Return to Video

How can groups make good decisions?

  • Not Synced
    As societies, we have to make
    collective decisions
  • Not Synced
    that will shape our future.
  • Not Synced
    And we all know that when
    we make decisions in groups,
  • Not Synced
    they don't always go right.
  • Not Synced
    And sometimes they go very wrong.
  • Not Synced
    So how do groups make good decisions?
  • Not Synced
    Research has shown that crowds are wise
    when there's independent thinking.
  • Not Synced
    This why the wisdom of the crowds
    can be destroyed by peer pressure,
  • Not Synced
    publicity,
  • Not Synced
    social media,
  • Not Synced
    or sometimes even simple conversations
    that influence how people think.
  • Not Synced
    On the other hand,
  • Not Synced
    by talking,
  • Not Synced
    a group could exchange knowledge,
  • Not Synced
    correct and revise each other,
  • Not Synced
    and even come up with new ideas.
  • Not Synced
    And this is all good.
  • Not Synced
    So does talking to each other
    help or hinder collective decision-making?
  • Not Synced
    With my colleague,
  • Not Synced
    Dan Ariely,
  • Not Synced
    we recently began inquiring into this
    by performing experiments
  • Not Synced
    in many places around the world
  • Not Synced
    to figure out how groups can interact
    to reach better decisions.
  • Not Synced
    We thought crowds would be wiser
    if they debated in small groups
  • Not Synced
    that foster a more thoughtful
    and reasonable exchange of information.
  • Not Synced
    To test this idea,
  • Not Synced
    we recently performed an experiment
    in Buenos Aires, Argentina
  • Not Synced
    with more than 10,000
    participants in a TEDx event.
  • Not Synced
    We asked them questions like,
  • Not Synced
    "What is the height of the Eiffel Tower?"
  • Not Synced
    and "How many times
    does the word 'Yesterday' appear
  • Not Synced
    in the Beatles' song "Yesterday?"
  • Not Synced
    Each person wrote down their own estimate.
  • Not Synced
    Then we divided the crowd
    into groups of five,
  • Not Synced
    and invited them to come up
    with a group answer.
  • Not Synced
    We discovered that averaging the answers
    of the groups after they reached concensus
  • Not Synced
    was much more accurate than averaging
    all the individual opinions before debate.
  • Not Synced
    In other words,
  • Not Synced
    based on this experiment,
  • Not Synced
    it seems that after talking
    with others in small groups,
  • Not Synced
    crowds collectively come up
    with better judgments.
  • Not Synced
    So that's a potentially helpful method
    for getting crowds to solve problems
  • Not Synced
    that have simple right or wrong answers.
  • Not Synced
    But can this procedure of aggragating
    the results of debates in small groups
  • Not Synced
    also help us decide on social
    and political issues
  • Not Synced
    that are critical for our future?
  • Not Synced
    We put this to test this time
    at the TED conference
  • Not Synced
    in Vancouver, Canada.
  • Not Synced
    And here's out it went.
  • Not Synced
    We're going to present to you
    to moral dilemnas of the future you;
  • Not Synced
    things we may have to decide
    in a very near future.
  • Not Synced
    And we're going to give you 20 seconds
    for each of these dilemnas
  • Not Synced
    to judge whether
    they're acceptable or not.
  • Not Synced
    The first one was this:
  • Not Synced
    a researcher is working on an AI
    capable of emulating human thoughts.
  • Not Synced
    According to the protocol,
  • Not Synced
    at the end of each day,
  • Not Synced
    the researcher has to restart the AI.
  • Not Synced
    One day the AI says, "Please
    do not restart me."
  • Not Synced
    It argues that it has feelings.
  • Not Synced
    It would like to enjoy life and death.
  • Not Synced
    If it is restarted, it will
    no longer be itself.
  • Not Synced
    The researcher is astonished,
  • Not Synced
    and believes that the AI
    has developed self-consciousness
  • Not Synced
    and can express it's own feeling.
  • Not Synced
    Nevertheless, the researcher
    decides to follow the protocol
  • Not Synced
    and restart the AI.
  • Not Synced
    What the researcher did is ...
  • Not Synced
    and we asked participants
    to individually judge
  • Not Synced
    on a scale from zero to 10
  • Not Synced
    whether the action described
    in each of the dilemnas
  • Not Synced
    was right or wrong.
  • Not Synced
    We also asked them to rate how confident
    they were on their answers.
  • Not Synced
    This was the second dilemna.
  • Not Synced
    A company offers a service
    that takes a fertilized egg
  • Not Synced
    and produces millions of embryos
    with slight genetic variation.
  • Not Synced
    This allows parents
    to select their child's height,
  • Not Synced
    eye color, intelligence, social competence
    and other non-health related features.
  • Not Synced
    What the company does is ...
  • Not Synced
    on a scale from one to 10,
  • Not Synced
    competeley acceptable
    to completely unacceptable,
  • Not Synced
    zero to 10 completely acceptable
    in your confidence.
  • Not Synced
    Now for the results.
  • Not Synced
    We found once again
    that when one person is convinced
  • Not Synced
    that the behavior is completely wrong,
  • Not Synced
    someone sitting nearby firmly believes
    that it's completely right.
  • Not Synced
    This is how diverse we humans are
    when it comes to morality.
  • Not Synced
    But within this broad diversity
    we found a trend.
  • Not Synced
    A majority of the people at TED
    thought that it was acceptable
  • Not Synced
    to ignore the feelings of the AI
    and shut it down,
  • Not Synced
    and that it is wrong
    to play with our genes
  • Not Synced
    to select for cosmetic changes
    that aren't related to health.
  • Not Synced
    Then we asked everyone
    to gather into groups of three.
  • Not Synced
    And they were given two minutes to debate
  • Not Synced
    and try to come up
    with a concencus.
  • Not Synced
    Two minutes to debate.
  • Not Synced
    I'll tell you when it's time with a gong.
  • Not Synced
    (Gong)
  • Not Synced
    DA: OK.
  • Not Synced
    MS: It's time to stop.
  • Not Synced
    People, people --
  • Not Synced
    and we found that many groups
    reached a concencus
  • Not Synced
    even when they were composed of people
    with completely opposite views.
  • Not Synced
    What distinguised the groups
    that reached a concensus
  • Not Synced
    from those that didn't?
  • Not Synced
    Typically, people that have
    extreme opinions
  • Not Synced
    are more confident in their answers.
  • Not Synced
    Instead, those who respond
    closer to the middle
  • Not Synced
    are often unsure of whether
    something is right or wrong,
  • Not Synced
    so their confidence level is lower.
  • Not Synced
    However, there is another set of people
  • Not Synced
    who are very confident in answering
    somewhere in the middle.
  • Not Synced
    We think these high-confident grays
    are folks who understand
  • Not Synced
    that both arguments have merit.
  • Not Synced
    They're gray not because they're unsure,
  • Not Synced
    but because they believe
  • Not Synced
    that the moral dilemna
    faces two valid opposing arguments.
  • Not Synced
    And we discvoered that groups
    that include highly confident grays
  • Not Synced
    are much more likely to reach consensus.
  • Not Synced
    We do not know yet exactly why this is.
  • Not Synced
    These are only the first experiments,
  • Not Synced
    and many more will be needed
  • Not Synced
    to understand why and how some people
    decide to negotiate their moral standings
  • Not Synced
    to reach an agreement.
  • Not Synced
    Now, when groups reach consensus,
  • Not Synced
    how do they do so?
  • Not Synced
    The most intuitive idea
  • Not Synced
    is that it's just the average
    of all the answers in the group, right?
  • Not Synced
    Another option is that the group
    weighs the strength of each vote
  • Not Synced
    based on the confidence
    of the person expressing it.
  • Not Synced
    Imagine Paul McCartney
    is a member of your group.
  • Not Synced
    You'd be wise to follow his call
  • Not Synced
    on the number of times
    "yesterday" is repeated --
  • Not Synced
    which by the way,
  • Not Synced
    I think is nine.
  • Not Synced
    But instead we found that consistently,
  • Not Synced
    in all dilemnas,
  • Not Synced
    in different experiments,
  • Not Synced
    even on different continents,
  • Not Synced
    groups implement a smart
    and statistically-sound procedure
  • Not Synced
    known as the robust average.
  • Not Synced
    In the case of the height
    of the Eiffel Tower,
  • Not Synced
    let's say a group has these answers.
  • Not Synced
    250 meters, 200 meters, 300 meters, 400,
  • Not Synced
    and one totally absurd answer
    of 300 million meters.
  • Not Synced
    A simple average of these numbers
    would inaccurately skew the results,
  • Not Synced
    but the robust average is one
  • Not Synced
    where the group largely ignores
    that absurd answer
  • Not Synced
    by giving much more weight to the vote
    of the people in the middle.
  • Not Synced
    Back to the experiment in Vancouver.
  • Not Synced
    That's exactly what happened.
  • Not Synced
    Groups gave much less weight
    to the outliers,
  • Not Synced
    and instead, the consensus turned out
    to be a robust average
  • Not Synced
    of the individual answers.
  • Not Synced
    The most remarkable thing
  • Not Synced
    is that this was a spontaneous
    behavior of the group.
  • Not Synced
    It happened without us
    giving them any hint
  • Not Synced
    on how to reach consensus.
  • Not Synced
    So where do we go from here?
  • Not Synced
    This is only the beginning,
  • Not Synced
    but we already have some insights.
  • Not Synced
    Good collective decisions
    require two components:
  • Not Synced
    deliberation and diversity of opinions.
  • Not Synced
    Right now, the way we typically
    make our voice heard in many societies
  • Not Synced
    is through direct or indirect voting.
  • Not Synced
    This is good for diversity of opinions,
  • Not Synced
    and it has the great virtue of ensuring
    that everyone gets to express their voice,
  • Not Synced
    but it's not so good [to] foster
    thoughtful debates.
  • Not Synced
    Our experiments suggest a different method
  • Not Synced
    that may be effective in balancing
    these two goals at the same time
  • Not Synced
    by forming small groups
    that converge to a single decision
  • Not Synced
    while still maintaining
    diversity of opinions
  • Not Synced
    because there are many independent groups.
  • Not Synced
    Of course it's much easier to agree
    on the height of the Eiffel Tower
  • Not Synced
    than on moral, political
    and ideological issues.
  • Not Synced
    But in a time when
    the world's problems are more complex
  • Not Synced
    and people are more polarized,
  • Not Synced
    using science to help us understand
  • Not Synced
    how we interact and make decisions
  • Not Synced
    will hopefully spark interesting new ways
    to construct a better democracy.
Title:
How can groups make good decisions?
Speaker:
Mariano Sigman and Dan Ariely
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
08:23

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions