Sensation & Perception - Crash Course Psychology #5
-
0:00 - 0:04Let me tell you about Oliver Sacks, the famous
physician, professor and author of unusual -
0:05 - 0:09neurological case studies. We’ll be looking
at some of his fascinating research in future -
0:09 - 0:12lessons, but for now, I just want to talk
about Sacks himself. Although he possesses -
0:13 - 0:18a brilliant and inquisitive mind, Dr. Sacks
cannot do a simple thing that your average -
0:17 - 0:21toddler can. He can’t recognize his own
face in the mirror. -
0:21 - 0:26Sacks has a form of prosopagnosia, a neurological
disorder that impairs a person’s ability -
0:26 - 0:30to perceive or recognize faces, also known
as face blindness. Last week we talked about -
0:31 - 0:35how brain function is localized, and this
is another peculiarly excellent example of -
0:36 - 0:41that. Sacks can recognize his coffee cup on
the shelf, but he can’t pick out his oldest -
0:42 - 0:46friend from a crowd, because the specific
sliver of his brain responsible for facial -
0:46 - 0:50recognition is malfunctioning. There’s nothing
wrong with his vision. The sense is intact. -
0:50 - 0:54The problem is with his perception, at least
when it comes to recognizing faces. Prosopagnosia -
0:55 - 0:59is a good example of how sensing and perceiving
are connected, but different. -
0:59 - 1:03Sensation is the bottom-up process by which
our senses, like vision, hearing and smell, -
1:04 - 1:09receive and relay outside stimuli. Perception,
on the other hand, is the top-down way our -
1:08 - 1:12brains organize and interpret that information
and put it into context. So right now at this -
1:13 - 1:17very moment, you’re probably receiving light
from your screen through your eyes, which -
1:17 - 1:21will send the data of that sensation to your
brain. Perception meanwhile is your brain -
1:21 - 1:24telling you that what you’re seeing is a
diagram explaining the difference between -
1:25 - 1:29sensation and perception, which is pretty
meta. Now your brain is interpreting that -
1:29 - 1:36light as a talking person, whom your brain
might additionally recognize as Hank. -
1:39 - 1:44[Intro]
-
1:44 - 1:47We are constantly bombarded by stimuli even
though we’re only aware of what our own -
1:47 - 1:52senses can pick up. Like I can see and hear
and feel and even smell this Corgi, but I -
1:53 - 1:58can’t hunt using sonar like a bat or hear
a mole tunneling underground like an owl or -
1:58 - 2:03see ultraviolet and infrared light like a
mantis shrimp. I probably can’t even smell -
2:03 - 2:09half of what you can smell. No! No! We have
different senses. Mwah mwah mwah mwah mwah. -
2:10 - 2:10Yeah.
-
2:10 - 2:14There’s a lot to sense in the world, and
not everybody needs to sense all the same -
2:15 - 2:19stuff. So every animal has its limitations
which we can talk about more precisely if -
2:19 - 2:24we define the Absolute Threshold of Sensation,
the minimum stimulation needed to register -
2:24 - 2:29a particular stimulus, 50% of the time. So
if I play a tiny little beep in your ear and -
2:28 - 2:31you tell me that you hear it fifty percent
of the times that I play it, that’s your -
2:31 - 2:35absolute threshold of sensation. We have to
use a percentage because sometimes I'll play -
2:36 - 2:39the beep and you’ll hear it and sometimes
you won’t even though it’s the exact same -
2:39 - 2:42volume. Why? Because brains are complicated.
-
2:43 - 2:46Detecting a weak sensory signal like that
beep in daily life isn’t only about the -
2:46 - 2:50strength of the stimulus. It’s also about
your psychological state; your alertness and -
2:51 - 2:56expectations in the moment. This has to do
with Signal Detection Theory, a model for -
2:56 - 3:01predicting how and when a person will detect
a weak stimuli, partly based on context. Exhausted -
3:01 - 3:05new parents might hear their baby’s tiniest
whimper, but not even register the bellow -
3:06 - 3:11of a passing train. Their paranoid parent
brains are so trained on their baby, it gives -
3:11 - 3:15their senses a sort of boosted ability, but
only in relation to the subject of their attention. -
3:15 - 3:19Conversely, if you’re experiencing constant
stimulation, your senses will adjust in a -
3:20 - 3:24process called sensory adaptation. It is the
reason that I have to check and see if my -
3:24 - 3:28wallet is there if it’s in my right pocket,
but if I move it to my left pocket, it feels -
3:28 - 3:31like a big uncomfortable lump. It’s also
useful to be able to talk about our ability -
3:32 - 3:35to detect the difference between two stimuli.
I might go out at night and look up at the -
3:35 - 3:40sky and, well, I know with my objective science
brain that no two stars have the exact same -
3:40 - 3:44brightness, and yeah, I can tell with my eyeballs
that some stars are brighter than others, -
3:45 - 3:50but other stars just look exactly the same
to me. I can’t tell the difference in their -
3:50 - 3:51brightness.
-
3:50 - 3:57Are you done? Is it time for your to go? Gimme,
gimme a kiiiissss. Yes, yes. Okay. Good girl. -
3:58 - 4:01The point at which one can tell the difference
is the difference threshold, but it’s not -
4:02 - 4:06linear. Like. if a tiny star is just a tiny
bit brighter than another tiny star, I can -
4:06 - 4:11tell. But if a big star is that same tiny
amount brighter than another big star, I won’t -
4:11 - 4:14be able to tell the difference. This is important
enough that we gave the guy who discovered -
4:14 - 4:19it a law. Weber’s Law says that we perceive
differences on a logarithmic, not a linear -
4:19 - 4:23scale. It’s not the amount of change. It’s
the percentage change that matters. -
4:23 - 4:28Alright. How about now we take a more in depth
look at how one of our most powerful senses -
4:28 - 4:33works? Vision. Your ability to see your face
in the mirror is the result of a long but -
4:34 - 4:38lightning quick sequence of events. Light
bounces off your face and then off the mirror -
4:38 - 4:43and then into your eyes, which take in all
that varied energy and transforms it into -
4:43 - 4:47neural messages that your brain processes
and organizes into what you actually see, -
4:48 - 4:51which is your face. Or if you’re looking
elsewhere, you could see a coffee cup or a -
4:52 - 4:54Corgi or a scary clown holding a tiny cream
pie. -
4:54 - 4:57So how do we transform light waves into meaningful
information? Well, let’s start with the -
4:58 - 5:02light itself. What we humans see as light
is only a small fraction of the full spectrum -
5:02 - 5:07of electromagnetic radiation that ranges from
gamma to radio waves. Now light has all kinds -
5:08 - 5:12of fascinating characteristics that determine
how we sense it, but for the purposes of this -
5:12 - 5:16topic, we’ll understand light as traveling
in waves. The wave’s wavelength and frequency -
5:17 - 5:20determines their hue, and their amplitude
determines their intensity or brightness. -
5:21 - 5:26For instance a short wave has a high frequency.
Our eyes register short wavelengths with high -
5:26 - 5:31frequencies as blueish colors while we see
long, low frequency wavelengths as reddish -
5:31 - 5:34hues. The way we register the brightness of
a color, the contrast between the orange of -
5:34 - 5:38a sherbet and the orange of a construction
cone has to do with the intensity or amount -
5:38 - 5:43of energy in a given light wave. Which as
we’ve just said is determined by its amplitude. -
5:44 - 5:47Greater amplitude means higher intensity,
means brighter color. -
5:47 - 5:52Someone’s just told me that sherbet doesn’t-
isn’t a word that exists. His name is Michael -
5:52 - 5:58Aranda and he’s a dumbhead. Did you type
it into the dictionary? Type it into Google. -
5:59 - 6:02Ask Google about sherbet. So sherbet is a
thing. -
6:02 - 6:06So after taking this light in through the
cornea and the pupil, it hits the transparent -
6:06 - 6:12disc behind the pupil: the lens, which focuses
the light rays into specific images, and just -
6:11 - 6:15as you’d expect the lens to do, it projects
these images onto the retina, the inner surface -
6:15 - 6:19of the eyeball that contains all the receptor
cells that begin sensing that visual information. -
6:20 - 6:24Now your retinas don’t receive a full image
like a movie being projected onto a screen. -
6:24 - 6:29It’s more like a bunch of pixel points of
light energy that millions of receptors translate -
6:28 - 6:31into neural impulses and zip back into the
brain. -
6:32 - 6:36These retinal receptors are called rods and
cones. Our rods detect gray scale and are -
6:37 - 6:41used in our peripheral vision as well as to
avoid stubbing our toes in twilight conditions -
6:41 - 6:45when we can’t really see in color. Our cones
detect fine detail and color. Concentrated -
6:45 - 6:50near the retina’s central focal point called
the fovea, cones function only in well lit -
6:50 - 6:54conditions, allowing you to appreciate the
intricacies of your grandma’s china pattern -
6:55 - 7:00or your uncle’s sleeve tattoo. And the human
eye is terrific at seeing color. Our difference -
7:00 - 7:05threshold for colors is so exceptional that
the average person can distinguish a million -
7:05 - 7:06different hues.
-
7:05 - 7:09There’s a good deal of ongoing research
around exactly how our color vision works. -
7:10 - 7:14But two theories help us explain some of what
we know. One model, called the Young-Helmholtz -
7:13 - 7:17trichromatic theory suggests that the retina
houses three specific color receptor cones -
7:18 - 7:21that register red, green and blue, and when
stimulated together, their combined power -
7:22 - 7:26allows the eye to register any color. Unless,
of course you’re colorblind. About one in -
7:26 - 7:30fifty people have some level of color vision
deficiency. They’re mostly dudes because -
7:30 - 7:33the genetic defect is sex linked. If you can’t
see the Crash Course logo pop out at you in -
7:34 - 7:38this figure, it’s likely that your red or
green cones are missing or malfunctioning -
7:39 - 7:43which means you have dichromatic instead of
trichromatic vision and can’t distinguish -
7:43 - 7:45between shades of red and green.
-
7:45 - 7:48The other model for color vision, known as
the opponent-process theory, suggests that -
7:48 - 7:52we see color through processes that actually
work against each other. So some receptor -
7:53 - 7:58cells might be stimulated by red but inhibited
by green, while others do the opposite, and -
7:58 - 8:00those combinations allow us to register colors.
-
8:01 - 8:04But back to your eyeballs. When stimulated,
the rods and cones trigger chemical changes -
8:05 - 8:10that spark neural signals which in turn activate
the cells behind them called bipolar cells, -
8:10 - 8:15whose job it is to turn on the neighboring
ganglion cells. The long axon tails of these -
8:15 - 8:20ganglions braid together to form the ropy
optic nerve, which is what carries the neural -
8:19 - 8:23impulses from the eyeball to the brain. That
visual information then slips through a chain -
8:23 - 8:28of progressively complex levels as it travels
from optic nerve, to the thalamus, and on -
8:29 - 8:32to the brain’s visual cortex. The visual
cortex sits at the back of the brain in the -
8:32 - 8:37occipital lobe, where the right cortex processes
input from the left eye and vice versa. This -
8:37 - 8:42cortex has specialized nerve cells, called
feature detectors that respond to specific -
8:43 - 8:48features like shapes, angles and movements.
In other words different parts of your visual -
8:47 - 8:50cortex are responsible for identifying different
aspects of things. -
8:50 - 8:53A person who can’t recognize human faces
may have no trouble picking out their set -
8:54 - 8:57of keys from a pile on the counter. That’s
because the brains object perception occurs -
8:58 - 9:01in a different place from its face perception.
In the case of Dr. Sacks, his condition affects -
9:02 - 9:06the region of the brain called the fusiform
gyrus, which activates in response to seeing -
9:06 - 9:10faces. Sacks’s face blindness is congenital,
but it may also be acquired through disease -
9:10 - 9:13or injury to that same region of the brain.
And some cells in a region may respond to -
9:14 - 9:18just one type of stimulus, like posture or
movement or facial expression, while other -
9:19 - 9:23clusters of cells weave all that separate
information together in an instant analysis -
9:23 - 9:28of a situation. That clown is frowning and
running at me with a tiny cream pie. I’m -
9:28 - 9:31putting these factors together. Maybe I should
get out of here. -
9:30 - 9:34This ability to process and analyze many separate
aspects of the situation at once is called -
9:35 - 9:39parallel processing. In the case of visual
processing, this means that the brain simultaneously -
9:39 - 9:44works on making sense of form, depth, motion
and color and this is where we enter the whole -
9:44 - 9:49world of perception which gets complicated
quickly, and can even get downright philosophical. -
9:50 - 9:55So we’ll be exploring that in depth next
time but for now, if you were paying attention, -
9:54 - 9:57you learned the difference between sensation
and perception, the different thresholds that -
9:58 - 10:02limit our senses, and some of the neurology
and biology and psychology of human vision. -
10:02 - 10:05Thanks for watching this lesson with your
eyeballs, and thanks to our generous co-sponsors -
10:06 - 10:13who made this episode possible: Alberto Costa,
Alpna Agrawal PhD, Frank Zegler, Philipp Dettmer -
10:14 - 10:14and Kurzgesagt.
-
10:14 - 10:17And if you’d like to sponsor an episode
and get your own shout out, you can learn -
10:17 - 10:22about that and other perks available to our
Subbable subscribers, just go to subbable.com/crashcourse. -
10:23 - 10:27This episode was written by Kathleen Yale,
edited by Blake de Pastino, and our consultant -
10:27 - 10:31is Dr. Ranjit Bhagwat. Our director and editor
is Nicholas Jenkins, the script supervisor -
10:31 - 10:34is Michael Aranda who is also our sound designer,
and our graphics team is Thought Cafe.
- Title:
- Sensation & Perception - Crash Course Psychology #5
- Description:
-
You can directly support Crash Course at http://www.subbable.com/crashcourse Subscribe for as little as $0 to keep up with everything we're doing. Also, if you can afford to pay a little every month, it really helps us to continue producing great content.
Just what is the difference between sensing and perceiving? And how does vision actually work? And what does this have to do with a Corgi? In this episode of Crash Course Psychology, Hank takes us on a journey through the brain to better explain these and other concepts. Plus, you know, CORGI!
--
Table of Contents:
Sensation vs. Perception :54
Sense Thresholds 2:11
Neurology of Vision 4:23
--
Want to find Crash Course elsewhere on the internet?
Facebook - http://www.facebook.com/YouTubeCrashCourse
Twitter - http://www.twitter.com/TheCrashCourse
Tumblr - http://thecrashcourse.tumblr.com
Support CrashCourse on Subbable: http://subbable.com/crashcourse - Video Language:
- English
- Duration:
- 10:46
Becky Davis edited English subtitles for Sensation & Perception - Crash Course Psychology #5 |