • 9:16 - 9:19
    remotely approaching the knowledge
    to understand it all,
  • 9:19 - 9:20
    not even close.
  • 9:21 - 9:23
    Everything great we do as human beings
  • 9:23 - 9:27
    depends on this ability to share
    knowledge and to collaborate.
  • 9:28 - 9:32
    So, that's the positive side
    of the knowledge-sharing story.
  • 9:32 - 9:36
    When we put our minds together,
    we can do incredible things.
  • 9:37 - 9:39
    But there's also a dark side.
  • 9:40 - 9:45
    Because we are built to so seamlessly
    draw on knowledge outside of our heads,
  • 9:45 - 9:50
    we often fail to realize the limits
    of our own understanding.
  • 9:50 - 9:54
    Let me tell you about a study that
    my colleague Steven Sloman recently ran.
  • 9:55 - 9:59
    He told his study participants
    about some new scientific discoveries
  • 9:59 - 10:01
    that he completely made up.
  • 10:01 - 10:03
    For instance, a kind of glowing rock.
  • 10:04 - 10:06
    He told one group of people
  • 10:06 - 10:10
    that scientists had not yet
    explained why the rocks glow,
  • 10:10 - 10:13
    and then he asked them,
    "How well do you understand?"
  • 10:13 - 10:16
    Unsurprisingly, they said
    they had no clue!
  • 10:16 - 10:20
    This makes perfect sense,
    they knew nothing about the rocks.
  • 10:20 - 10:22
    The more surprising result
    is what happened
  • 10:22 - 10:25
    when he told a different group of people
    about the same discovery,
  • 10:25 - 10:27
    but this time he told them
  • 10:27 - 10:30
    that scientists had explained
    exactly how the rocks glowed.
  • 10:30 - 10:36
    Now the participants claimed to understand
    the rocks a little bit better themselves,
  • 10:36 - 10:37
    which is kind of weird
  • 10:37 - 10:40
    because just like the other group,
    they knew nothing about the rocks.
  • 10:40 - 10:44
    It was as if the scientists' knowledge
    had been directly transmitted to them,
  • 10:44 - 10:47
    even though it was never described.
  • 10:47 - 10:52
    And it turns out that a similar thing
    happens when you surf the Internet.
  • 10:52 - 10:55
    Just having access
    to all of that information
  • 10:55 - 10:58
    makes you feel like you know
    a lot more than you do.
  • 10:59 - 11:03
    This sense of understanding is contagious.
  • 11:03 - 11:05
    (Laughter)
  • 11:05 - 11:10
    And when contagious understanding
    is paired with individual ignorance,
  • 11:10 - 11:12
    it can be a toxic recipe.
  • 11:12 - 11:14
    (Laughter)
  • 11:15 - 11:18
    The danger is that I may
    express a strong belief
  • 11:18 - 11:20
    because I feel like I understand.
  • 11:20 - 11:22
    But my sense of understanding is false.
  • 11:22 - 11:25
    It comes from those around me,
    expressing strong beliefs,
  • 11:25 - 11:28
    because they feel like they understand.
  • 11:29 - 11:33
    But their sense of understanding
    comes from those around them and so on.
  • 11:33 - 11:38
    Individually, none of us knows enough
    to tell what's true and what's false.
  • 11:38 - 11:42
    And yet, because we feel
    like we're on firm ground,
  • 11:42 - 11:44
    we don't do enough to verify,
  • 11:44 - 11:49
    and that is how entire groups of people
    can come to believe things
  • 11:49 - 11:51
    that aren't true.
  • 11:51 - 11:57
    We can build cathedrals,
    but we can also build houses of cards.
  • 11:59 - 12:03
    Now, the real tragedy occurs
    in how we relate to people
  • 12:03 - 12:05
    who have different beliefs than us.
  • 12:05 - 12:07
    We live in the illusion
  • 12:07 - 12:11
    that we have arrived at out own positions
    via a serious analysis
  • 12:11 - 12:15
    and that we can support and justify
    what we believe based on what we know.
  • 12:15 - 12:18
    Therefore, when someone
    doesn't believe what we believe,
  • 12:18 - 12:20
    it's obvious what the problem is:
  • 12:20 - 12:23
    "They're too stupid to see the truth!"
  • 12:23 - 12:24
    (Laughter)
  • 12:24 - 12:27
    And there's actually a sense
    in which you're right when you think that.
  • 12:27 - 12:28
    It's true!
  • 12:28 - 12:31
    They did not arrive at their position
  • 12:31 - 12:34
    via a rational process
    of evidence evaluation
  • 12:34 - 12:37
    and they don't understand
    the issue in depth.
  • 12:37 - 12:39
    But neither do you!
  • 12:39 - 12:41
    (Laughter)
  • 12:41 - 12:44
    Think about how we talk
    about a complex issue like healthcare.
  • 12:44 - 12:46
    If you're a liberal,
  • 12:46 - 12:49
    the Affordable Care Act
    is the bee's knees.
  • 12:49 - 12:54
    If your a conservative,
    "It's destroying America."
  • 12:54 - 12:55
    But most of the time,
  • 12:55 - 12:59
    arguments about the policy's merits
    amount to little more
  • 12:59 - 13:03
    than repetition of sound bites
    that we heard from someone else.
  • 13:03 - 13:04
    As non-experts,
  • 13:04 - 13:09
    we can't possibly do justice
    to the complexity of an issue like that.
  • 13:09 - 13:11
    When we express our beliefs,
  • 13:11 - 13:16
    we are all just channeling
    our communities of knowledge.
  • 13:16 - 13:17
    That's what we do.
  • 13:18 - 13:21
    Knowledge is not in my head
    and it's not in your head.
  • 13:21 - 13:23
    Knowledge is shared,
  • 13:23 - 13:26
    and therefore, the things
    that you really care about,
  • 13:26 - 13:28
    those things are shared too.
  • 13:28 - 13:34
    Now, the point is decidedly
    not that people are stupid.
  • 13:35 - 13:38
    It's true, we are all ignorant,
  • 13:38 - 13:40
    but that's not something
    we should hide from.
  • 13:40 - 13:45
    The world is far too complex for anyone
    of us to understand much of it.
  • 13:45 - 13:50
    What makes us special is the ability
    to thrive amidst that complexity
  • 13:50 - 13:51
    by sharing knowledge.
  • 13:51 - 13:57
    From our individual ignorance
    can arise collective genius.
  • 13:57 - 14:02
    Ignorance is a feature
    of the human mind, not a bug,
  • 14:04 - 14:08
    but we don't have to be so darn sure
    about things we don't understand.
  • 14:09 - 14:10
    Of course,
  • 14:10 - 14:14
    we have to take positions on issues
    without knowing everything about them,
  • 14:14 - 14:18
    and if we have good sources
    of expertise in our communities
  • 14:18 - 14:20
    and a culture that values truth,
  • 14:20 - 14:22
    we'll get things right
    more often than not.
  • 14:23 - 14:28
    But when we go through life feeling like
    we individually have it all figured out,
  • 14:29 - 14:33
    it can lead to a warped
    and simplistic view of the world:
  • 14:33 - 14:38
    "My way is perfect.
    Yours is crazy, or evil."
  • 14:38 - 14:39
    In reality,
  • 14:40 - 14:45
    most issues are complicated
    and most people have good intentions.
  • 14:46 - 14:48
    Okay, now the bad news:
  • 14:48 - 14:51
    we can't eradicate false belief.
  • 14:53 - 14:55
    It's too basic to the way that we think.
  • 14:56 - 15:00
    What we can do is practice
    a little more intellectual humility,
  • 15:01 - 15:03
    to open our minds to the possibility
  • 15:03 - 15:10
    that some of those false beliefs
    probably reside in our own communities.
  • 15:11 - 15:14
    We have a tremendous opportunity,
  • 15:14 - 15:18
    an opportunity to improve
    the quality of our discourse
  • 15:19 - 15:22
    by recognizing the limits
    of our understanding
  • 15:22 - 15:24
    and by appreciating
  • 15:24 - 15:30
    just how much of what we believe
    depends on those around us.
  • 15:31 - 15:32
    Thank you.
  • 15:32 - 15:34
    (Applause)
Title:
Why do we believe things that aren't true? | Philip Fernbach | TEDxMileHigh
Description:

It seems like we're living in an epidemic of false belief. Clearly the other side just doesn’t have all the facts, right? Or are they really that stupid? In this fascinating and hilarious talk, cognitive scientist Philip Fernbach peels back the layers of what we really know and reveals some surprising truths about the human mind.

Philip Fernbach is a cognitive scientist and professor in the Leeds School of Business at the University of Colorado, Boulder. Co-author of "The Knowledge Illusion: Why We Never Think Alone," Philip's research focuses on why we think we know more than we actually do and the implications this has on individuals and society. He lives in Boulder with his wife and two children. In his free time, he plays bluegrass music and ice hockey.

This talk was given at a TEDx event using the TED conference format but independently organized by a local community. Learn more at https://www.ted.com/tedx

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDxTalks
Duration:
15:51

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions