< Return to Video

A new way to diagnose autism | Ami Klin | TEDxPeachtree

  • 0:00 - 0:05
    I always wanted to become
    a walking laboratory of social engagement:
  • 0:06 - 0:12
    to resonate other people's feelings,
    thoughts, intentions, motivations,
  • 0:12 - 0:13
    in the act of being with them.
  • 0:15 - 0:20
    As a scientist, I always wanted
    to measure that resonance,
  • 0:21 - 0:23
    that sense of the other
    that happens so quickly,
  • 0:23 - 0:25
    in the blink of an eye.
  • 0:26 - 0:28
    We intuit other people's feelings;
  • 0:28 - 0:31
    we know the meaning of their actions
    even before they happen.
  • 0:32 - 0:34
    We're always in this stance
  • 0:34 - 0:37
    of being the object
    of somebody else's subjectivity.
  • 0:37 - 0:40
    We do that all the time.
    We just can't shake it off.
  • 0:40 - 0:44
    It's so important that the very tools
    we use to understand ourselves,
  • 0:44 - 0:46
    to understand the world around us,
  • 0:46 - 0:47
    are shaped by that stance.
  • 0:48 - 0:50
    We are social to the core.
  • 0:52 - 0:54
    So my journey in autism really started
  • 0:54 - 0:58
    when I lived in a residential unit
    for adults with autism.
  • 0:58 - 1:01
    Most of those individuals
    had spent most of their lives
  • 1:01 - 1:03
    in long-stay hospitals.
  • 1:03 - 1:04
    This is a long time ago.
  • 1:05 - 1:09
    And for them, autism was devastating.
  • 1:09 - 1:12
    They had profound
    intellectual disabilities.
  • 1:13 - 1:14
    They didn't talk.
  • 1:14 - 1:16
    But most of all,
  • 1:16 - 1:21
    they were extraordinarily isolated
    from the world around them,
  • 1:21 - 1:23
    from their environment
  • 1:23 - 1:25
    and from the people.
  • 1:25 - 1:28
    In fact, at the time,
    if you walked into a school
  • 1:28 - 1:30
    for individuals with autism,
  • 1:30 - 1:32
    you'd hear a lot of noise,
  • 1:32 - 1:36
    plenty of commotion, actions,
    people doing things.
  • 1:38 - 1:40
    But they're always doing
    things by themselves.
  • 1:41 - 1:45
    So they may be looking
    at a light in the ceiling,
  • 1:46 - 1:48
    or they may be isolated in the corner,
  • 1:49 - 1:52
    or they might be engaged
    in these repetitive movements,
  • 1:52 - 1:56
    in self-stimulatory movements
    that led them nowhere.
  • 1:57 - 1:59
    Extremely, extremely isolated.
  • 2:01 - 2:06
    Well, now we know that autism
    is this disruption,
  • 2:06 - 2:10
    the disruption of this resonance
    that I am telling you about.
  • 2:10 - 2:12
    These are survival skills.
  • 2:12 - 2:14
    These are survival skills
    that we inherited
  • 2:15 - 2:18
    over many, many hundreds
    of thousands of years of evolution.
  • 2:19 - 2:24
    You see, babies are born
    in a state of utter fragility.
  • 2:24 - 2:26
    Without the caregiver,
    they wouldn't survive,
  • 2:26 - 2:29
    so it stands to reason
    that nature would endow them
  • 2:29 - 2:30
    with these mechanisms of survival.
  • 2:32 - 2:34
    They orient to the caregiver.
  • 2:35 - 2:37
    From the first days and weeks of life,
  • 2:38 - 2:41
    babies prefer to hear human sounds,
  • 2:41 - 2:43
    rather than just sounds
    in the environment.
  • 2:43 - 2:45
    They prefer to look at people
    rather than at things,
  • 2:45 - 2:49
    and even as they're looking at people,
    they look at people's eyes,
  • 2:50 - 2:54
    because the eye is the window
    to the other person's experiences,
  • 2:54 - 2:56
    so much so that they even prefer
    to look at people
  • 2:56 - 3:00
    who are looking at them
    rather than people who are looking away.
  • 3:01 - 3:03
    Well, they orient to the caregiver.
  • 3:03 - 3:05
    The caregiver seeks the baby.
  • 3:05 - 3:09
    And it's out of this mutually
    reinforcing choreography
  • 3:09 - 3:13
    that a lot that is of importance
    to the emergence of mind --
  • 3:13 - 3:16
    the social mind, the social
    brain -- depends on.
