< Return to Video

Different ways of knowing

  • 0:00 - 0:02
    I'm a savant,
  • 0:02 - 0:04
    or more precisely,
  • 0:04 - 0:06
    a high-functioning
  • 0:06 - 0:08
    autistic savant.
  • 0:08 - 0:10
    It's a rare condition.
  • 0:10 - 0:13
    And rarer still when accompanied,
  • 0:13 - 0:15
    as in my case,
  • 0:15 - 0:17
    by self-awareness
  • 0:17 - 0:20
    and a mastery of language.
  • 0:20 - 0:23
    Very often when I meet someone
  • 0:23 - 0:25
    and they learn this about me,
  • 0:25 - 0:28
    there's a certain kind of awkwardness.
  • 0:28 - 0:31
    I can see it in their eyes.
  • 0:31 - 0:34
    They want to ask me something.
  • 0:34 - 0:36
    And in the end, quite often,
  • 0:36 - 0:39
    the urge is stronger than they are
  • 0:39 - 0:41
    and they blurt it out:
  • 0:41 - 0:43
    "If I give you my date of birth,
  • 0:43 - 0:45
    can you tell me what day of the week I was born on?"
  • 0:45 - 0:48
    (Laughter)
  • 0:48 - 0:51
    Or they mention cube roots
  • 0:51 - 0:55
    or ask me to recite a long number or long text.
  • 0:55 - 0:57
    I hope you'll forgive me
  • 0:57 - 1:00
    if I don't perform
  • 1:00 - 1:04
    a kind of one-man savant show for you today.
  • 1:04 - 1:07
    I'm going to talk instead
  • 1:07 - 1:09
    about something
  • 1:09 - 1:11
    far more interesting
  • 1:11 - 1:14
    than dates of birth or cube roots --
  • 1:14 - 1:16
    a little deeper
  • 1:16 - 1:19
    and a lot closer, to my mind, than work.
  • 1:19 - 1:21
    I want to talk to you briefly
  • 1:21 - 1:24
    about perception.
  • 1:24 - 1:27
    When he was writing the plays and the short stories
  • 1:27 - 1:29
    that would make his name,
  • 1:29 - 1:32
    Anton Chekhov kept a notebook
  • 1:32 - 1:34
    in which he noted down
  • 1:34 - 1:36
    his observations
  • 1:36 - 1:38
    of the world around him --
  • 1:38 - 1:40
    little details
  • 1:40 - 1:43
    that other people seem to miss.
  • 1:43 - 1:46
    Every time I read Chekhov
  • 1:46 - 1:50
    and his unique vision of human life,
  • 1:50 - 1:52
    I'm reminded of why I too
  • 1:52 - 1:54
    became a writer.
  • 1:54 - 1:56
    In my books,
  • 1:56 - 1:58
    I explore the nature of perception
  • 1:58 - 2:01
    and how different kinds of perceiving
  • 2:01 - 2:03
    create different kinds of knowing
  • 2:03 - 2:06
    and understanding.
  • 2:08 - 2:10
    Here are three questions
  • 2:10 - 2:12
    drawn from my work.
  • 2:12 - 2:14
    Rather than try to figure them out,
  • 2:14 - 2:17
    I'm going to ask you to consider for a moment
  • 2:17 - 2:19
    the intuitions
  • 2:19 - 2:21
    and the gut instincts
  • 2:21 - 2:23
    that are going through your head and your heart
  • 2:23 - 2:26
    as you look at them.
  • 2:26 - 2:29
    For example, the calculation:
  • 2:29 - 2:31
    can you feel where on the number line
  • 2:31 - 2:34
    the solution is likely to fall?
  • 2:34 - 2:37
    Or look at the foreign word and the sounds:
  • 2:37 - 2:39
    can you get a sense of the range of meanings
  • 2:39 - 2:42
    that it's pointing you towards?
  • 2:42 - 2:45
    And in terms of the line of poetry,
  • 2:45 - 2:47
    why does the poet use the word hare
  • 2:47 - 2:50
    rather than rabbit?
  • 2:51 - 2:53
    I'm asking you to do this
  • 2:53 - 2:57
    because I believe our personal perceptions, you see,
  • 2:57 - 2:59
    are at the heart
  • 2:59 - 3:01
    of how we acquire knowledge.
  • 3:01 - 3:03
    Aesthetic judgments,
  • 3:03 - 3:06
    rather than abstract reasoning,
  • 3:06 - 3:08
    guide and shape the process
  • 3:08 - 3:11
    by which we all come to know
  • 3:11 - 3:13
    what we know.
  • 3:13 - 3:16
    I'm an extreme example of this.
  • 3:16 - 3:19
    My worlds of words and numbers
  • 3:19 - 3:21
    blur with color, emotion
  • 3:21 - 3:23
    and personality.
  • 3:23 - 3:25
    As Juan said,
  • 3:25 - 3:28
    it's the condition that scientists call synesthesia,
  • 3:28 - 3:30
    an unusual cross-talk
  • 3:30 - 3:33
    between the senses.
  • 3:36 - 3:38
    Here are the numbers one to 12
  • 3:38 - 3:40
    as I see them --
  • 3:40 - 3:44
    every number with its own shape and character.
  • 3:44 - 3:46
    One is a flash of white light.
  • 3:46 - 3:51
    Six is a tiny and very sad black hole.
  • 3:51 - 3:54
    The sketches are in black and white here,
  • 3:54 - 3:56
    but in my mind they have colors.
  • 3:56 - 3:58
    Three is green.
  • 3:58 - 4:00
    Four is blue.
  • 4:00 - 4:03
    Five is yellow.
  • 4:05 - 4:07
    I paint as well.
  • 4:07 - 4:10
    And here is one of my paintings.
  • 4:10 - 4:14
    It's a multiplication of two prime numbers.
  • 4:14 - 4:16
    Three-dimensional shapes
  • 4:16 - 4:19
    and the space they create in the middle
  • 4:19 - 4:21
    creates a new shape,
  • 4:21 - 4:24
    the answer to the sum.
  • 4:24 - 4:26
    What about bigger numbers?
  • 4:26 - 4:30
    Well you can't get much bigger than Pi,
  • 4:30 - 4:32
    the mathematical constant.
  • 4:32 - 4:34
    It's an infinite number --
  • 4:34 - 4:36
    literally goes on forever.
  • 4:36 - 4:38
    In this painting that I made
  • 4:38 - 4:42
    of the first 20 decimals of Pi,
  • 4:42 - 4:44
    I take the colors
  • 4:44 - 4:47
    and the emotions and the textures
  • 4:47 - 4:49
    and I pull them all together
  • 4:49 - 4:54
    into a kind of rolling numerical landscape.
  • 4:54 - 4:57
    But it's not only numbers that I see in colors.
  • 4:57 - 4:59
    Words too, for me,
  • 4:59 - 5:01
    have colors and emotions
  • 5:01 - 5:03
    and textures.
  • 5:03 - 5:05
    And this is an opening phrase
  • 5:05 - 5:07
    from the novel "Lolita."
  • 5:07 - 5:11
    And Nabokov was himself synesthetic.
  • 5:11 - 5:13
    And you can see here
  • 5:13 - 5:16
    how my perception of the sound L
  • 5:16 - 5:18
    helps the alliteration
  • 5:18 - 5:21
    to jump right out.
  • 5:21 - 5:23
    Another example:
  • 5:23 - 5:25
    a little bit more mathematical.
  • 5:25 - 5:27
    And I wonder if some of you will notice
  • 5:27 - 5:29
    the construction of the sentence
  • 5:29 - 5:32
    from "The Great Gatsby."
  • 5:33 - 5:36
    There is a procession of syllables --
  • 5:36 - 5:38
    wheat, one;
  • 5:38 - 5:40
    prairies, two;
  • 5:40 - 5:43
    lost Swede towns, three --
  • 5:43 - 5:45
    one, two, three.
  • 5:45 - 5:49
    And this effect is very pleasant on the mind,
  • 5:49 - 5:51
    and it helps the sentence
  • 5:51 - 5:54
    to feel right.
  • 5:54 - 5:56
    Let's go back to the questions
  • 5:56 - 5:59
    I posed you a moment ago.
  • 5:59 - 6:02
    64 multiplied by 75.
  • 6:02 - 6:05
    If some of you play chess,
  • 6:05 - 6:07
    you'll know that 64
  • 6:07 - 6:10
    is a square number,
  • 6:10 - 6:12
    and that's why chessboards,
  • 6:12 - 6:14
    eight by eight,
  • 6:14 - 6:17
    have 64 squares.
  • 6:17 - 6:19
    So that gives us a form
  • 6:19 - 6:22
    that we can picture, that we can perceive.
  • 6:22 - 6:25
    What about 75?
  • 6:25 - 6:27
    Well if 100,
  • 6:27 - 6:30
    if we think of 100 as being like a square,
  • 6:30 - 6:33
    75 would look like this.
  • 6:33 - 6:35
    So what we need to do now
  • 6:35 - 6:37
    is put those two pictures
  • 6:37 - 6:39
    together in our mind --
  • 6:39 - 6:42
    something like this.
  • 6:42 - 6:46
    64 becomes 6,400.
  • 6:46 - 6:50
    And in the right-hand corner,
  • 6:50 - 6:52
    you don't have to calculate anything.
  • 6:52 - 6:54
    Four across, four up and down --
  • 6:54 - 6:57
    it's 16.
  • 6:57 - 6:59
    So what the sum is actually asking you to do
  • 6:59 - 7:01
    is 16,
  • 7:01 - 7:04
    16, 16.
  • 7:04 - 7:06
    That's a lot easier
  • 7:06 - 7:09
    than the way that the school taught you to do math, I'm sure.
  • 7:09 - 7:11
    It's 16, 16, 16, 48,
  • 7:11 - 7:13
    4,800 --
  • 7:13 - 7:15
    4,800,
  • 7:15 - 7:18
    the answer to the sum.
  • 7:18 - 7:20
    Easy when you know how.
  • 7:20 - 7:23
    (Laughter)
  • 7:23 - 7:26
    The second question was an Icelandic word.
  • 7:26 - 7:29
    I'm assuming there are not many people here
  • 7:29 - 7:31
    who speak Icelandic.
  • 7:31 - 7:34
    So let me narrow the choices down to two.
  • 7:36 - 7:38
    Hnugginn:
  • 7:38 - 7:40
    is it a happy word,
  • 7:40 - 7:42
    or a sad word?
  • 7:42 - 7:44
    What do you say?
  • 7:45 - 7:47
    Okay.
  • 7:47 - 7:49
    Some people say it's happy.
  • 7:49 - 7:51
    Most people, a majority of people,
  • 7:51 - 7:53
    say sad.
  • 7:53 - 7:57
    And it actually means sad.
  • 7:57 - 8:00
    (Laughter)
  • 8:00 - 8:03
    Why do, statistically,
  • 8:03 - 8:05
    a majority of people
  • 8:05 - 8:07
    say that a word is sad, in this case,
  • 8:07 - 8:10
    heavy in other cases?
  • 8:10 - 8:13
    In my theory, language evolves in such a way
  • 8:13 - 8:15
    that sounds match,
  • 8:15 - 8:18
    correspond with, the subjective,
  • 8:18 - 8:20
    with the personal,
  • 8:20 - 8:22
    intuitive experience
  • 8:22 - 8:24
    of the listener.
  • 8:25 - 8:28
    Let's have a look at the third question.
  • 8:29 - 8:32
    It's a line from a poem by John Keats.
  • 8:32 - 8:35
    Words, like numbers,
  • 8:35 - 8:38
    express fundamental relationships
  • 8:38 - 8:40
    between objects
  • 8:40 - 8:42
    and events and forces
  • 8:42 - 8:44
    that constitute our world.
  • 8:44 - 8:47
    It stands to reason that we, existing in this world,
  • 8:47 - 8:49
    should in the course of our lives
  • 8:49 - 8:52
    absorb intuitively those relationships.
  • 8:52 - 8:55
    And poets, like other artists,
  • 8:55 - 8:58
    play with those intuitive understandings.
  • 8:58 - 9:01
    In the case of hare,
  • 9:01 - 9:03
    it's an ambiguous sound in English.
  • 9:03 - 9:06
    It can also mean the fibers that grow from a head.
  • 9:06 - 9:08
    And if we think of that --
  • 9:08 - 9:10
    let me put the picture up --
  • 9:10 - 9:13
    the fibers represent vulnerability.
  • 9:14 - 9:17
    They yield to the slightest movement
  • 9:17 - 9:20
    or motion or emotion.
  • 9:20 - 9:24
    So what you have is an atmosphere
  • 9:24 - 9:26
    of vulnerability and tension.
  • 9:26 - 9:28
    The hare itself, the animal --
  • 9:28 - 9:31
    not a cat, not a dog, a hare --
  • 9:31 - 9:33
    why a hare?
  • 9:33 - 9:35
    Because think of the picture --
  • 9:35 - 9:37
    not the word, the picture.
  • 9:37 - 9:39
    The overlong ears,
  • 9:39 - 9:41
    the overlarge feet,
  • 9:41 - 9:44
    helps us to picture, to feel intuitively,
  • 9:44 - 9:47
    what it means to limp
  • 9:47 - 9:50
    and to tremble.
  • 9:50 - 9:52
    So in these few minutes,
  • 9:52 - 9:54
    I hope I've been able to share
  • 9:54 - 9:57
    a little bit of my vision of things
  • 9:57 - 10:00
    and to show you
  • 10:00 - 10:03
    that words can have colors and emotions,
  • 10:03 - 10:06
    numbers, shapes and personalities.
  • 10:06 - 10:08
    The world is richer,
  • 10:08 - 10:10
    vaster
  • 10:10 - 10:13
    than it too often seems to be.
  • 10:13 - 10:16
    I hope that I've given you the desire
  • 10:16 - 10:19
    to learn to see the world with new eyes.
  • 10:19 - 10:21
    Thank you.
  • 10:21 - 10:32
    (Applause)
Title:
Different ways of knowing
Speaker:
Daniel Tammet
Description:

Daniel Tammet has linguistic, numerical and visual synesthesia -- meaning that his perception of words, numbers and colors are woven together into a new way of perceiving and understanding the world. The author of "Born on a Blue Day," Tammet shares his art and his passion for languages in this glimpse into his beautiful mind.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
10:33
TED edited English subtitles for Different ways of knowing
TED added a translation

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions