Return to Video

https:/.../2018-10-12_Expressive_Writing_pt2_edited_1.mp4

  • 0:06 - 0:09
    >> Hi, welcome back. We are here with
    Dr. Jamie Pennebaker,
  • 0:09 - 0:13
    and we're talking about expressive writing
    and welcome to our second hour.
  • 0:14 - 0:18
    We talked a lot about the research base
    and the history of kind of discovering
  • 0:18 - 0:21
    and figuring out that expressive writing
    works, especially to help people with
  • 0:21 - 0:26
    a variety of objective outcomes,
    including health measures,
  • 0:26 - 0:30
    job measures, and things related to that,
    and then we're going to go back
  • 0:30 - 0:34
    and talk more about how it is that
    we can think about expressive writing
  • 0:34 - 0:35
    most productively.
  • 0:36 - 0:38
    Well, welcome back Jamie.
    >> It's good to be back.
  • 0:38 - 0:40
    >> (laughing) Indeed.
  • 0:40 - 0:44
    So, when we're thinking about expressive
    writing, you know, one of the things
  • 0:44 - 0:47
    that I think about a lot here is sort of
    what's the magic ingredient,
  • 0:47 - 0:51
    what's the mechanism of treatment
    effect, do you think, for this process?
  • 0:52 - 0:58
    >> When the first studies were done,
    there were all of these hypotheses on
  • 0:58 - 1:03
    what it must be, and one of them,
    people were-- felt that
  • 1:03 - 1:05
    "Well, this must be some kind of
    catharsis or venting."
  • 1:05 - 1:10
    And all of our analyses and various
    studies showed that's not it at all,
  • 1:10 - 1:13
    it's a much more-- there are
    many things going on.
  • 1:13 - 1:20
    And the other thing-- science hates this,
    science wants a nice simple answer,
  • 1:20 - 1:24
    but spoilers--
    >> What clinicians are (crosstalk)?
  • 1:24 - 1:30
    >> Spoiler alert! There is not a silver
    bullet that explains it, that there are
  • 1:30 - 1:33
    multiple, multiple factors involved.
  • 1:33 - 1:37
    So for example, with expressive writing,
    we-- there have now been these
  • 1:37 - 1:44
    various studies that show first of all
    just labeling an upsetting experience
  • 1:44 - 1:45
    is important.
  • 1:45 - 1:49
    So there have been these studies
    looking at brain activity,
  • 1:49 - 1:53
    and if you can just label what happens,
    or just label your emotions,
  • 1:54 - 1:55
    that's beneficial.
  • 1:56 - 2:00
    A second is that writing about an
    experience helps to give it some
  • 2:00 - 2:02
    structure and organization.
  • 2:03 - 2:05
    So that too is really valuable.
  • 2:05 - 2:10
    Another related one is that expressive
    writing also helps to provide meaning
  • 2:10 - 2:11
    for a person.
  • 2:12 - 2:16
    And then-- and creating a narrative
    seems to be beneficial.
  • 2:17 - 2:22
    But another thing that I think is really
    central is that-- think about what happens
  • 2:22 - 2:26
    when you don't write, or you don't talk
    about an upsetting experience.
  • 2:27 - 2:31
    You're walking down the street, and you've
    had, just had a terrible interaction
  • 2:31 - 2:32
    with somebody.
  • 2:33 - 2:38
    What happens is in your mind, you think
    about "Ooh, I shouldn't have said that",
  • 2:38 - 2:42
    "Ooh, I can't believe that they said that"
    and then you go off in another direction.
  • 2:42 - 2:46
    Your thought patterns, very often after a
    trauma, in the days and weeks afterwards,
  • 2:47 - 2:50
    are random, they're just-- they are--
    >> Destroying you.
  • 2:51 - 2:52
    >> Destroying-- exactly.
    >> (inaudible).
  • 2:52 - 3:00
    >> And so what writing does, it forces you
    to bring together all of these aspects,
  • 3:00 - 3:05
    so you start to realize "Wow, this
    upsetting experience is associated
  • 3:05 - 3:09
    "with why I'm having problems
    with my spouse, this is associated with
  • 3:09 - 3:14
    "why I'm having sleep problems,
    this explains why I don't
  • 3:14 - 3:16
    "have any money in the bank right now,
    this..." And so you start putting--
  • 3:16 - 3:18
    >> Think of those connections
    we were talking about--
  • 3:18 - 3:20
    >> You're making connections that's right.
    >> You want people to be indicating, yeah.
  • 3:20 - 3:24
    >> So it's all of this. But then there
    have been all of these other studies
  • 3:24 - 3:28
    looking at expressive writing, and one
    was one that I particularly loved
  • 3:28 - 3:32
    that was looking at essentially
    working memory or executive function.
  • 3:33 - 3:40
    And working memory, as you'll remember,
    is this-- the idea of your ability to
  • 3:41 - 3:45
    keep multiple ideas in your head at once,
    it's a frontal lobe phenomenon.
  • 3:46 - 3:50
    And what this research found was that
    when people did expressive writing,
  • 3:50 - 3:54
    that they found enhancements,
    improved working memory,
  • 3:54 - 3:56
    in the days and weeks afterwards.
    >> Oh, yeah.
  • 3:56 - 4:01
    >> Now, having more working memory
    does a lot of things, and it's also
  • 4:01 - 4:07
    associated with... that's one reason
    I think, by the way, people do better
  • 4:07 - 4:09
    in school after writing.
    >> Sure, yeah,
  • 4:09 - 4:11
    better focus and concentration.
    >> That's right.
  • 4:11 - 4:13
    >> They can direct their thinking
    more effectively.
  • 4:13 - 4:15
    >> That's right. And there have been
    some studies showing that people
  • 4:15 - 4:20
    who are taking the SATs, or college boards
    of some kind, that if they do expressive
  • 4:20 - 4:23
    writing beforehand, they do actually
    better than if they don't.
  • 4:23 - 4:26
    And again, I think it's the
    working memory issue.
  • 4:27 - 4:30
    And there's another thing that's also
    relevant to working memory,
  • 4:30 - 4:34
    and a lot of other things, is after
    people write, they also sleep better.
  • 4:35 - 4:37
    >> Ah. Good.
    >> And think about
  • 4:37 - 4:38
    what we not know about sleep.
  • 4:38 - 4:42
    We know sleep is associated with
    reduction in depression, we know
  • 4:42 - 4:45
    that it's associated with enhanced
    immune functions, many of the things
  • 4:45 - 4:47
    that we know expressive
    writing is beneficial for.
  • 4:47 - 4:50
    >> Sure, it's the quality and quantity
    are related to these things, yeah.
  • 4:50 - 4:51
    >> That's exactly right.
  • 4:51 - 4:54
    So that's another factor
    that feeds into this.
  • 4:54 - 4:59
    But then there's one that I found also
    quite compelling. Years ago,
  • 4:59 - 5:08
    we did a study with college students,
    and what we were interested in was
  • 5:08 - 5:12
    does expressive writing
    change your daily behaviors?
  • 5:12 - 5:16
    Now, we had done studies and we
    gave people questionnaires,
  • 5:16 - 5:19
    and one thing in my research is I'm
    very suspicious of questionnaries,
  • 5:19 - 5:22
    and one of the questionnaire items
    we asked was, you know,
  • 5:22 - 5:25
    "To what degree over the last
    few weeks have you been able to
  • 5:25 - 5:26
    "connect with others?"
  • 5:27 - 5:30
    Nobody could answer that question,
    it's a stupid question.
  • 5:31 - 5:36
    I wanted, though, to find out can we
    get at that? So what we devised was a--
  • 5:37 - 5:42
    it's a method called the electronically
    activated reporter, the EAR, or the ear.
  • 5:43 - 5:47
    And it was essentially a glorified tape
    recorder that would come on for
  • 5:47 - 5:51
    30 seconds and go off for 12 minutes,
    and we had people wear this for two days
  • 5:51 - 5:53
    before they came in to a writing study.
  • 5:54 - 5:59
    And then we had them wear this again
    one month later, and what it does is
  • 5:59 - 6:01
    it just captures how much of the time
    you're talking with others,
  • 6:01 - 6:04
    the degree to which you are laughing,
    the kind of language that you are
  • 6:04 - 6:05
    using, so forth.
  • 6:06 - 6:09
    What we found was that people in the
    experimental conditions, people who
  • 6:09 - 6:11
    wrote about their deepest thoughts
    and feelings about something
  • 6:11 - 6:16
    ended up talking more with others,
    laughing more with others,
  • 6:16 - 6:18
    they were more connected with others.
  • 6:19 - 6:23
    And I think this gets back to
    executive function, or working memory,
  • 6:23 - 6:29
    that when you have an upsetting
    experience, you are thinking about it
  • 6:29 - 6:30
    all the time.
  • 6:30 - 6:34
    You are a worse friend, because you're
    just not paying attention to your friends,
  • 6:34 - 6:37
    you're not sleeping as well,
    you're not doing as well at school,
  • 6:37 - 6:39
    because you're distracted--
    >> You're distracted by all your thoughts.
  • 6:39 - 6:39
    >> That's exactly right.
  • 6:39 - 6:44
    And then what expressive writing does is
    it helps to put this upsetting experience
  • 6:44 - 6:48
    into some kind of context, and you
    just don't think about it as much.
  • 6:48 - 6:50
    And if you do think about it,
    it's not as upsetting.
  • 6:51 - 6:55
    And you sleep better, and you
    can connect with others better.
  • 6:55 - 6:56
    So why does it work?
  • 6:57 - 7:00
    I think all of those things in this
    cascade of things, all of those things
  • 7:00 - 7:02
    help to explain the effect.
    >> It's basically a form of
  • 7:02 - 7:05
    cognitive processing, clearly.
    >> It very much is. It is.
  • 7:05 - 7:08
    >> And that obviously has been
    shown in a lot of ways to be
  • 7:08 - 7:11
    beneficial to people in terms of
    putting traumating experiences
  • 7:11 - 7:14
    and other past experiences
    sort of in their place, right?
  • 7:14 - 7:15
    >> Yes, that's right.
    >> It kind of helps to
  • 7:15 - 7:17
    actually put them in the past,
    and they don't have to be
  • 7:17 - 7:19
    thinking about them so actively
    all the time anymore.
  • 7:19 - 7:21
    >> That's right, that's right,
    and a cognitive experience
  • 7:21 - 7:24
    is also an emotional experience,
    and this distinction between
  • 7:24 - 7:26
    cognition and emotion, I think,
    is somewhat false,
  • 7:26 - 7:32
    because the two are-- they're all
    part of the same phenomenon.
  • 7:32 - 7:34
    >> Yeah, clinically we end up making
    those distinctions a lot,
  • 7:34 - 7:37
    I think just mostly as a shorthand
    way to kind of make life easier
  • 7:37 - 7:40
    for us as clinicians sometimes,
    and kind of looking at
  • 7:40 - 7:42
    someone's emotional functioning
    and their cognitive functioning.
  • 7:43 - 7:46
    Obviously those are very intertwined
    in this way, and especially--
  • 7:46 - 7:50
    obviously, this is the primary basis of
    all CBT, cognitive behavioral therapy,
  • 7:50 - 7:53
    is that those things are very
    closely linked that way.
  • 7:53 - 7:56
    So this discussion about working memory
    kind of makes me wonder about
  • 7:56 - 8:01
    the timing of these interventions,
    and whether we have any kind of
  • 8:01 - 8:04
    indication of boundary conditions,
    having to do with when we would
  • 8:04 - 8:07
    recommend that we not
    use these techniques--
  • 8:07 - 8:15
    >> This is a critical question.
    And... so there are interventions,
  • 8:15 - 8:20
    and I think clinicians worry about this
    as well, if there is a major
  • 8:20 - 8:25
    trauma for a person, should you jump
    right in and start doing some kind of
  • 8:25 - 8:26
    therapy immediately?
  • 8:26 - 8:29
    >> Yes, this is very dicey to us.
    >> It is.
  • 8:29 - 8:32
    >> Given the evidence base these days.
    >> That's right, and the data shows
  • 8:32 - 8:38
    pretty clearly, therapy-- at least
    deep therapy immediately after trauma's
  • 8:38 - 8:40
    probably a bad idea.
  • 8:40 - 8:43
    And I think that includes writing.
    >> We want much more naturalistic
  • 8:43 - 8:45
    social support and that kind of
    thing during that time.
  • 8:45 - 8:46
    >> Yeah, that's exactly right.
  • 8:46 - 8:52
    And, you know, if you look at the data,
    and this is since I started
  • 8:52 - 8:55
    doing expressive writing, I've since
    moved into another world
  • 8:55 - 8:57
    doing analysis of language.
  • 8:57 - 8:58
    And look--
    >> We'll have to have you back
  • 8:58 - 8:59
    to talk about it.
    >> Exactly.
  • 8:59 - 9:04
    But looking at how people write
    in blogs after, say, 9/11,
  • 9:04 - 9:06
    or other traumas.
  • 9:06 - 9:12
    And what you find is that people who
    are able to distract themselves
  • 9:12 - 9:16
    actually do much better than people
    who are really obsessing about
  • 9:16 - 9:19
    this trauma immediately afterwards.
    >> Digging into it right away.
  • 9:19 - 9:23
    >> But the rule I have for expressive
    writing is you should write when you
  • 9:23 - 9:26
    find that you're thinking
    about a trauma too much.
  • 9:27 - 9:32
    Now, if your spouse died last week
    and you're thinking about it all the time.
  • 9:33 - 9:34
    >> How would we define
    when that's too much?
  • 9:34 - 9:37
    >> That's not too much,
    that's completely normal.
  • 9:38 - 9:40
    If, two years later, you're thinking
    about the spouse all the time,
  • 9:40 - 9:42
    that's too much.
    >> Not so acceptable.
  • 9:42 - 9:48
    >> So I think you have to weigh
    when-- at what point do your friends
  • 9:48 - 9:49
    not want to talk about this anymore?
  • 9:50 - 9:54
    At what point do you realize
    that nobody wants to hear this,
  • 9:54 - 9:56
    or that your friends are avoiding you?
  • 9:56 - 9:59
    That's a sign you're thinking about it
    too much, and that's where
  • 9:59 - 10:01
    expressive writing can be
    really beneficial.
  • 10:03 - 10:08
    Now, I should also say, there are
    some people immediately after trauma,
  • 10:08 - 10:11
    they need to start writing immediately.
  • 10:11 - 10:12
    Fine!
  • 10:12 - 10:13
    >> Do we know who these people are--
    >> No.
  • 10:13 - 10:15
    >> I mean from a clinical perspective--
    >> No, no.
  • 10:15 - 10:18
    >> So, you know, if we're doing
    a crisis intervention kind of a--
  • 10:19 - 10:21
    an intervention for example,
    you know, if somebody
  • 10:21 - 10:25
    runs a practice, for example, who is
    very centered around crisis intervention,
  • 10:25 - 10:28
    this might not be the place where we would
    suggest using this type of technique
  • 10:28 - 10:32
    as much, I'm not sure we have a way
    to test who would benefit from it
  • 10:32 - 10:34
    and who wouldn't.
  • 10:34 - 10:38
    >> Well, I'll tell you what I have done,
    and that is when I dealt with
  • 10:38 - 10:39
    a group like that.
  • 10:39 - 10:45
    I'll say "look, I don't recommend
    writing right now, but there might be
  • 10:45 - 10:49
    "some of you here who feel as though
    you would benefit from it. Try it."
  • 10:49 - 10:55
    You know-- but if you're finding that
    it's not helping, try something else.
  • 10:55 - 10:58
    >> Yeah, there's that autonomy piece
    again, and that sort of self-reflective
  • 10:58 - 11:00
    piece that we want people
    to be engaging in.
  • 11:00 - 11:02
    That makes total sense.
  • 11:02 - 11:06
    So when we think about the ways that
    this gets implemented, are there
  • 11:06 - 11:10
    any other sort of really negative effects
    that can come off of this?
  • 11:10 - 11:13
    Iatrogenic effects, you know, effects
    that are actually harmful to people
  • 11:13 - 11:16
    when we're attempting to help them,
    other ways in which you would consider
  • 11:16 - 11:18
    this technique to be dangerous?
  • 11:18 - 11:21
    >> I've never seen it
    to be dangerous.
  • 11:21 - 11:26
    I've seen some people who got
    quite distressed when they were writing.
  • 11:27 - 11:34
    Now in the laboratory studies, of the,
    I don't know, let's say 2,000 people
  • 11:34 - 11:39
    that I've been involved with,
    I've probably seen cases of
  • 11:39 - 11:41
    maybe three cases that
    were problematic.
  • 11:43 - 11:51
    One was a project-- we were dealing
    with people who were self-harmers.
  • 11:52 - 11:59
    And this was-- actually, this was a
    pivotal study for me, because
  • 12:00 - 12:07
    we were not reading people's writings,
    and I-- but we were vague,
  • 12:07 - 12:11
    a little bit vague about it, and one
    person was essentially writing,
  • 12:11 - 12:14
    you know, "I have my life, I'm going to
    commit suicide" and so forth.
  • 12:14 - 12:16
    And weren't reading it--
    >> And we didn't see that.
  • 12:16 - 12:23
    >> They then... she made a half-hearted
    attempt in the sense that she
  • 12:24 - 12:26
    took a lot of antidepressants.
  • 12:28 - 12:30
    And then told somebody immediately.
  • 12:30 - 12:32
    So that was--
    >> A cry for help attempt.
  • 12:32 - 12:36
    >> That's right, and, you know, I look
    at that thinking just how stupid I was
  • 12:36 - 12:43
    not to be much more clear that we weren't
    reading this, and that if she were
  • 12:43 - 12:47
    distressed, she should tell us.
    >> Take to the usual things
  • 12:47 - 12:51
    that you would need to do
    to communicate that type of distress.
  • 12:51 - 12:55
    But we haven't seen this to have any
    sort of systematic negative effect--
  • 12:55 - 12:56
    >> Not at all.
    >> On people's mood,
  • 12:56 - 13:00
    or to produce suicidal ideation where
    that isn't already what the person
  • 13:00 - 13:02
    is expressing or anything
    along that line certainly.
  • 13:02 - 13:07
    >> In fact, that's what's been the most
    intriguing thing about this,
  • 13:07 - 13:12
    'cause in our early days, this was the
    big concern for every, you know,
  • 13:12 - 13:17
    review board, and others, which was
    "Oh my god, people are--
  • 13:17 - 13:20
    "You can't let someone
    talk about their feelings."
  • 13:20 - 13:24
    And this was the biggest, our biggest
    problem was in the medical IRBs.
  • 13:24 - 13:28
    The medical IRBs would-- you could
    basically submit saying
  • 13:28 - 13:30
    "I'm doing a project, I want to
    saw off people's arms,"
  • 13:30 - 13:31
    and they go "Okay."
  • 13:32 - 13:35
    And then we would say "We want people
    to write about that", and they go
  • 13:35 - 13:41
    "Oh no, that might make people really..."
    >> Mental health does make people
  • 13:41 - 13:44
    uncomfortable.
    >> But I would say
  • 13:44 - 13:47
    looking across all of the studies
    that have been done,
  • 13:47 - 13:53
    there are no major-- I've not seen any
    particular people who are at risk,
  • 13:54 - 13:57
    except those people who--
    >> Already at high risk.
  • 13:57 - 13:59
    >> Who are in crisis at the time.
  • 14:00 - 14:02
    >> Well these are people that obviously
    from a clinical perspective,
  • 14:02 - 14:07
    if you have a client who's at high risk
    of suicial ideation, other kinds of
  • 14:07 - 14:10
    similar problems, obviously we would
    expect for that to be being monitored
  • 14:10 - 14:13
    in other ways that are independent
    of the expressive writing technique,
  • 14:13 - 14:16
    since one of the keys I think that
    Dr. Pennebaker is really
  • 14:16 - 14:20
    bringing forward to us here is that we
    really don't want to review this writing.
  • 14:20 - 14:23
    And in fact, we don't want them to be
    encouraged, people to be encouraged
  • 14:23 - 14:25
    to share this writing with others either.
  • 14:25 - 14:29
    So for example, if we have a group setting
    in which we're actually doing
  • 14:29 - 14:34
    group therapy, and we might assign this
    technique as an individual tool
  • 14:34 - 14:37
    for people to use, I don't think we would
    want to encourage them to share
  • 14:37 - 14:39
    their writing with each other, would we?
  • 14:40 - 14:46
    >> Probably not. And, you know, it's--
    again, I always get nervous about
  • 14:46 - 14:49
    being too, you know, because--
    >> Directive.
  • 14:49 - 14:55
    >> Too directive. But one of the
    early studies showed that
  • 14:55 - 14:58
    expressive writing was bad,
    and I was so upset
  • 14:58 - 15:02
    by this experiment, but the project was--
    turned out to be an odd one,
  • 15:02 - 15:07
    what happened was it was done in
    a situation where they--
  • 15:07 - 15:11
    these were standing groups,
    and in one group,
  • 15:11 - 15:15
    they wrote-- and then afterwards,
    they got up and had to share--
  • 15:15 - 15:16
    read what they had written.
  • 15:16 - 15:20
    And I was thinking "Geez, that's a
    terrible thing," because what
  • 15:20 - 15:27
    what writing is doing is you're writing--
    think about how we share an upsetting
  • 15:27 - 15:29
    experience with another person.
  • 15:29 - 15:33
    So for example, all of us had this
    experience, you know, you start to
  • 15:33 - 15:36
    tell a friend about something that was
    really distressing, and you're starting
  • 15:36 - 15:39
    to talk, and you're looking at their face,
    and their face is going "ahh!"
  • 15:40 - 15:44
    And so what you do is you change the
    topic and you move it off in some--
  • 15:45 - 15:48
    >> Make social adjustments in terms of
    what you're trying to say.
  • 15:48 - 15:53
    >> But if you're reading your writing,
    you can't do that, and so therein's
  • 15:53 - 15:59
    the issue. So I often-- what I have done,
    some people would call it therapy,
  • 15:59 - 16:03
    I don't call it therapy, but when I've
    done work with someone who
  • 16:03 - 16:08
    wants to write, I say "I want you to
    write about this, and if you want to
  • 16:08 - 16:10
    "talk to me about it, that's great,
    but, you know, I don't need to
  • 16:10 - 16:13
    "read it, I don't-- and you don't
    need to read it to me.
  • 16:13 - 16:14
    "You can just tell me about it."
  • 16:15 - 16:18
    >> Yeah, there you go. So obviously,
    I think we're saying that our
  • 16:18 - 16:20
    patients are welcome to talk to us
    about their expressive writing,
  • 16:20 - 16:23
    that it's not that we would say
    "Ah, I don't want to hear about that,"
  • 16:23 - 16:25
    obviously we do, and we want to
    encourage that communicative
  • 16:25 - 16:29
    relationship, but we also want to make it
    clear that this writing is private,
  • 16:29 - 16:33
    so that they are able to fully unload
    and to, you know, kind of engage
  • 16:33 - 16:36
    in that process the best way that
    they can subjectively,
  • 16:36 - 16:38
    without truncating it for them
    in some way that would
  • 16:38 - 16:42
    involve them trying to manage us,
    really, and our feelings that way.
  • 16:42 - 16:43
    Fantastic.
  • 16:44 - 16:48
    So, when we think about the effect
    that expressive writing has on,
  • 16:48 - 16:51
    you know, sort of-- sounds very powerful,
    it sounds like something that I would
  • 16:51 - 16:53
    be very tempted to start to use
    and to really encourage,
  • 16:53 - 16:57
    myself potentially as a practice,
    and then also with my clients.
  • 16:57 - 17:00
    But I also know that there's only
    so much change we can make
  • 17:00 - 17:03
    for people, and change is often
    incremental and happens
  • 17:03 - 17:05
    in small and kind of subtle ways.
  • 17:05 - 17:08
    What would you say is sort of
    the powerfulness of this technique
  • 17:08 - 17:11
    or the effect size, if you will,
    if we're going to speak
  • 17:11 - 17:12
    in research terms.
    >> Right.
  • 17:12 - 17:21
    So this is a-- I would call this a major
    question. So it's important to remember
  • 17:21 - 17:24
    that expressive writing-- it's free.
  • 17:25 - 17:32
    You know-- so as techniques go,
    this is a really low-impact
  • 17:32 - 17:36
    from a clinician's perspective, and also
    from the client's perspective,
  • 17:36 - 17:38
    this is something anybody can do
    in any context.
  • 17:39 - 17:40
    How effective is it?
  • 17:41 - 17:46
    I would-- in the grand effect sizes,
    it's a modest effect size.
  • 17:47 - 17:50
    It doesn't work for everybody,
    it's doesn't work for any one person
  • 17:50 - 17:51
    all the time.
  • 17:52 - 17:59
    It's something that you can do
    any way in all-- and in different methods.
  • 17:59 - 18:03
    But don't think of this as a
    panacea for everything.
  • 18:03 - 18:08
    So I guess that would be my--
    for those of you out in TV land,
  • 18:08 - 18:13
    who are into statistics, the average
    effect size is about 0.16.
  • 18:13 - 18:19
    And it's also important to appreciate,
    there have now been, I'm guessing
  • 18:19 - 18:23
    1,000 studies done on expressive writing.
    >> Or more. Or more.
  • 18:23 - 18:28
    >> And they have been-- they've been done
    all over the world in every-- with every
  • 18:28 - 18:30
    kind of group you could imagine.
    >> Interesting.
  • 18:30 - 18:33
    >> And sometimes it works,
    sometimes it doesn't.
  • 18:33 - 18:38
    And sometimes it-- you know, it depends
    a lot on the kind of measures they have.
  • 18:38 - 18:43
    But there have been five that I know of,
    meta analyses, so these are
  • 18:43 - 18:45
    analyses of large groups of studies.
  • 18:46 - 18:51
    And still, this effect size of about 0.16,
    it bounces around a little bit.
  • 18:51 - 18:54
    But it's generally beneficial.
    >> Yeah, got it.
  • 18:54 - 18:58
    And it sounds like there's no systematic
    group that we would say
  • 18:58 - 19:01
    "This never works for them, this is
    not a group that we want to use
  • 19:01 - 19:02
    "this technique with."
  • 19:03 - 19:05
    We've talked about the complexities,
    I think, of using this technique
  • 19:05 - 19:09
    with children and their developmental
    status being the main issue there.
  • 19:09 - 19:14
    Obviously, it looks like literacy would be
    involved here, can you address that
  • 19:14 - 19:15
    a little bit for us?
  • 19:15 - 19:16
    >> You know, it's funny.
  • 19:17 - 19:21
    One of our early studies looked at
    the maximum security prisoners.
  • 19:21 - 19:23
    >> Interesting, yeah.
  • 19:23 - 19:26
    >> And-- you know, some of these guys
    couldn't write worth a damn.
  • 19:26 - 19:29
    >> Yeah, low education level--
    >> Low education level.
  • 19:29 - 19:30
    >> Low (inaudible) or writing ability.
  • 19:31 - 19:39
    >> But they-- it did work, an even though
    they couldn't spell or write well,
  • 19:39 - 19:44
    they wrote incredibly powerful
    stories that were beneficial.
  • 19:45 - 19:51
    And we-- occasionally in our research,
    we come across groups or individuals
  • 19:51 - 19:53
    who are essentially not literate.
  • 19:53 - 19:58
    And what we have done with them
    is have them just talk into
  • 19:58 - 20:02
    a tape recorder, or we've had them
    try other kinds of methods.
  • 20:03 - 20:08
    So they may have had a little bit of
    training with writing, but they
  • 20:08 - 20:10
    just can't write.
  • 20:10 - 20:13
    Or some, they just physically can't write.
    >> Right.
  • 20:13 - 20:17
    >> And some of the methods that had
    been used, one of my colleagues
  • 20:17 - 20:24
    in Mexico, for example, worked with
    a Mayan group, and one of their
  • 20:24 - 20:28
    cultural traditions there are worry dolls,
    and what he did was have them,
  • 20:28 - 20:31
    gave them essentially the writing
    instructions, but instead of
  • 20:31 - 20:35
    writing, they talked to these little
    worry dolls, and found beneficial
  • 20:35 - 20:36
    effects there.
  • 20:37 - 20:41
    >> Fascinating, so obviously the writing
    in and of itself may not be the sort of
  • 20:41 - 20:44
    crucial mechanism that we were
    talking about, but instead
  • 20:44 - 20:47
    is something that prompts the thinking--
    >> It's the translation into words.
  • 20:47 - 20:51
    >> And relates what's happening.
    >> In fact, another study that
  • 20:51 - 20:55
    I'm really proud of, this was done by
    Anne Krantz, she's a dance therapist
  • 20:55 - 21:00
    in San Francisco who was getting
    her PhD, and came-- and worked
  • 21:00 - 21:01
    with me for her PhD.
  • 21:02 - 21:08
    And we had people do expressive movement
    in a room by themselves, you know,
  • 21:08 - 21:12
    express your most traumatic experience,
    or they would do this, and then
  • 21:12 - 21:13
    they would write afterwards.
  • 21:14 - 21:16
    And then there was a control group.
  • 21:16 - 21:20
    And what we found was with the
    expressive movement, people found this
  • 21:20 - 21:23
    to be wonderful, and on their
    questionnaires, they say
  • 21:23 - 21:25
    "It was valuable, meaningful,
    it changed my life."
  • 21:26 - 21:29
    But it had no effect on their
    physical health, nor on their grades.
  • 21:29 - 21:32
    The group that changed were the
    ones who did the expressive movement
  • 21:32 - 21:34
    and they also wrote.
  • 21:35 - 21:40
    And if you think of how dance
    therapy works, or art therapy,
  • 21:40 - 21:44
    what happens is the person does this
    expressive movement, or they
  • 21:44 - 21:49
    draw or whatever, but then
    afterwards, the clinician says
  • 21:49 - 21:50
    "Tell me about that."
    >> "Let's talk about this."
  • 21:50 - 21:55
    >> That's right. And I think what
    happens is that therapy works--
  • 21:55 - 22:01
    that the expression is valuable,
    it makes you think, but it's the words
  • 22:01 - 22:02
    that cement the changes.
  • 22:02 - 22:05
    >> That verbalizing, and whatever
    mechanism of verbalizing
  • 22:05 - 22:09
    we're talking about, but the verbalizing
    is sort of the key in this way.
  • 22:09 - 22:14
    >> That's right. And another issue that
    I think is also important,
  • 22:14 - 22:20
    is when we put things into words,
    there are different ways that
  • 22:20 - 22:20
    we can do it.
  • 22:20 - 22:26
    So talking is a good one, but there's
    also-- there's the feedback.
  • 22:27 - 22:31
    And one of the things that's
    always is a high risk for a person is
  • 22:31 - 22:34
    "I'm going to tell you something
    so profoundly personal,
  • 22:35 - 22:37
    "and you're not going to validate me."
    >> Right.
  • 22:37 - 22:39
    >> And I think people are nervous
    even in therapy.
  • 22:39 - 22:41
    >> Oh, I know they're nervous
    even in therapy of that,
  • 22:41 - 22:43
    and I think they even have the
    experience in therapy,
  • 22:43 - 22:45
    >> That's right.
    >> Sometimes, of being invalidated.
  • 22:45 - 22:48
    >> That's right. And I think all of us
    have had that experience sometimes.
  • 22:49 - 22:51
    And this is where writing is good.
  • 22:52 - 22:56
    But speaking also fast, you know,
    words just explode out of our mouths.
  • 22:56 - 22:57
    >> It is, it's very quick, yeah.
  • 22:57 - 23:01
    >> So you can compare, say, typing
    with writing by hand.
  • 23:02 - 23:04
    Or another method that I call
    finger writing.
  • 23:04 - 23:08
    And all of these-- these are kind of
    different types. Expressive writing
  • 23:08 - 23:11
    with typing, so there have been
    several studies comparing
  • 23:11 - 23:15
    expressive writing to typing
    versus with handwriting.
  • 23:15 - 23:16
    >> Versus handwriting, yeah.
  • 23:16 - 23:20
    >> And by and large the studies
    don't show much difference,
  • 23:20 - 23:24
    but if you stand back and kind of
    squint at the data,
  • 23:24 - 23:26
    this is called the squint method.
  • 23:26 - 23:30
    And you kind of look and you see
    some slight benefit for handwriting
  • 23:30 - 23:32
    over typing, but it's not
    pretty big.
  • 23:33 - 23:35
    And if you're more comfortable
    just typing...
  • 23:35 - 23:36
    >> Just do it, yeah.
  • 23:38 - 23:42
    >> I think handwriting might work a little
    bit better, because it's slower.
  • 23:42 - 23:47
    And I think how quickly you write
    makes a big difference.
  • 23:48 - 23:51
    >> I wonder if people slow down
    over time in the process,
  • 23:51 - 23:53
    with themselves, even, as they go.
  • 23:53 - 23:55
    >> It could be.
  • 23:55 - 23:57
    I've often wondered, actually,
    if you had a typewriter,
  • 23:57 - 24:00
    you had people come in, they write
    either on their computer,
  • 24:00 - 24:02
    on a typewriter, you know, the old
    manuals where you have to push--
  • 24:02 - 24:05
    >> Boom, boom, boom, yeah,
    it's more efffort, slower there.
  • 24:05 - 24:06
    >> That that might be more beneficial.
  • 24:07 - 24:09
    >> What is finger writing,
    define this for our audience.
  • 24:09 - 24:12
    >> I'm the inventor of finger writing.
  • 24:12 - 24:13
    >> (laughing) Congratulations.
  • 24:13 - 24:20
    >> No, no. So many years ago, my wife
    and I were travelling in another country,
  • 24:20 - 24:26
    we were in some hotel, and I can't
    remember even what the issue was,
  • 24:26 - 24:29
    but I was tossing and turning about
    some issue that I was dealing with.
  • 24:29 - 24:34
    And I didn't want to get up and turn on
    the light and so forth, so I just
  • 24:34 - 24:39
    got up and sat in the chair, and just
    started writing in the air.
  • 24:39 - 24:41
    >> Like as though you were writing
    and your finger was the pen?
  • 24:41 - 24:42
    >> That's right, that's right.
  • 24:43 - 24:45
    And I, you know, I'd go like this, and...
  • 24:47 - 24:51
    And then I started noticing that
    "Wow, this works really well."
  • 24:51 - 24:54
    And then I tried writing in much
    bigger letters, which I did--
  • 24:54 - 24:55
    >> Big, loopy letters.
  • 24:55 - 24:58
    >> And when you do that, you're slowing
    it down even more.
  • 24:58 - 25:00
    >> Yes you are, that's the connection here
    between that verbal and movement
  • 25:00 - 25:02
    and all. That's interesting.
    >> That's right.
  • 25:03 - 25:07
    And so I then started doing this
    in workshop, so I would have people
  • 25:08 - 25:11
    write by hand, versus they would
    have one period for you to just
  • 25:11 - 25:13
    write by fingers.
  • 25:13 - 25:17
    And what I find is a lot of people
    actually prefer finger writing
  • 25:17 - 25:18
    to writing by hand.
  • 25:18 - 25:21
    And I would ask people, "Well, what was
    the difference?" And many people would say
  • 25:22 - 25:26
    "Well, I swear more when I'm writing
    with my fingers," 'cause you know,
  • 25:26 - 25:27
    they don't want to leave--
    >> More open, they're
  • 25:27 - 25:30
    leaving no trace.
    >> There's no trace, exactly.
  • 25:30 - 25:33
    >> Interesting. It's like another
    level of privacy, almost,
  • 25:33 - 25:36
    on the product that you're creating.
    >> That's exactly right.
  • 25:36 - 25:38
    >> That's really neat (laughing).
  • 25:38 - 25:43
    >> So-- but all of these, the ultimate
    goal is translating an emotional
  • 25:43 - 25:45
    experience into words.
  • 25:45 - 25:49
    And I think that is really a critical
    dimension. Well in many ways,
  • 25:49 - 25:52
    I'm sure that that's a commonality
    that underlies a lot of
  • 25:52 - 25:54
    effective therapies, actually.
    >> Effective therapies,
  • 25:54 - 25:57
    social support.
    >> Anything that helps us.
  • 25:57 - 26:02
    >> Just like religion and prayers, all of
    these are is that translation process.
  • 26:03 - 26:06
    >> Often many I think meditative,
    and contemplative practices
  • 26:06 - 26:09
    also involve that same type of process,
    that's why it's interesting to
  • 26:09 - 26:11
    see that it's there.
  • 26:11 - 26:16
    So I wonder-- you know, we talked a lot
    about sort of the, a bit of the
  • 26:16 - 26:20
    symptomatology that people might
    find is eased through this process,
  • 26:20 - 26:22
    sleep problems, for example.
  • 26:22 - 26:25
    Obviously frontal lobe-related things,
    if you're having a cluttered
  • 26:25 - 26:27
    working memory, that kind of thing.
  • 26:27 - 26:31
    And in my experience of this,
    those symptoms overlap
  • 26:31 - 26:33
    through a lot of different kinds of
    disorders that we might see
  • 26:33 - 26:34
    in clinical practice.
  • 26:35 - 26:38
    I don't think that this-- this hasn't
    really been done, experimenting,
  • 26:38 - 26:41
    looking at specific diagnostic
    groups so much.
  • 26:41 - 26:45
    >> Well it kind of has.
    >> Yeah, so I've talked (inaudible).
  • 26:45 - 26:50
    >> In a sense-- and this is so intersting
    looking at expressive writing
  • 26:50 - 26:58
    versus other methods like kind of
    the official ones, CVT, ACT,
  • 26:58 - 27:00
    you know, they all have three letters.
    (laughter)
  • 27:00 - 27:03
    >> Yes, DBT, we've got lots of them.
    >> Expressive writing is just EW,
  • 27:03 - 27:05
    just for...
    >> 'Cause you're a rebel,
  • 27:05 - 27:07
    through and through, there it is.
    >> (laughing) That's right.
  • 27:08 - 27:14
    But expressive writing, because it's,
    you know, because it's, there's no
  • 27:14 - 27:18
    license for it, you don't need a
    certificate, you just do it.
  • 27:18 - 27:22
    And there's no money that changes
    hands, there haven't been these
  • 27:22 - 27:27
    giant RCTs of 10,000 people.
    >> Yes, indeed.
  • 27:28 - 27:30
    Sort of safety and efficacy
    studies in that way.
  • 27:30 - 27:35
    >> That's right. And my approach is,
    well, why would you want to do it?
  • 27:35 - 27:38
    I certainly-- I'm not going to do it.
    >> Sure.
  • 27:38 - 27:43
    >> So what this means is it's been a--
    it's kind of been a ground swell of
  • 27:43 - 27:48
    small researchers in clinical psychology,
    social psychology, cognitive psychology,
  • 27:48 - 27:53
    and frankly, in anthropology,
    sociology, and other areas.
  • 27:54 - 27:59
    And so you have this giant community
    that has done this expressive writing,
  • 27:59 - 28:03
    and so now we have to look, so this
    person-- so it's been done with
  • 28:03 - 28:06
    people awaiting surgery.
  • 28:06 - 28:10
    This is chronic pain, this one has been
    done on this kind of cancer,
  • 28:10 - 28:14
    this one's been done on--
    you name a disorder,
  • 28:14 - 28:16
    there are studies that have been done.
  • 28:18 - 28:22
    Does it work better for some diseases,
    some people more than others?
  • 28:22 - 28:29
    You know, if you stand back and look
    at it, there's just not a clear answer.
  • 28:29 - 28:32
    It seems to be generally beneficial.
  • 28:32 - 28:35
    >> Yeah. But it's not so much that we
    would say this is an empirically
  • 28:35 - 28:39
    supported treatment for depression,
    or for anxiety, or anything
  • 28:39 - 28:42
    along that line, but instead that this is
    much more of a general technique
  • 28:42 - 28:45
    that would expect to be potentially
    beneficial to anyone that's
  • 28:45 - 28:47
    carrying something around, really.
  • 28:47 - 28:51
    >> That's right. That's right, and so
    depression would be the obvious one.
  • 28:52 - 28:54
    And--
    >> But PTSD also,
  • 28:54 - 28:56
    and other kinds of anxiety experiences.
    >> And PTSD is another one.
  • 28:56 - 29:02
    And what you see when you look at this is
    it seems to work, but it's not,
  • 29:03 - 29:05
    they're not huge effect sizes,
    it's very modest,
  • 29:05 - 29:11
    and I think people right in the midst
    of it, so if one of these PTSD studies
  • 29:11 - 29:16
    was done right after trauma, which
    means its really not PTSD,
  • 29:16 - 29:19
    it's probably not going to work.
    >> You know, that's acute stress disorder.
  • 29:19 - 29:23
    >> And I think also a person in the--
    right in the depths of a
  • 29:23 - 29:26
    depressive episode, I would
    not recommend that.
  • 29:26 - 29:29
    >> I doubt they'd be willing to engage
    in it or able to engage in it.
  • 29:29 - 29:32
    >> You know, and I wouldn't
    even recommend it.
  • 29:32 - 29:34
    >> Yeah, yeah. And so this is also
    something that we would think of
  • 29:34 - 29:38
    as being kind of relevant to the
    therapists as well, right?
  • 29:38 - 29:41
    I mean it sounds to me like it's
    probably, you know, appropriate
  • 29:41 - 29:45
    for a general population that would also
    include the worriers among us,
  • 29:45 - 29:48
    the clinicians who are carrying
    client data around potentially.
  • 29:49 - 29:51
    I know you and I had talked about
    this separately, that there are
  • 29:51 - 29:54
    some ways that we might think about
    this as a tool, possibly, also
  • 29:54 - 30:00
    for sort of processing information
    as a therapist, case conceptualization,
  • 30:00 - 30:04
    thinking about how it is that a
    particular patient is managing things,
  • 30:04 - 30:08
    perhaps you would write your deepest
    thoughts and feelings about a patient.
  • 30:08 - 30:11
    >> I think something like that
    can be really beneficial.
Title:
https:/.../2018-10-12_Expressive_Writing_pt2_edited_1.mp4
Video Language:
English
Duration:
30:11

English subtitles

Revisions