Return to Video

How risk-taking changes a teenager's brain

  • 0:01 - 0:05
    Have you ever tried
    to understand a teenager?
  • 0:05 - 0:07
    It's exhausting, right?
  • 0:07 - 0:12
    You must be puzzled by the fact
    that some teens do well in school,
  • 0:12 - 0:14
    lead clubs and teams
  • 0:14 - 0:16
    and volunteer in their communities,
  • 0:16 - 0:19
    but they eat Tide Pods
    for an online challenge,
  • 0:20 - 0:22
    speed and text while driving,
  • 0:22 - 0:24
    binge drink,
  • 0:24 - 0:26
    and experiment with illicit drugs?
  • 0:26 - 0:32
    How can so many teens be so smart,
    skilled and responsible,
  • 0:32 - 0:35
    and careless risk-takers at the same time?
  • 0:35 - 0:37
    When I was 16,
  • 0:37 - 0:40
    while frequently observing
    my peers in person
  • 0:40 - 0:42
    as well as on social media,
  • 0:42 - 0:47
    I began to wonder why so many teens
    took such crazy risks?
  • 0:47 - 0:51
    It seems like getting a certificate
    from DARE class in the fifth grade
  • 0:51 - 0:53
    can't stop them.
  • 0:53 - 0:54
    (Laughter)
  • 0:54 - 0:55
    What was even more alarming to me
  • 0:55 - 0:59
    was that the more they exposed
    themselves to these harmful risks,
  • 0:59 - 1:03
    the easier it became for them
    to continue taking risks.
  • 1:03 - 1:05
    Now, this confused me,
  • 1:05 - 1:08
    but it also made me incredibly curious.
  • 1:08 - 1:13
    So, as someone with a name that literally
    means "to explore knowledge,"
  • 1:13 - 1:16
    I started searching
    for a scientific explanation.
  • 1:17 - 1:20
    Now, it's no secret that teens
    ages 13 to 18 are more prone
  • 1:20 - 1:24
    to risk-taking than children or adults,
  • 1:24 - 1:27
    but what makes them so daring?
  • 1:27 - 1:29
    Do they suddenly become reckless,
  • 1:29 - 1:32
    or is this just a natural phase
    that they're going through?
  • 1:32 - 1:35
    Well, neuroscientists
    have already found evidence
  • 1:35 - 1:37
    that the teen brain is still
    in the process of maturation
  • 1:37 - 1:41
    and that this makes them
    exceptionally poor at decision-making,
  • 1:41 - 1:45
    causing them to fall prey
    to risky behaviors.
  • 1:45 - 1:48
    But in that case,
    if the maturing brain is to blame,
  • 1:48 - 1:51
    then why are teens
    more vulnerable than children
  • 1:51 - 1:54
    even though their brains
    are more developed than those of children?
  • 1:54 - 1:58
    Also, not all teens in the world
    take risks at the same level.
  • 1:58 - 2:01
    Are there some other underlying
    or unintentional causes
  • 2:01 - 2:04
    driving them to risk-taking?
  • 2:04 - 2:08
    Well, this is exactly
    what I decided to research.
  • 2:08 - 2:12
    So, I found in my research
    on the basis of a psychological process
  • 2:12 - 2:13
    known as "habituation,"
  • 2:13 - 2:18
    or simply what we refer to as
    "getting used to it."
  • 2:18 - 2:21
    Habituation explains how our brains
    adapt to some behaviors,
  • 2:21 - 2:25
    like lying, with repeated exposures.
  • 2:25 - 2:30
    And this concept inspired me
    to design a project
  • 2:30 - 2:31
    to determine if the same principle
  • 2:31 - 2:34
    could be applied to the relentless
    rise of risk-taking in teenagers.
  • 2:34 - 2:38
    So I predicted that
    habituation to risk-taking
  • 2:38 - 2:42
    may have the potential to change
    the already-vulnerable teenage brain
  • 2:42 - 2:44
    by blunting or even eradicating
  • 2:44 - 2:47
    the negative emotions
    associated with risk,
  • 2:47 - 2:49
    like fear or guilt.