  • 3:18 - 3:21
    We always think about autism
  • 3:21 - 3:25
    as something that happens
    later on in life.
  • 3:25 - 3:29
    It doesn't; it begins
    with the beginning of life.
  • 3:31 - 3:36
    As babies engage with caregivers,
    they soon realize that, well,
  • 3:37 - 3:41
    there is something between the ears
    that is very important --
  • 3:41 - 3:44
    it's invisible, you can't see it,
    but it's really critical.
  • 3:45 - 3:47
    And that thing is called attention.
  • 3:47 - 3:48
    And they learn soon enough,
  • 3:48 - 3:51
    even before they can utter one word,
  • 3:51 - 3:54
    that they can take that attention
    and move somewhere
  • 3:54 - 3:56
    in order to get things they want.
  • 3:58 - 4:01
    They also learn to follow
    other people's gazes,
  • 4:01 - 4:05
    because whatever people are looking at
    is what they are thinking about.
  • 4:07 - 4:10
    And soon enough, they start to learn
    about the meaning of things,
  • 4:10 - 4:13
    because when somebody
    is looking at something
  • 4:13 - 4:15
    or somebody is pointing at something,
  • 4:15 - 4:17
    they're not just getting
    a directional cue.
  • 4:18 - 4:21
    They are getting the other
    person's meaning of that thing,
  • 4:21 - 4:23
    the attitude.
  • 4:23 - 4:27
    And soon enough, they start
    building this body of meanings,
  • 4:27 - 4:31
    but meanings that were acquired
    within the realm of social interaction.
  • 4:32 - 4:34
    Those are meanings that are acquired
  • 4:34 - 4:37
    as part of their shared
    experiences with others.
  • 4:38 - 4:44
    Well, this is a 15-month-old little girl,
  • 4:46 - 4:47
    and she has autism.
  • 4:49 - 4:54
    And I am coming so close to her
    that I am maybe two inches from her face,
  • 4:54 - 4:56
    and she's quite oblivious to me.
  • 4:56 - 5:00
    Imagine if I did that to you,
    came two inches from your face.
  • 5:00 - 5:02
    You'd do probably
    two things, wouldn't you?
  • 5:02 - 5:05
    You would recoil.
    You would call the police.
  • 5:05 - 5:06
    (Laughter)
  • 5:06 - 5:07
    You would do something,
  • 5:07 - 5:11
    because it's literally impossible
    to penetrate somebody's physical space
  • 5:11 - 5:12
    and not get that reaction.
  • 5:12 - 5:16
    We do so, remember,
    intuitively, effortlessly.
  • 5:16 - 5:17
    This is our body wisdom;
  • 5:17 - 5:19
    it's not something mediated
    by our language.
  • 5:19 - 5:22
    Our body just knows that.
  • 5:22 - 5:25
    And we've known that for a long time.
  • 5:25 - 5:28
    And this is not something
    that happens to humans only.
  • 5:28 - 5:31
    It happens to some
    of our phyletic cousins,
  • 5:31 - 5:35
    because if you're a monkey,
    and you look at another monkey,
  • 5:35 - 5:39
    and that monkey has a higher
    hierarchy position than you,
  • 5:39 - 5:42
    and that is considered
    to be a signal or threat,
  • 5:42 - 5:45
    well, you are not going
    to be alive for long.
  • 5:45 - 5:50
    So something that in other species
    are survival mechanisms,
  • 5:50 - 5:53
    without which they
    wouldn't basically live,
  • 5:53 - 5:56
    we bring into the context of human beings,
  • 5:56 - 6:00
    and this is what we need
    to simply act, socially.
  • 6:00 - 6:03
    Now, she is oblivious to me
    and I'm so close to her,
  • 6:03 - 6:05
    and you think, maybe she can see you,
  • 6:05 - 6:06
    maybe she can hear you.
  • 6:07 - 6:08
    Well, a few minutes later,
  • 6:08 - 6:10
    she goes to the corner of the room,
  • 6:10 - 6:14
    and she finds a tiny little piece
    of candy, an M&M.
  • 6:15 - 6:19
    So I could not attract her attention,
  • 6:19 - 6:21
    but something -- a thing -- did.
  • 6:22 - 6:24
    Now, most of us make a big dichotomy
  • 6:24 - 6:27
    between the world of things
    and the world of people.
  • 6:29 - 6:33
    Now, for this girl,
    that division line is not so clear,
  • 6:33 - 6:37
    and the world of people
    is not attracting her
  • 6:37 - 6:38
    as much as we would like.
  • 6:38 - 6:42
    Now, remember that we learn a great deal
    by sharing experiences.
  • 6:42 - 6:48
    What she is doing right now
    is that her path of learning is diverging,
  • 6:48 - 6:50
    moment by moment,
  • 6:50 - 6:53
    as she is isolating herself
    further and further.
  • 6:54 - 6:57
    So we feel sometimes
    that the brain is deterministic,
  • 6:57 - 6:59
    the brain determines
    who we're going to be.
  • 6:59 - 7:02
    But, in fact, the brain
    also becomes who we are,
  • 7:02 - 7:06
    and at the same time
    that her behaviors are taking away
  • 7:06 - 7:08
    from the realm of social interaction,
  • 7:08 - 7:10
    this is what's happening with her mind,
  • 7:10 - 7:13
    and this is what's happening
    with her brain.
  • 7:15 - 7:20
    Well, autism is the most strongly
    genetic condition
  • 7:20 - 7:22
    of all developmental disorders.
  • 7:24 - 7:26
    And it's a brain disorder.
  • 7:27 - 7:30
    It's a disorder that begins
    much prior to the time
  • 7:30 - 7:31
    that the child is born.
  • 7:32 - 7:36
    We now know that there is a very
    broad spectrum of autism.
  • 7:36 - 7:40
    There are those individuals
    who are profoundly intellectually disabled
  • 7:40 - 7:42
    but there are those that are gifted.
  • 7:42 - 7:44
    There are those individuals
    who don't talk at all;
  • 7:44 - 7:47
    there are those individuals
    who talk too much.
  • 7:47 - 7:50
    There are those individuals
    that if you observe them in their school,
  • 7:50 - 7:54
    you see them running the periphery fence
    all the school day if you let them,
  • 7:54 - 7:56
    to those individuals
    who cannot stop coming to you
  • 7:56 - 7:59
    and trying to engage you
    repeatedly, relentlessly,
  • 7:59 - 8:01
    but often in an awkward fashion,
  • 8:02 - 8:05
    without that immediate resonance.
  • 8:06 - 8:10
    Well, this is much more prevalent
    than we thought at the time.
  • 8:10 - 8:11
    When I started in this field,
  • 8:11 - 8:14
    we thought there were four individuals
    with autism per 10,000 --
  • 8:14 - 8:15
    a very rare condition.
  • 8:16 - 8:20
    Well, now we know it's more
    like one in 100.
  • 8:20 - 8:23
    There are millions of individuals
    with autism all around us.
  • 8:25 - 8:28
    The societal cost
    of this condition is huge,
  • 8:29 - 8:32
    in the US alone,
    maybe 35 to 80 billion dollars.
  • 8:32 - 8:33
    And you know what?
  • 8:33 - 8:37
    Most of those funds are associated
    with adolescents and particularly adults
  • 8:37 - 8:39
    who are severely disabled,
  • 8:39 - 8:41
    individuals who need
    wraparound services --
  • 8:41 - 8:44
    services that are very, very intensive.
  • 8:44 - 8:48
    And those services can cost in excess
    of 60,000 to 80,000 dollars a year.
  • 8:49 - 8:52
    Those are individuals who did not
    benefit from early treatment,
  • 8:52 - 8:57
    because now we know
    that autism creates itself
  • 8:57 - 9:01
    as individuals diverge in that pathway
    of learning that I mentioned to you.
  • 9:01 - 9:04
    Were we to be able
    to identify this condition
  • 9:04 - 9:07
    at an earlier point,
    and intervene and treat --
  • 9:08 - 9:12
    I can tell you, this has been probably
    something that has changed my life
  • 9:12 - 9:13
    in the past 10 years,
  • 9:13 - 9:17
    this notion that we can absolutely
    attenuate this condition.
  • 9:19 - 9:21
    Also, we have a window of opportunity,
  • 9:21 - 9:24
    because the brain
    is malleable for just so long,
  • 9:24 - 9:28
    and that window of opportunity
    happens in the first three years of life.
  • 9:28 - 9:31
    It's not that that window
    closes; it doesn't.
  • 9:32 - 9:34
    But it diminishes considerably.
  • 9:35 - 9:38
    And yet, the median age
    of diagnosis in this country
  • 9:38 - 9:40
    is still about five years,
  • 9:40 - 9:42
    and in disadvantaged populations,
  • 9:42 - 9:45
    the populations that don't have
    access to clinical services,
  • 9:45 - 9:48
    rural populations, minorities,
  • 9:48 - 9:51
    the age of diagnosis is later still,
  • 9:51 - 9:53
    which is almost as if I were to tell you
  • 9:53 - 9:57
    that we are condemning those communities
    to have individuals with autism
  • 9:57 - 9:59
    whose condition is going
    to be more severe.
  • 10:00 - 10:03
    So I feel that we have
    a bioethical imperative.
  • 10:03 - 10:05
    The science is there.
  • 10:06 - 10:08
    But no science is of relevance
  • 10:08 - 10:11
    if it doesn't have an impact
    on the community.
  • 10:12 - 10:15
    And we just can't afford
    that missed opportunity,
  • 10:15 - 10:18
    because children with autism
    become adults with autism.
  • 10:18 - 10:22
    And we feel that those things we can do
  • 10:22 - 10:25
    for these children,
    for those families, early on,
  • 10:25 - 10:27
    will have lifetime consequences --
  • 10:27 - 10:31
    for the child, for the family,
    and for the community at large.
  • 10:31 - 10:34
    So this is our view of autism.
  • 10:34 - 10:37
    There are over a hundred genes
    that are associated with autism.
  • 10:37 - 10:39
    In fact, we believe there are going to be
  • 10:39 - 10:43
    something between 300 and 600
    genes associated with autism,
  • 10:43 - 10:47
    and genetic anomalies,
    much more than just genes.
  • 10:47 - 10:51
    And we actually have
    a bit of a question here,
  • 10:52 - 10:55
    because if there are so many
    different causes of autism,
  • 10:55 - 10:59
    how do you go from those liabilities
    to the actual syndrome?
  • 10:59 - 11:01
    Because people like myself,
  • 11:02 - 11:04
    when we walk into a playroom,
  • 11:04 - 11:06
    we recognize a child as having autism.
  • 11:07 - 11:09
    So how do you go from multiple causes
  • 11:09 - 11:11
    to a syndrome that has some homogeneity?
  • 11:12 - 11:15
    And the answer is what lies in between,
  • 11:16 - 11:17
    which is development.
  • 11:18 - 11:22
    And in fact, we are very interested
    in those first two years of life,
  • 11:22 - 11:26
    because those liabilities
    don't necessarily convert into autism.
  • 11:26 - 11:28
    Autism creates itself.
  • 11:29 - 11:33
    Were we to be able to intervene
    during those years of life,
  • 11:34 - 11:39
    we might attenuate for some, and God
    knows, maybe even prevent for others.
  • 11:40 - 11:41
    So how do we do that?
  • 11:42 - 11:45
    How do we enter that feeling of resonance,
  • 11:45 - 11:48
    how do we enter another person's being?
  • 11:49 - 11:52
    I remember when I interacted
    with that 15-month-old,
  • 11:52 - 11:54
    the thing that came to my mind was,
  • 11:54 - 11:57
    "How do you come into her world?
  • 11:57 - 12:01
    Is she thinking about me?
    Is she thinking about others?"
  • 12:02 - 12:05
    Well, it's hard to do that,
  • 12:05 - 12:07
    so we had to create the technologies.
  • 12:07 - 12:09
    We had to basically step inside a body.
  • 12:09 - 12:12
    We had to see the world through her eyes.
  • 12:13 - 12:16
    And so in the past many years,
  • 12:16 - 12:18
    we've been building these new technologies
  • 12:18 - 12:20
    that are based on eye tracking.
  • 12:20 - 12:24
    We can see, moment by moment,
    what children are engaging with.
  • 12:26 - 12:28
    This is my colleague, Warren Jones,
  • 12:28 - 12:31
    with whom we've been building
    these methods, these studies,
  • 12:31 - 12:32
    for the past 12 years.
  • 12:33 - 12:36
    And you see there a happy five-month-old,
  • 12:37 - 12:42
    a five-month little boy
    who is going to watch things
  • 12:42 - 12:44
    that are brought from his world:
  • 12:45 - 12:47
    his mom, the caregiver,
  • 12:47 - 12:51
    but also experiences that he would have
    were he to be in his daycare.
  • 12:52 - 12:56
    What we want is to embrace that world
    and bring it into our laboratory,
  • 12:56 - 12:58
    but in order for us to do that,
  • 12:58 - 13:02
    we had to create
    these very sophisticated measures,
  • 13:02 - 13:06
    measures of how people, how little babies,
  • 13:06 - 13:10
    how newborns, engage
    with the world, moment by moment.
  • 13:10 - 13:12
    What is important and what is not.
  • 13:13 - 13:16
    Well, we created those measures,
  • 13:16 - 13:19
    and here, what you see
    is what we call a funnel of attention.
  • 13:20 - 13:21
    You're watching a video --
  • 13:22 - 13:24
    those frames are separated
    by about a second --
  • 13:24 - 13:28
    through the eyes of 35 typically
    developing two-year-olds.
  • 13:29 - 13:31
    And we freeze one frame,
  • 13:32 - 13:35
    and this is what the typical
    children are doing.
  • 13:35 - 13:39
    In this scan pass, in green here,
    are two-year-olds with autism.
  • 13:39 - 13:45
    So on that frame, the children
    who are typical are watching this,
  • 13:46 - 13:49
    the emotion of expression
    of that little boy
  • 13:49 - 13:51
    as he's fighting a little bit
    with the little girl.
  • 13:51 - 13:53
    What are the children with autism doing?
  • 13:53 - 13:56
    They are focusing on the revolving door,
  • 13:57 - 13:59
    opening and shutting.
  • 14:00 - 14:03
    Well, I can tell you that this divergence
    that you're seeing here
  • 14:03 - 14:06
    doesn't happen only
    in our five-minute experiment.
  • 14:06 - 14:09
    It happens moment by moment
    in their real lives,
  • 14:10 - 14:14
    and their minds are being formed
    and their brains are being specialized
  • 14:14 - 14:18
    in something other than what is happening
    with their typical peers.
  • 14:19 - 14:25
    Well, we took a construct
    from our pediatrician friends,
  • 14:25 - 14:27
    the concept of growth charts --
  • 14:27 - 14:29
    you know, when you take
    a child to the pediatrician,
  • 14:29 - 14:32
    and you have physical height and weight.
  • 14:33 - 14:36
    Well, we decided we were going
    to create growth charts
  • 14:36 - 14:37
    of social engagement.
  • 14:38 - 14:41
    We sought children
    from the time they're born.
  • 14:42 - 14:45
    What you see here on the x-axis
  • 14:45 - 14:49
    is two, three, four, five,
    six months and nine,
  • 14:49 - 14:51
    until about the age of 24 months.
  • 14:51 - 14:54
    This is the percent of their viewing time
  • 14:54 - 14:56
    that they're focusing on people's eyes,
  • 14:56 - 14:58
    and this is their growth chart.
  • 14:58 - 15:01
    They start over here --
    they love people's eyes --
  • 15:01 - 15:03
    and it remains quite stable.
  • 15:03 - 15:07
    It sort of goes up a little bit
    in those initial months.
  • 15:07 - 15:11
    Now, let's see what's happening
    with babies who became autistic.
  • 15:12 - 15:13
    It's something very different.
  • 15:14 - 15:17
    It starts way up here,
    but then it's a free fall.
  • 15:18 - 15:22
    It's very much like they brought
    into this world the reflex
  • 15:22 - 15:25
    that orients them to people,
    but it has no traction.
  • 15:26 - 15:28
    It's almost as if that stimulus -- you --
  • 15:29 - 15:32
    you're not exerting
    influence on what happens
  • 15:32 - 15:34
    as they navigate their daily lives.
  • 15:35 - 15:42
    Now, we thought those data
    were so powerful, in a way,
  • 15:43 - 15:46
    that we wanted to see what happened
    in the first six months of life,
  • 15:46 - 15:49
    because if you interact
    with a two- and a three-month-old,
  • 15:49 - 15:52
    you'd be surprised
    by how social those babies are.
  • 15:54 - 15:56
    And what we see
    in the first six months of life
  • 15:56 - 16:01
    is that those two groups
    can be segregated very easily.
  • 16:02 - 16:05
    And using these kinds
    of measures and many others,
  • 16:05 - 16:09
    what we found out
    is that our science could, in fact,
  • 16:09 - 16:11
    identify this condition early on.
  • 16:12 - 16:15
    We didn't have to wait
    for the behaviors of autism
  • 16:15 - 16:17
    to emerge in the second year of life.
  • 16:18 - 16:23
    If we measured things that are,
    evolutionarily, highly conserved,
  • 16:23 - 16:25
    and developmentally very early-emerging --
  • 16:25 - 16:28
    things that are online
    from the first weeks of life --
  • 16:28 - 16:30
    we could push the detection of autism
  • 16:30 - 16:32
    all the way to those first months,
  • 16:32 - 16:34
    and that's what we are doing now.
  • 16:37 - 16:40
    Now, we can create
    the very best technologies
  • 16:40 - 16:43
    and the very best methods
    to identify the children,
  • 16:44 - 16:47
    but this would be for naught
    if we didn't have an impact
  • 16:47 - 16:49
    on what happens in their reality
    in the community.
  • 16:50 - 16:52
    Now we want those devices, of course,
  • 16:52 - 16:55
    to be deployed by those
    who are in the trenches --
  • 16:55 - 16:59
    our colleagues, the primary care
    physicians, who see every child --
  • 17:00 - 17:02
    and we need to transform
    those technologies
  • 17:02 - 17:05
    into something that is going
    to add value to their practice,
  • 17:05 - 17:08
    because they have to see so many children.
  • 17:08 - 17:12
    And we want to do that universally
    so that we don't miss any child.
  • 17:12 - 17:13
    But this would be immoral
  • 17:15 - 17:20
    if we also did not have an infrastructure
    for intervention, for treatment.
  • 17:20 - 17:24
    We need to be able to work
    with the families, support the families,
  • 17:24 - 17:27
    to manage those first years with them.
  • 17:28 - 17:31
    We need to be able to really go
  • 17:31 - 17:35
    from universal screening
    to universal access to treatment,
  • 17:35 - 17:38
    because those treatments
    are going to change
  • 17:38 - 17:41
    these children's
    and those families' lives.
  • 17:42 - 17:49
    Now, when we think about what we [can]
    do in those first years,
  • 17:49 - 17:53
    I can tell you, having been
    in this field for so long,
  • 17:54 - 17:56
    one feels really rejuvenated.
  • 17:57 - 18:02
    There is a sense that the science
    that one worked on
  • 18:02 - 18:04
    can actually have an impact on realities,
  • 18:04 - 18:07
    preventing, in fact, those experiences
  • 18:08 - 18:11
    that I really started
    in my journey in this field.
  • 18:11 - 18:15
    I thought at the time
    that this was an intractable condition.
  • 18:15 - 18:18
    No longer. We can do
    a great deal of things.
  • 18:19 - 18:21
    And the idea is not to cure autism.
  • 18:21 - 18:23
    That's not the idea.
  • 18:24 - 18:26
    What we want is to make sure
  • 18:26 - 18:28
    that those individuals
    with autism can be free
  • 18:28 - 18:32
    from the devastating consequences
    that come with it at times,
  • 18:33 - 18:36
    the profound intellectual disabilities,
    the lack of language,
  • 18:36 - 18:38
    the profound, profound isolation.
  • 18:40 - 18:42
    We feel that individuals
    with autism, in fact,
  • 18:42 - 18:44
    have a very special
    perspective on the world,
  • 18:44 - 18:46
    and we need diversity.
  • 18:46 - 18:50
    And they can work extremely well
    in some areas of strength:
  • 18:50 - 18:53
    predictable situations,
    situations that can be defined.
  • 18:53 - 18:56
    Because after all,
    they learn about the world
  • 18:56 - 18:58
    almost, like, about it,
  • 18:58 - 19:01
    rather than learning
    how to function in it.
  • 19:01 - 19:05
    But this is a strength if you're working,
    for example, in technology.
  • 19:06 - 19:10
    And there are those individuals
    who have incredible artistic abilities.
  • 19:10 - 19:12
    We want them to be free to do that.
  • 19:12 - 19:15
    We want that the next generations
    of individuals with autism
  • 19:15 - 19:19
    will be able not only
    to express their strengths,
  • 19:19 - 19:20
    but to fulfill their promise.
  • 19:21 - 19:23
    Well, thank you for listening to me.
  • 19:23 - 19:25
    (Applause)
Title:
A new way to diagnose autism | Ami Klin | TEDxPeachtree
Description:

World renowned autism authority Dr. Ami Klin takes a deeper look at autism beyond its widely acknowledged genetic origins. He explores how autism results when the evolutionarily conserved and developmentally early emerging mechanisms of social adaptation, such as the mutually reinforcing
choreography between infant and caregiver, are disrupted.

In the spirit of ideas worth spreading, TEDx is a program of local, self-organized events that bring people together to share a TED-like experience. At a TEDx event, TEDTalks video and live speakers combine to spark deep discussion and connection in a small group. These local, self-organized events are branded TEDx, where x = independently organized TED event. The TED Conference provides general guidance for the TEDx program, but individual TEDx events are self-organized.* (*Subject to certain rules and regulations)

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDxTalks
Duration:
22:08

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions