-
[no audio yet]
-
In this video, we'll differentiate
-
between stereotypes, prejudice,
and discrimination;
-
and we'll discuss several important
social psychological concepts
-
and hypotheses related to each,
-
including what causes them
to arise in the first place.
-
Let's go over a bit of
terminology to kick things off.
-
A stereotype is a belief
(which can be positive or negative )
-
about the characteristics
of members of a group
-
that is applied generally
to most members of that group.
-
Believing that Asians are good
at math, for example, is positive;
-
it's not necessarily derogatory,
-
but it's nonetheless a stereotype
that you have about Asians.
-
Now, stereotypes (these beliefs) can
lead to prejudice, which in contrast,
-
can only ever be negative.
-
Prejudice involves drawing
negative conclusions about
-
a person, a group of people,
-
or a situation prior to
evaluating the evidence.
-
These baseless conclusions are typically
-
the result of those stereotypes
that you hold about that group.
-
Also, in contrast to stereotypes,
-
prejudice involves emotion;
it’s an attitude.
-
Being prejudiced against a person
or a group of people involves
-
feeling negatively toward them.
-
Now, because of these negative emotions
-
and these negative conclusions
that you're coming to,
-
prejudice often leads to discrimination,
-
which is negative behavior
towards members of an out-group.
-
And by the way, an out-group is a group
-
that we don't belong to or one that we
view as fundamentally different from us;
-
whereas an in-group, in contrast,
-
refers to a group that we DO identify
with or see ourselves as belonging to.
-
So I might be using that
terminology quite a bit–
-
important to know.
-
So just to summarize,
-
stereotypes are beliefs,
prejudice is an attitude,
-
and discrimination is a behavior.
-
Let's go over an example
that puts all of this together.
-
Let's say, for example, that you
believe older adults are incompetent,
-
and that's a stereotype that
you have about older adults.
-
(And I'll note that I'm not endorsing
this stereotype or any other stereotype
-
that I use as an example in this video,
-
but we have to have some kind
of an example to work with here.)
-
So let's say you work at,
I don't know, a tech company
-
and you're looking to hire an assistant.
-
If an elderly gentleman applies,
-
you might walk into that interview
with the gentleman,
-
assuming he won't be a good fit
or that he'd be difficult to train.
-
Now, we would call this
premature conclusion;
-
this negative attitude toward
this gentleman [is] prejudice.
-
Finally, you may decide not
to hire the gentleman at all
-
because of your stereotype,
because of your prejudice.
-
In this case, the behavior
of not hiring him
-
would be discrimination.
-
Now, stereotypes and prejudice
can be either explicit
-
(meaning, we're consciously
aware of having this bias)
-
or implicit (meaning, it's there,
but we aren't aware of it).
-
Research shows that explicit prejudice
is in decline, which is encouraging;
-
however, implicit prejudice
really isn't much.
-
That is, people report being
very anti-bias nowadays,
-
but their behavior still
tells us a different story.
-
Let's take a look at a few
examples to illustrate.
-
Starting with the realm of gender,
we can look to some of my own data.
-
In one study, I searched through
-
the language used by students
evaluating their teachers
-
in over 14 million reviews posted to
a popular instructor evaluation website,
-
RateMyProfessors.com,
which you've perhaps used in the past.
-
I was specifically interested in
stereotypes about intelligence,
-
so I searched through uses of
the words “genius” and “brilliant.”
-
So let's take a look at the results.
There's a lot of information here.
-
Let me help you interpret these graphs.
-
These are graphs for uses of
-
the words “genius” on the left
and “brilliant” on the right.
-
The x-axis on both of these graphs
-
represents uses per millions
of words of text,
-
which might sound a little
complicated, but really isn't.
-
There's a ton of text here,
-
so to keep the numbers on the
x-axis from being enormous
-
and just visually unappealing,
-
I used this uses per millions of words of text,
but the interpretation is basically the same.
-
The further to the right
you go on the x-axis
-
(the higher the number),
the more this word was used.
-
So that's how you can interpret that.
-
The y-axis here displays all of the
different fields such as philosophy,
-
music, mathematics, psychology;
-
so you can look for your own field
-
or just pause the video and look through
them in general, if you're curious,
-
And fields that are higher up
on the y-axis were the ones
-
in which the words were used the most often.
-
The blue dots here on the slide
represent reviews of male professors,
-
whereas the orange dots represent
reviews of female professors.
-
Before I give you the punch line,
what do you notice here?
-
Well, what I found is that every
field for which we have data,
-
students describe their male
professors as genius and brilliant
-
significantly more often than
they do their female professors.
-
And in no field was this effect reversed,
-
even for fields where women
were the statistical majority.
-
And this points to a stereotype
in favor of men's intelligence
-
and against women's intelligence.
-
You might be wondering:
-
Does this reflect an overall
bias against women,
-
or is the stereotype specific
to intellectual ability?
-
Well, I was curious about this as well,
-
but if you look at the data for
the terms “excellent” and “amazing,”
-
the gender bias goes away entirely.
-
It appears that students believe
-
that their female professors
can be excellent and amazing,
-
but they believe it's mainly the male
professors who are genius and brilliant.
-
Again, this is evidence of implicit bias
-
because students are likely
-
not consciously aware
of this discrepancy.
-
They're simply going on line
to review their professors
-
and they're not giving their
stereotypes any thought.
-
So explicitly, students would
likely say they don't hold a bias,
-
yet implicitly, they respond in this way.
-
This is a common theme
-
in modern research on stereotypes,
prejudice, and discrimination.
-
Now that's gender.
What about race?
-
One study found that doctors
were only 60% as likely to suggest
-
a top-rated diagnostic test
for Black heart patients
-
than for White heart patients.
-
There's also evidence to suggest
-
that White men are offered
greater financial opportunities.
-
As one example, a study found
that White men were offered
-
the best deals at used car dealerships.
-
White men paid $109 on average
less than White women,
-
$318 less than Black women,
-
and a whopping $935 less for a
used car on average than Black men.
-
Now, these are just two examples out
of thousands that I could tell you about,
-
but again, it's likely the case
-
that these doctors and car salesmen
aren't EXPLICITLY biased,
-
but their behavior provides
evidence of IMPLICIT bias.
-
Okay, so let's finish
with a brief discussion
-
of what leads to the development and
perpetuation of some of these things
-
(stereotypes, prejudice,
and discrimination),
-
starting with stereotypes.
-
A factor that we've learned about
before is confirmation bias,
-
the tendency to seek out evidence
that supports our beliefs
-
and to deny, dismiss, or distort
evidence that contradicts them.
-
Say, for example that you believe
women to be bad drivers.
-
If you're out driving for an hour,
-
you might encounter several bad
drivers, some male, some female.
-
If you don't have a stereotype
against male drivers, though,
-
you might not think much of them
when they speed or make dangerous moves.
-
But the second a female driver
cuts you off, for example,
-
you feel vindicated as though
you've found additional evidence
-
or proof for your belief.
-
And this reinforces your stereotype
-
even though, in truth, many people are
bad drivers regardless of their gender.
-
Now, if we used System 2 thinking
-
(which we've learned
about in a previous video)
-
to evaluate these kinds of assumptions
and the data that we base them on,
-
we might realize that those assumptions
are erroneous, but we usually don't.
-
This is because we are cognitive misers.
-
That is, we seek to use only
minimal cognitive resources
-
when explaining the world around us.
-
Evaluating our stereotypes takes effort,
-
and because we generally
don't go to more effort
-
than we deem absolutely necessary,
-
we don't evaluate or re-evaluate them at all.
-
Now, what causes prejudice?
-
First, we have in-group bias,
which refers to the tendency
-
to favor individuals from within our
group over those from outside our group.
-
Evidence from developmental
psychology suggests that this bias
-
is innate, with young infants showing
strong preferences, for example,
-
for others who share their preferences
(such as their favorite snack)
-
and infants disliking others who
do not share their preferences
-
(for example, if the other person shows
that they like a different snack more).
-
Think of the implications
for racism, sexism, and so on.
-
Another factor is called
the ultimate attribution error,
-
which refers to the assumption
-
that behaviors among
individual members of a group
-
are due to their internal dispositions.
-
Out-group members’ flaws
are due to internal factors
-
such as their personality or their race,
whereas in-group members flaws aren't.
-
This might sound a lot like
the fundamental attribution error,
-
which we've learned about before,
but it is a bit different.
-
Think of the ultimate attribution error
-
as more of a narrow case of
the fundamental attribution error
-
applied specifically to attributions
-
about an individual in relation to
the group to which they belong.
-
Along similar lines,
-
out-group homogeneity
refers to the tendency to view
-
all individuals outside our group
as highly similar to one another.
-
Here, think of the implications
-
for identifying a suspect in
a police lineup, for example,
-
but also consider this bias in relation
to the ultimate attribution error.
-
It's a very bad combination to assume
-
that out-group members flaws
are due to inherent factors
-
such as their personalities or their race,
-
and to simultaneously assume
-
that out-group members are all
highly similar to one another.
-
Finally, scapegoating refers to
the act of blaming an out-group
-
when the in-group experiences frustration or
is blocked from obtaining some kind of a goal.
-
People scapegoat because it preserves
a positive self-concept.
-
If you believe the reason you can't get a job
is because immigrants are taking them all,
-
well, then you don't have to
come to terms with the reality
-
that you simply aren't qualified or
competent enough for that line of work.
-
Now, this list of causes here
is by no means all-inclusive
-
but should give you a good idea of
the general psychological phenomena
-
that lead to the formation and
perpetuation of stereotypes,
-
prejudice, and discrimination. [END]