The mysterious workings of the adolescent brain
-
0:01 - 0:04Fifteen years ago, it was widely assumed
-
0:04 - 0:06that the vast majority of brain development
-
0:06 - 0:09takes place in the first few years of life.
-
0:09 - 0:11Back then, 15 years ago, we didn't have the ability
-
0:11 - 0:14to look inside the living human brain
-
0:14 - 0:17and track development across the lifespan.
-
0:17 - 0:20In the past decade or so, mainly due to advances
-
0:20 - 0:22in brain imaging technology
-
0:22 - 0:25such as magnetic resonance imaging, or MRI,
-
0:25 - 0:28neuroscientists have started to look inside the living
-
0:28 - 0:30human brain of all ages, and to track changes
-
0:30 - 0:33in brain structure and brain function,
-
0:33 - 0:37so we use structural MRI if you'd like to take a snapshot,
-
0:37 - 0:40a photograph, at really high resolution of the inside
-
0:40 - 0:43of the living human brain, and we can ask questions like,
-
0:43 - 0:46how much gray matter does the brain contain,
-
0:46 - 0:48and how does that change with age?
-
0:48 - 0:51And we also use functional MRI, called fMRI,
-
0:51 - 0:55to take a video, a movie, of brain activity
-
0:55 - 0:57when participants are taking part in some kind of task
-
0:57 - 1:00like thinking or feeling or perceiving something.
-
1:00 - 1:03So many labs around the world are involved in this kind
-
1:03 - 1:05of research, and we now have a really rich
-
1:05 - 1:09and detailed picture of how the living human brain develops,
-
1:09 - 1:12and this picture has radically changed the way
-
1:12 - 1:15we think about human brain development
-
1:15 - 1:17by revealing that it's not all over in early childhood,
-
1:17 - 1:20and instead, the brain continues to develop
-
1:20 - 1:24right throughout adolescence and into the '20s and '30s.
-
1:24 - 1:28So adolescence is defined as the period of life that starts
-
1:28 - 1:32with the biological, hormonal, physical changes of puberty
-
1:32 - 1:36and ends at the age at which an individual attains
-
1:36 - 1:39a stable, independent role in society.
-
1:39 - 1:41(Laughter)
-
1:41 - 1:44It can go on a long time. (Laughter)
-
1:44 - 1:47One of the brain regions that changes most dramatically
-
1:47 - 1:50during adolescence is called prefrontal cortex.
-
1:50 - 1:53So this is a model of the human brain,
-
1:53 - 1:56and this is prefrontal cortex, right at the front.
-
1:56 - 1:58Prefrontal cortex is an interesting brain area.
-
1:58 - 2:02It's proportionally much bigger in humans than
-
2:02 - 2:05in any other species, and it's involved in a whole range of
-
2:05 - 2:08high level cognitive functions, things like decision-making,
-
2:08 - 2:11planning, planning what you're going to do tomorrow
-
2:11 - 2:13or next week or next year, inhibiting
-
2:13 - 2:16inappropriate behavior, so stopping yourself saying
-
2:16 - 2:19something really rude or doing something really stupid.
-
2:19 - 2:21It's also involved in social interaction,
-
2:21 - 2:24understanding other people, and self-awareness.
-
2:24 - 2:27So MRI studies looking at the development of this region
-
2:27 - 2:29have shown that it really undergoes dramatic development
-
2:29 - 2:32during the period of adolescence.
-
2:32 - 2:35So if you look at gray matter volume, for example,
-
2:35 - 2:40gray matter volume across age from age four to 22 years
-
2:40 - 2:42increases during childhood, which is what you can see
-
2:42 - 2:46on this graph. It peaks in early adolescence.
-
2:46 - 2:49The arrows indicate peak gray matter volume
-
2:49 - 2:52in prefrontal cortex. You can see that that peak happens
-
2:52 - 2:56a couple of years later in boys relative to girls,
-
2:56 - 2:58and that's probably because boys go through puberty
-
2:58 - 3:00a couple of years later than girls on average,
-
3:00 - 3:03and then during adolescence, there's a significant decline
-
3:03 - 3:06in gray matter volume in prefrontal cortex.
-
3:06 - 3:08Now that might sound bad, but actually this is
-
3:08 - 3:11a really important developmental process, because
-
3:11 - 3:15gray matter contains cell bodies and connections
-
3:15 - 3:19between cells, the synapses, and this decline
-
3:19 - 3:21in gray matter volume during prefrontal cortex
-
3:21 - 3:24is thought to correspond to synaptic pruning,
-
3:24 - 3:27the elimination of unwanted synapses.
-
3:27 - 3:30This is a really important process. It's partly dependent
-
3:30 - 3:33on the environment that the animal or the human is in,
-
3:33 - 3:36and the synapses that are being used are strengthened,
-
3:36 - 3:38and synapses that aren't being used
-
3:38 - 3:41in that particular environment are pruned away.
-
3:41 - 3:43You can think of it a bit like pruning a rosebush.
-
3:43 - 3:46You prune away the weaker branches so that
-
3:46 - 3:49the remaining, important branches, can grow stronger,
-
3:49 - 3:53and this process, which effectively fine-tunes brain tissue
-
3:53 - 3:56according to the species-specific environment,
-
3:56 - 3:58is happening in prefrontal cortex and in other brain regions
-
3:58 - 4:02during the period of human adolescence.
-
4:02 - 4:06So a second line of inquiry that we use to track changes
-
4:06 - 4:09in the adolescent brain is using functional MRI
-
4:09 - 4:12to look at changes in brain activity across age.
-
4:12 - 4:14So I'll just give you an example from my lab.
-
4:14 - 4:17So in my lab, we're interested in the social brain, that is
-
4:17 - 4:21the network of brain regions that we use to understand
-
4:21 - 4:23other people and to interact with other people.
-
4:23 - 4:27So I like to show a photograph of a soccer game
-
4:27 - 4:32to illustrate two aspects of how your social brains work.
-
4:32 - 4:33So this is a soccer game. (Laughter)
-
4:33 - 4:36Michael Owen has just missed a goal, and he's lying
-
4:36 - 4:38on the ground, and the first aspect of the social brain
-
4:38 - 4:41that this picture really nicely illustrates is how automatic
-
4:41 - 4:44and instinctive social emotional responses are,
-
4:44 - 4:47so within a split second of Michael Owen missing this goal,
-
4:47 - 4:49everyone is doing the same thing with their arms
-
4:49 - 4:51and the same thing with their face, even Michael Owen
-
4:51 - 4:53as he slides along the grass, is doing the same thing
-
4:53 - 4:55with his arms, and presumably has a similar
-
4:55 - 4:57facial expression, and the only people who don't
-
4:57 - 5:01are the guys in yellow at the back — (Laughs) —
-
5:01 - 5:03and I think they're on the wrong end of the stadium,
-
5:03 - 5:05and they're doing another social emotional response
-
5:05 - 5:08that we all instantly recognize, and that's the second aspect
-
5:08 - 5:12of the social brain that this picture really nicely illustrates,
-
5:12 - 5:15how good we are at reading other people's behavior,
-
5:15 - 5:18their actions, their gestures, their facial expressions,
-
5:18 - 5:22in terms of their underlying emotions and mental states.
-
5:22 - 5:23So you don't have to ask any of these guys.
-
5:23 - 5:26You have a pretty good idea of what they're feeling
-
5:26 - 5:28and thinking at this precise moment in time.
-
5:28 - 5:30So that's what we're interested in looking at in my lab.
-
5:30 - 5:34So in my lab, we bring adolescents and adults into the lab
-
5:34 - 5:36to have a brain scan, we give them some kind of task
-
5:36 - 5:40that involves thinking about other people, their minds,
-
5:40 - 5:42their mental states, their emotions, and one of the findings
-
5:42 - 5:45that we've found several times now, as have other labs
-
5:45 - 5:48around the world, is part of the prefrontal cortex called
-
5:48 - 5:52medial prefrontal cortex, which is shown in blue on the slide,
-
5:52 - 5:54and it's right in the middle of prefrontal cortex
-
5:54 - 5:57in the midline of your head.
-
5:57 - 6:00This region is more active in adolescents when they make
-
6:00 - 6:02these social decisions and think about other people
-
6:02 - 6:05than it is in adults, and this is actually a meta-analysis
-
6:05 - 6:08of nine different studies in this area from labs around
-
6:08 - 6:11the world, and they all show the same thing, that activity
-
6:11 - 6:14in this medial prefrontal cortex area decreases
-
6:14 - 6:17during the period of adolescence.
-
6:17 - 6:19And we think that might be because adolescents and adults
-
6:19 - 6:22use a different mental approach, a different
-
6:22 - 6:25cognitive strategy, to make social decisions,
-
6:25 - 6:29and one way of looking at that is to do behavioral studies
-
6:29 - 6:31whereby we bring people into the lab and we give them
-
6:31 - 6:33some kind of behavioral task, and I'll just give you
-
6:33 - 6:36another example of the kind of task that we use in my lab.
-
6:36 - 6:39So imagine that you're the participant in one of our
-
6:39 - 6:41experiments. You come into the lab,
-
6:41 - 6:44you see this computerized task.
-
6:44 - 6:46In this task, you see a set of shelves.
-
6:46 - 6:49Now, there are objects on these shelves, on some of them,
-
6:49 - 6:52and you'll notice there's a guy standing behind the set
-
6:52 - 6:56of shelves, and there are some objects that he can't see.
-
6:56 - 6:58They're occluded, from his point of view, with a kind of
-
6:58 - 7:01gray piece of wood.
-
7:01 - 7:04This is the same set of shelves from his point of view.
-
7:04 - 7:08Notice that there are only some objects that he can see,
-
7:08 - 7:10whereas there are many more objects that you can see.
-
7:10 - 7:12Now your task is to move objects around.
-
7:12 - 7:15The director, standing behind the set of shelves,
-
7:15 - 7:17is going to direct you to move objects around,
-
7:17 - 7:19but remember, he's not going to ask you to move objects
-
7:19 - 7:23that he can't see. This introduces a really interesting
-
7:23 - 7:25condition whereby there's a kind of conflict
-
7:25 - 7:28between your perspective and the director's perspective.
-
7:28 - 7:32So imagine he tells you to move the top truck left.
-
7:32 - 7:34There are three trucks there. You're going to instinctively
-
7:34 - 7:36go for the white truck, because that's the top truck
-
7:36 - 7:39from your perspective, but then you have to remember,
-
7:39 - 7:41"Oh, he can't see that truck, so he must mean
-
7:41 - 7:43me to move the blue truck," which is the top truck
-
7:43 - 7:46from his perspective. Now believe it or not,
-
7:46 - 7:49normal, healthy, intelligent adults like you make errors
-
7:49 - 7:52about 50 percent of the time on that kind of trial.
-
7:52 - 7:55They move the white truck instead of the blue truck.
-
7:55 - 7:58So we give this kind of task to adolescents and adults,
-
7:58 - 8:00and we also have a control condition
-
8:00 - 8:04where there's no director and instead we give people a rule.
-
8:04 - 8:06We tell them, okay, we're going to do exactly the same thing
-
8:06 - 8:09but this time there's no director. Instead you've got to
-
8:09 - 8:12ignore objects with the dark gray background.
-
8:12 - 8:15You'll see that this is exactly the same condition, only
-
8:15 - 8:17in the no-director condition they just have to remember
-
8:17 - 8:20to apply this somewhat arbitrary rule, whereas
-
8:20 - 8:22in the director condition, they have to remember
-
8:22 - 8:26to take into account the director's perspective
-
8:26 - 8:30in order to guide their ongoing behavior.
-
8:30 - 8:33Okay, so if I just show you the percentage errors
-
8:33 - 8:35in a large developmental study we did,
-
8:35 - 8:39this is in a study ranging from age seven to adulthood,
-
8:39 - 8:40and what you're going to see is the percentage errors
-
8:40 - 8:42in the adult group in both conditions,
-
8:42 - 8:45so the gray is the director condition, and you see
-
8:45 - 8:48that our intelligent adults are making errors about 50 percent
-
8:48 - 8:50of the time, whereas they make far fewer errors
-
8:50 - 8:53when there's no director present, when they just have
-
8:53 - 8:56to remember that rule of ignoring the gray background.
-
8:56 - 8:58Developmentally, these two conditions develop
-
8:58 - 9:01in exactly the same way. Between late childhood
-
9:01 - 9:03and mid-adolescence, there's an improvement,
-
9:03 - 9:07in other words a reduction of errors, in both of these trials,
-
9:07 - 9:08in both of these conditions.
-
9:08 - 9:10But it's when you compare the last two groups,
-
9:10 - 9:12the mid-adolescent group and the adult group
-
9:12 - 9:15where things get really interesting, because there, there is
-
9:15 - 9:18no continued improvement in the no-director condition.
-
9:18 - 9:21In other words, everything you need to do in order to
-
9:21 - 9:24remember the rule and apply it seems to be fully developed
-
9:24 - 9:26by mid-adolescence, whereas in contrast,
-
9:26 - 9:29if you look at the last two gray bars, there's still
-
9:29 - 9:32a significant improvement in the director condition
-
9:32 - 9:34between mid-adolescence and adulthood, and what
-
9:34 - 9:38this means is that the ability to take into account someone
-
9:38 - 9:41else's perspective in order to guide ongoing behavior,
-
9:41 - 9:43which is something, by the way, that we do in everyday life all
-
9:43 - 9:48the time, is still developing in mid-to-late adolescence.
-
9:48 - 9:51So if you have a teenage son or a daughter and you
-
9:51 - 9:53sometimes think they have problems taking other people's
-
9:53 - 9:57perspectives, you're right. They do. And this is why.
-
9:57 - 10:00So we sometimes laugh about teenagers.
-
10:00 - 10:04They're parodied, sometimes even demonized in the media
-
10:04 - 10:08for their kind of typical teenage behavior. They take risks,
-
10:08 - 10:10they're sometimes moody, they're very self-conscious.
-
10:10 - 10:13I have a really nice anecdote from a friend of mine
-
10:13 - 10:15who said that the thing he noticed most
-
10:15 - 10:18about his teenage daughters before and after puberty
-
10:18 - 10:21was their level of embarrassment in front of him.
-
10:21 - 10:23So, he said, "Before puberty, if my two daughters
-
10:23 - 10:25were messing around in a shop, I'd say, 'Hey,
-
10:25 - 10:27stop messing around and I'll sing your favorite song,'
-
10:27 - 10:29and instantly they'd stop messing around and he'd sing
-
10:29 - 10:32their favorite song. After puberty, that became the threat.
-
10:32 - 10:34(Laughter)
-
10:34 - 10:38The very notion of their dad singing in public
-
10:38 - 10:40was enough to make them behave.
-
10:40 - 10:42So people often ask,
-
10:42 - 10:44"Well, is adolescence a kind of recent phenomenon?
-
10:44 - 10:46Is it something we've invented recently in the West?"
-
10:46 - 10:49And actually, the answer is probably not. There are lots
-
10:49 - 10:52of descriptions of adolescence in history that sound
-
10:52 - 10:55very similar to the descriptions we use today.
-
10:55 - 10:59So there's a famous quote by Shakespeare from "The Winter's Tale"
-
10:59 - 11:02where he describes adolescence as follows:
-
11:02 - 11:04"I would there were no age between ten and
-
11:04 - 11:08three-and-twenty, or that youth would sleep out the rest;
-
11:08 - 11:10for there is nothing in the between but getting wenches
-
11:10 - 11:16with child, wronging the ancientry, stealing, fighting." (Laughter)
-
11:16 - 11:21He then goes on to say, "Having said that, would any
-
11:21 - 11:24but these boiled brains of nineteen and two-and-twenty
-
11:24 - 11:26hunt in this weather?" (Laughter)
-
11:26 - 11:29So almost 400 years ago, Shakespeare was portraying
-
11:29 - 11:32adolescents in a very similar light to the light that we
-
11:32 - 11:35portray them in today, but today we try to understand
-
11:35 - 11:38their behavior in terms of the underlying changes
-
11:38 - 11:40that are going on in their brain.
-
11:40 - 11:44So for example, take risk-taking. We know that adolescents
-
11:44 - 11:46have a tendency to take risks. They do.
-
11:46 - 11:49They take more risks than children or adults,
-
11:49 - 11:51and they are particularly prone to taking risks
-
11:51 - 11:54when they're with their friends. There's an important drive
-
11:54 - 11:57to become independent from one's parents
-
11:57 - 12:00and to impress one's friends in adolescence.
-
12:00 - 12:02But now we try to understand that in terms of
-
12:02 - 12:05the development of a part of their brain called the limbic system,
-
12:05 - 12:08so I'm going to show you the limbic system in red
-
12:08 - 12:10in the slide behind me, and also on this brain.
-
12:10 - 12:13So the limbic system is right deep inside the brain,
-
12:13 - 12:16and it's involved in things like emotion processing
-
12:16 - 12:20and reward processing. It gives you the rewarding feeling
-
12:20 - 12:23out of doing fun things, including taking risks.
-
12:23 - 12:25It gives you the kick out of taking risks.
-
12:25 - 12:28And this region, the regions within the limbic system,
-
12:28 - 12:32have been found to be hypersensitive to the rewarding
-
12:32 - 12:35feeling of risk-taking in adolescents compared with adults,
-
12:35 - 12:39and at the very same time, the prefrontal cortex,
-
12:39 - 12:41which you can see in blue in the slide here,
-
12:41 - 12:44which stops us taking excessive risks,
-
12:44 - 12:48is still very much in development in adolescents.
-
12:48 - 12:51So brain research has shown that the adolescent brain
-
12:51 - 12:55undergoes really quite profound development,
-
12:55 - 12:59and this has implications for education, for rehabilitation,
-
12:59 - 13:03and intervention. The environment, including teaching,
-
13:03 - 13:06can and does shape the developing adolescent brain,
-
13:06 - 13:09and yet it's only relatively recently that we have been
-
13:09 - 13:11routinely educating teenagers in the West.
-
13:11 - 13:15All four of my grandparents, for example, left school
-
13:15 - 13:19in their early adolescence. They had no choice.
-
13:19 - 13:22And that's still the case for many, many teenagers
-
13:22 - 13:25around the world today. Forty percent of teenagers
-
13:25 - 13:29don't have access to secondary school education.
-
13:29 - 13:32And yet, this is a period of life where the brain is
-
13:32 - 13:35particularly adaptable and malleable.
-
13:35 - 13:38It's a fantastic opportunity for learning and creativity.
-
13:38 - 13:41So what's sometimes seen as the problem
-
13:41 - 13:43with adolescents — heightened risk-taking, poor impulse
-
13:43 - 13:47control, self-consciousness — shouldn't be stigmatized.
-
13:47 - 13:50It actually reflects changes in the brain that provide
-
13:50 - 13:53an excellent opportunity for education
-
13:53 - 13:57and social development. Thank you. (Applause)
-
13:57 - 14:05(Applause)
- Title:
- The mysterious workings of the adolescent brain
- Speaker:
- Sarah-Jayne Blakemore
- Description:
-
Why do teenagers seem so much more impulsive, so much less self-aware than grown-ups? Cognitive neuroscientist Sarah-Jayne Blakemore compares the prefrontal cortex in adolescents to that of adults, to show us how typically “teenage” behavior is caused by the growing and developing brain.
- Video Language:
- English
- Team:
- closed TED
- Project:
- TEDTalks
- Duration:
- 14:26
Thu-Huong Ha edited English subtitles for The mysterious workings of the adolescent brain | ||
Tamer Abdul Ghani edited English subtitles for The mysterious workings of the adolescent brain | ||
Iryna Melnykevych edited English subtitles for The mysterious workings of the adolescent brain | ||
Iryna Melnykevych edited English subtitles for The mysterious workings of the adolescent brain | ||
Iryna Melnykevych edited English subtitles for The mysterious workings of the adolescent brain | ||
Iryna Melnykevych edited English subtitles for The mysterious workings of the adolescent brain | ||
Hee Jae Ko edited English subtitles for The mysterious workings of the adolescent brain | ||
Thu-Huong Ha approved English subtitles for The mysterious workings of the adolescent brain |