  • 2:49 - 2:52
    I also thought because they
    would feel less fearful and guilty,
  • 2:52 - 2:56
    this desensitization would lead them
    to even more risk-taking.
  • 2:56 - 3:00
    In short, I wanted to conduct
    a research study to answer
  • 3:00 - 3:01
    one big question:
  • 3:01 - 3:04
    why do teens keep making
    outrageous choices
  • 3:04 - 3:07
    that are harmful
    to their health and well-being?
  • 3:08 - 3:10
    But there was one big obstacle in my way.
  • 3:10 - 3:12
    To investigate this problem,
  • 3:12 - 3:15
    I needed teenagers to experiment on,
  • 3:15 - 3:19
    laboratories and devices
    to measure their brain activity,
  • 3:19 - 3:23
    and teachers or professors to supervise me
    and guide me along the way.
  • 3:23 - 3:25
    I needed resources.
  • 3:25 - 3:31
    But, you see, I attended a high school
    in South Dakota with limited opportunity
  • 3:31 - 3:32
    for scientific exploration.
  • 3:32 - 3:35
    My school had athletics,
  • 3:35 - 3:38
    band, choir, debate, and other clubs,
  • 3:38 - 3:42
    but there were no STEM programs
    or research ??.
  • 3:42 - 3:46
    And the notion of high schoolers
    doing research or participating
  • 3:46 - 3:47
    in a science fair was completely foreign.
  • 3:47 - 3:52
    Simply put, I didn't exactly have
    the ingredients to make
  • 3:52 - 3:55
    a chef-worthy dish.
  • 3:55 - 3:57
    And these obstacles were frustrating,
  • 3:57 - 4:00
    but I was also a stubborn teenager,
  • 4:00 - 4:03
    and as the daughter
    of Bangladeshi immigrants
  • 4:03 - 4:06
    and one of just a handful
    of Muslim students in my high school
  • 4:06 - 4:07
    in South Dakota,
  • 4:07 - 4:10
    I often struggled to fit in,
  • 4:10 - 4:14
    and I wanted to be someone with something
    to contribute to society,
  • 4:14 - 4:17
    not just be deemed
    the scarf-wearing brown girl
  • 4:17 - 4:20
    who was an anomaly
    in my homogenous hometown.
  • 4:20 - 4:22
    I hope that by doing this research,
  • 4:22 - 4:24
    I could establish this
  • 4:24 - 4:24
    and how valuable scientific exploration
    could be for kids like me
  • 4:28 - 4:30
    who didn't necessarily
    find their niche elsewhere.
  • 4:31 - 4:34
    So with limited research opportunities,
  • 4:34 - 4:39
    inventiveness allowed me to overcome
    seemingly impossible obstacles.
  • 4:39 - 4:43
    I became more creative in working
    with a variety of methodologies,
  • 4:43 - 4:46
    materials and subjects.
  • 4:46 - 4:49
    I transformed my unassuming school library
  • 4:49 - 4:51
    into a laboratory
  • 4:51 - 4:53
    and my peers into lab rats.
  • 4:53 - 4:54
    (Laughter)
  • 4:54 - 4:58
    My enthusiastic geography teacher,
  • 4:58 - 5:00
    who also happens to be
    my school's football coach,
  • 5:00 - 5:02
    ended up as my cheerleader,
  • 5:02 - 5:06
    becoming my mentor to sign
    necessary paperwork,
  • 5:06 - 5:08
    and when it became logistically impossible
  • 5:08 - 5:11
    to use a laboratory
    electroencephalography,
  • 5:11 - 5:12
    or EEG,
  • 5:12 - 5:16
    those electrode devices used
    to measure emotional responses,
  • 5:16 - 5:20
    I bought a portable EEG headset
    with my own money,
  • 5:20 - 5:22
    instead of buying the new iPhone X
  • 5:22 - 5:24
    that a lot of kids my age
    were saving up for.
  • 5:24 - 5:27
    So finally I started the research
  • 5:27 - 5:28
    with 86 students,
  • 5:28 - 5:31
    ages 13 to 18 from my high school.
  • 5:31 - 5:34
    Using the computer cubicles
    in my school library,
  • 5:34 - 5:37
    I had them complete a computerized
    decision-making simulation
  • 5:37 - 5:39
    to measure their risk-taking behaviors
  • 5:39 - 5:41
    comparable to ones in the real world,
  • 5:41 - 5:45
    like alcohol use, drug use and gambling.
  • 5:45 - 5:47
    Wearing the EEG headset,
  • 5:47 - 5:51
    the students completed the test
    12 times over three days
  • 5:51 - 5:54
    to mimic repeated risk exposures.
  • 5:54 - 5:57
    A control panel on the EEG headset
  • 5:57 - 5:59
    measured their various
    emotional responses,
  • 5:59 - 6:04
    the tension, interest, excitement,
    frustration, guilt, stress levels,
  • 6:04 - 6:06
    and relaxation.
  • 6:06 - 6:08
    They also rate their emotions
  • 6:08 - 6:11
    on well-validated
    emotion-measuring scales.
  • 6:11 - 6:13
    This meant that I had measured
    the process of habituation
  • 6:13 - 6:16
    and its effects on decision-making.
  • 6:16 - 6:19
    And it took 29 days
    to complete this research,
  • 6:19 - 6:23
    and with months of frantically
    drafting proposals,
  • 6:23 - 6:27
    meticulously computing data
    in a caffeinated daze at 2am,
  • 6:27 - 6:31
    I was able to finalize my results.
  • 6:31 - 6:33
    And the results showed
    that habituation to risk-taking
  • 6:33 - 6:37
    could actually change a teen's brain
    by altering their emotional levels,
  • 6:37 - 6:39
    causing greater risk-taking.
  • 6:40 - 6:43
    The students' emotions that were
    normally associated with risks,
  • 6:43 - 6:47
    like fear, stress, guilt, and nervousness,
  • 6:47 - 6:48
    as well as attention,
  • 6:48 - 6:52
    were high when they were first
    exposed to the risk simulator.
  • 6:52 - 6:56
    This curbed their temptations
    and enforced self-control,
  • 6:56 - 6:59
    which prevented them
    from taking more risks.
  • 6:59 - 7:03
    However, the more they were exposed
    to the risks through the simulator,
  • 7:03 - 7:06
    the less fearful, guilty
    and stressed they became.
  • 7:07 - 7:09
    This caused a situation
  • 7:09 - 7:12
    in which they were no longer able
    to feel the brain's natural fear
  • 7:12 - 7:14
    and caution instincts,
  • 7:14 - 7:17
    and also,
  • 7:17 - 7:19
    because they were teenagers
    and their brains are still underdeveloped,
  • 7:19 - 7:23
    they became more interested and excited
    in thrill-seeking behaviors.
  • 7:24 - 7:26
    So what were the consequences?
  • 7:26 - 7:29
    They lacked self-control
    for logical decision-making,
  • 7:29 - 7:31
    took greater risks,
  • 7:31 - 7:34
    and made more harmful choices.
  • 7:34 - 7:37
    So the developing brain alone
    isn't to blame.
  • 7:37 - 7:41
    The process of habituation also plays
    a key role in risk-taking
  • 7:41 - 7:43
    and risk escalation.
  • 7:43 - 7:45
    Although a teen's willingness to seek risk
  • 7:45 - 7:48
    is largely a result of the structural
    and functional changes
  • 7:48 - 7:51
    associated with their developing brains,
  • 7:51 - 7:54
    the dangerous part that my research
    was able to highlight
  • 7:54 - 7:57
    was that a habituation to risks
  • 7:57 - 7:59
    can actually physically
    change a teen's brain
  • 7:59 - 8:02
    and cause greater risk-taking.
  • 8:02 - 8:05
    So it's the combination
    of the immature teen brain
  • 8:05 - 8:07
    and the impact of habituation
  • 8:07 - 8:11
    that is like a perfect storm
    to create more damaging effects.
  • 8:11 - 8:15
    And this research can help parents
    and the general public
  • 8:15 - 8:18
    understand that teens aren't just
    willfully ignoring warnings
  • 8:18 - 8:21
    or simply defying parents by engaging
    in increasingly more dangerous behavior.
  • 8:21 - 8:27
    The biggest hurdle they're facing
    is their habituation to risks,
  • 8:27 - 8:30
    all the physical, detectable,
  • 8:30 - 8:32
    and emotional functional changes
  • 8:32 - 8:37
    that drive and control and influence
    over-the-top risk-taking.
  • 8:37 - 8:41
    So yes we need policies
    that provide safer environments
  • 8:41 - 8:43
    and limit exposures to high risks,
  • 8:43 - 8:47
    but we also need policies
    that reflect this insight.
  • 8:48 - 8:51
    These results are
    a wake-up call for teens too.
  • 8:51 - 8:54
    It shows them that the natural
    and necessary fear and guilt
  • 8:54 - 8:57
    that protect them from unsafe situations
  • 8:57 - 9:02
    actually become numb when they
    repeatedly choose risky behaviors.
  • 9:02 - 9:05
    So with this hope to share my findings
  • 9:05 - 9:07
    with fellow teenagers and scientists,
  • 9:07 - 9:12
    I took my research
  • 9:12 - 9:13
    to the Intel International Science
    and Engineering Fair, or ISEF,
  • 9:13 - 9:16
    a culmination of over 1,800 students
  • 9:16 - 9:19
    from 75 countries,
    regions and territories,
  • 9:19 - 9:23
    who showcased their cutting edge
    research and inventions.
  • 9:23 - 9:26
    It's like the Olympics of science fair.
  • 9:26 - 9:31
    There, I was able to present my research
    to experts in neuroscience and psychology
  • 9:31 - 9:35
    and garner valuable feedback.
  • 9:35 - 9:37
    But perhaps the most
    memorable moment of the week
  • 9:37 - 9:41
    was when the booming speakers
    suddenly uttered my name
  • 9:41 - 9:42
    during the awards ceremony.
  • 9:42 - 9:45
    I was in such disbelief
    that I questioned myself,
  • 9:45 - 9:47
    was this just another "La La Land" blunder
  • 9:48 - 9:50
    like at the Oscars?
  • 9:50 - 9:51
    (Laughter)
  • 9:51 - 9:53
    Luckily, it wasn't.
  • 9:53 - 9:56
    I really had won first place
    in the category
  • 9:56 - 9:59
    "Behavioral and Social Sciences."
  • 9:59 - 10:02
    (Applause)
  • 10:04 - 10:06
    Needless to say,
  • 10:06 - 10:08
    I was not only thrilled
    to have this recognition,
  • 10:08 - 10:13
    but also the whole experience
    of science fair that validated my efforts
  • 10:13 - 10:16
    keeps my curiosity alive
  • 10:16 - 10:18
    and strengthens my creativity,
  • 10:18 - 10:20
    perseverance, and imagination.
  • 10:21 - 10:25
    This still image of me
    experimenting in my school library
  • 10:25 - 10:26
    may seem ordinary,
  • 10:26 - 10:30
    but to me, it represents
    a sort of inspiration.
  • 10:30 - 10:35
    It reminds me that this process
    taught me to take risks,
  • 10:35 - 10:37
    and I know that might sound
    incredibly ironic,
  • 10:37 - 10:43
    but I took risks realizing
    that unforeseen opportunities
  • 10:43 - 10:44
    often come from risk-taking,
  • 10:44 - 10:47
    not the hazardous,
    negative type that I studied,
  • 10:47 - 10:48
    but the good ones,
  • 10:48 - 10:50
    the positive risks.
  • 10:50 - 10:52
    The more risks I took,
  • 10:52 - 10:57
    the more capable I felt of withstanding
    my unconventional circumstances,
  • 10:57 - 11:02
    leading to more tolerance,
    resilience and patience
  • 11:02 - 11:04
    for completing my project,
  • 11:04 - 11:05
    and these lessons
    have led me to new ideas,
  • 11:05 - 11:08
    like, is the opposite
    of negative risk-taking also true?
  • 11:08 - 11:12
    Can positive risk-taking escalate
    with repeated exposures?
  • 11:12 - 11:17
    Does positive action
    build positive brain functioning?
  • 11:17 - 11:20
    I think I just might have
    my next research idea.
  • 11:20 - 11:27
    (Applause)
Title:
How risk-taking changes a teenager's brain
Speaker:
Kashfia Rahman
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
11:39

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions