Return to Video

What do all languages have in common? - Cameron Morin

  • 0:07 - 0:10
    Language is endlessly variable.
  • 0:10 - 0:13
    Each of us can come up with an infinite
    number of sentences
  • 0:13 - 0:14
    in our native language,
  • 0:14 - 0:17
    and we’re able to do so from an early age—
  • 0:17 - 0:20
    almost as soon as we start to communicate
    in sentences.
  • 0:20 - 0:22
    How is this possible?
  • 0:22 - 0:26
    In the early 1950s, Noam Chomsky proposed
    a theory
  • 0:26 - 0:31
    based on the observation that the key to
    this versatility seems to be grammar:
  • 0:31 - 0:35
    the familiar grammatical structure of an
    unfamiliar sentence
  • 0:35 - 0:37
    points us toward its meaning.
  • 0:37 - 0:42
    He suggested that there are grammatical
    rules that apply to all languages,
  • 0:42 - 0:45
    and that the rules are innate—
  • 0:45 - 0:50
    the human brain is hardwired to process
    language according to these rules.
  • 0:50 - 0:53
    He labelled this faculty
    Universal Grammar,
  • 0:53 - 0:57
    and it launched lines of inquiry that
    shaped both the field of linguistics
  • 0:57 - 1:02
    and the emerging field of cognitive
    science for decades to come.
  • 1:02 - 1:05
    Chomsky and other researchers
    set out to investigate
  • 1:05 - 1:08
    the two main components
    of universal grammar:
  • 1:08 - 1:13
    first, whether there are, in fact, grammar
    rules that are universal to all languages,
  • 1:13 - 1:18
    and, second, whether these rules are
    hardwired in the brain.
  • 1:18 - 1:21
    In attempts to establish the universal
    rules of grammar,
  • 1:21 - 1:26
    Chomsky developed an analytical tool
    known as generative syntax,
  • 1:26 - 1:32
    which represents the order of words in a
    sentence in hierarchical syntax trees
  • 1:32 - 1:34
    that show what structures are possible.
  • 1:34 - 1:38
    Based on this tree, we could suggest a
    grammar rule
  • 1:38 - 1:41
    that adverbs must occur in verb phrases.
  • 1:41 - 1:44
    But with more data, it quickly becomes
    clear
  • 1:44 - 1:47
    that adverbs can appear
    outside of verb phrases.
  • 1:47 - 1:51
    This simplified example illustrates a
    major problem:
  • 1:51 - 1:55
    it takes a lot of data from each
    individual language
  • 1:55 - 1:57
    to establish the rules for that language,
  • 1:57 - 1:59
    before we can even begin to determine
  • 1:59 - 2:03
    which rules all languages might
    have in common.
  • 2:03 - 2:06
    When Chomsky proposed universal grammar,
  • 2:06 - 2:10
    many languages lacked the volume of
    recorded samples necessary
  • 2:10 - 2:12
    to analyze them using generative syntax.
  • 2:12 - 2:14
    Even with lots of data,
  • 2:14 - 2:18
    mapping the structure of a language
    is incredibly complex.
  • 2:18 - 2:24
    After 50 years of analysis, we still
    haven’t completely figured out English.
  • 2:24 - 2:27
    As more linguist data was gathered
    and analyzed,
  • 2:27 - 2:31
    it became clear that languages around the
    world differ widely,
  • 2:31 - 2:35
    challenging the theory that there were
    universal grammar rules.
  • 2:35 - 2:39
    In the 1980s, Chomsky revised his theory
  • 2:39 - 2:41
    in an attempt to accommodate
    this variation.
  • 2:41 - 2:46
    According to his new hypothesis of
    principles and parameters,
  • 2:46 - 2:49
    all languages shared certain grammatical
    principles,
  • 2:49 - 2:53
    but could vary in their parameters,
    or the application of these principles.
  • 2:53 - 2:58
    For example, a principle is “every
    sentence must have a subject,"
  • 2:58 - 3:02
    but the parameter of whether the subject
    must be explicitly stated
  • 3:02 - 3:04
    could vary between languages.
  • 3:04 - 3:06
    The hypothesis of principles and
    parameters
  • 3:06 - 3:11
    still didn’t answer the question of which
    grammatical principles are universal.
  • 3:11 - 3:16
    In the early 2000s, Chomsky suggested
    that there’s just one shared principle,
  • 3:16 - 3:22
    called recursion, which means structures
    can be nested inside each other.
  • 3:22 - 3:23
    Take this sentence,
  • 3:23 - 3:27
    which embeds a sentence within a sentence
    within a sentence.
  • 3:27 - 3:32
    Or this sentence, which embeds a noun
    phrase in a noun phrase in a noun phrase.
  • 3:32 - 3:36
    Recursion was a good candidate for a
    universal grammar rule
  • 3:36 - 3:38
    because it can take many forms.
  • 3:38 - 3:42
    However, in 2005 linguists published
    findings
  • 3:42 - 3:45
    on an Amazonian language called Piraha,
  • 3:45 - 3:49
    which doesn’t appear to have any
    recursive structures.
  • 3:49 - 3:52
    So what about the other part of
    Chomsky’s theory,
  • 3:52 - 3:55
    that our language faculty is innate?
  • 3:55 - 3:58
    When he first proposed Universal Grammar,
  • 3:58 - 4:02
    the idea that there was a genetically
    determined aspect of language acquisition
  • 4:02 - 4:05
    had a profound, revolutionary impact.
  • 4:05 - 4:10
    It challenged the dominant paradigm,
    called behaviorism.
  • 4:10 - 4:15
    Behaviorists argued that all animal and
    human behaviors, including language,
  • 4:15 - 4:21
    were acquired from the outside by the
    mind, which starts out as a blank slate.
  • 4:21 - 4:25
    Today, scientists agree that behaviorism
    was wrong,
  • 4:25 - 4:29
    and there is underlying, genetically
    encoded biological machinery
  • 4:29 - 4:30
    for language learning.
  • 4:30 - 4:33
    Many think the same biology responsible
    for language
  • 4:33 - 4:37
    is also responsible for other
    aspects of cognition.
  • 4:37 - 4:42
    So they disagree with Chomsky’s idea that
    there is a specific, isolated,
  • 4:42 - 4:45
    innate language faculty in the brain.
  • 4:45 - 4:49
    The theory of universal grammar
    prompted the documentation and study
  • 4:49 - 4:52
    of many languages that hadn’t
    been studied before.
  • 4:52 - 4:57
    It also caused an old idea to be
    reevaluated and eventually overthrown
  • 4:57 - 5:01
    to make room for our growing understanding
    of the human brain.
Title:
What do all languages have in common? - Cameron Morin
Speaker:
Cameron Morin
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TED-Ed
Duration:
05:02

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions