Teaching Math to Dyslexic Students - Dr Steve Chinn
-
0:00 - 0:03What an incredible delight today to have
-
0:03 - 0:05Steve Chinn with us, he is one of the
-
0:05 - 0:07worlds authorities on teaching Math to
-
0:07 - 0:10Dyslexic students. He has a number of books
-
0:10 - 0:12that are available to Amazon and other
-
0:12 - 0:14places that I think many of you will find
-
0:14 - 0:19very useful. Steve has his doctorate in
-
0:19 - 0:22Applied Physics, but has had many years as
-
0:22 - 0:26teaching headmaster of Mathematics and
-
0:26 - 0:28Science, it's a real pleasure to welcome
-
0:28 - 0:30him today, and we look forward to hearing
-
0:30 - 0:32everything he has to tell us about teaching
-
0:32 - 0:33Math.
-
0:33 - 0:36Thank you for that, let me see if I can do
-
0:36 - 0:41this...A quick overview: I am not, in 45
-
0:41 - 0:45minutes be able to deal with everything,
-
0:45 - 0:47but I want to give you an overview, give
-
0:47 - 0:49you some practical examples and
-
0:49 - 0:52illustrations of what I think are the
-
0:52 - 0:56principals of teaching Math to dyslexic
-
0:56 - 0:59students. It's no good trying to use the
-
0:59 - 1:03traditional methods, whatever they are.
-
1:03 - 1:06In my experience, there are no quick fixes,
-
1:06 - 1:11as there aren't any for reading dyslexia.
-
1:11 - 1:15In many cases, it's about starting the math
-
1:15 - 1:19in the place where the problems become
-
1:19 - 1:22apparent, and tracking back quite a long
-
1:22 - 1:25way before you get to a place where they
-
1:25 - 1:29all disappear. Or often in children that
-
1:29 - 1:32miss some of the few concepts, I'll talk
-
1:32 - 1:34about that today. Of course we have to
-
1:34 - 1:38remember that all students are individuals.
-
1:38 - 1:41I've looked at the factors that are
-
1:41 - 1:44involved, I've build them into the talk
-
1:44 - 1:46rather than just list them at the start,
-
1:46 - 1:48things that might create barriers to
-
1:48 - 1:49learning & I'll break at various points so
-
1:49 - 1:53that you can ask questions. I'll do my best
-
1:53 - 1:55to answer. If I think it's a long answer, I
-
1:55 - 1:59might put you off until the end of the
-
1:59 - 2:03session. So, let's get going, and see if I
-
2:03 - 2:06can start at the beginning with some
-
2:06 - 2:09guiding principles and quotes. Let me
-
2:09 - 2:13start with a lovely saying of Margaret
-
2:13 - 2:16Rawson, a pioneer in the studying of
-
2:16 - 2:19Dyslexia, from the U.S.A., a lady who
-
2:19 - 2:21lived to be 104.
-
2:21 - 2:24"Teach the subject as it is to the child as
-
2:24 - 2:26she (or he) is."
-
2:26 - 2:29Just such wise words, in a sense,
-
2:29 - 2:33summarizes what I am trying to do, today.
-
2:33 - 2:34Something I once said:
-
2:34 - 2:38"It's quite complicated making things simple."
-
2:38 - 2:42Quite often, people don't realize what
-
2:42 - 2:46they think is simple, can be complicated to
-
2:46 - 2:48make it simple, to do.
-
2:48 - 2:50So once that's said,
-
2:50 - 2:52"It's usually best to start at the beginning
-
2:52 - 2:54and to be aware of where this stuff
-
2:54 - 2:56is heading." ..."Stuff" is a technical
-
2:56 - 2:58term I'll use for mathematics.
-
2:58 - 3:00I want to look at the
-
3:00 - 3:02development trajectory in maths, which
-
3:02 - 3:06sometimes means you start looking back,
-
3:06 - 3:08and sometimes means you go forward.
-
3:08 - 3:10You have do both of those things.
-
3:10 - 3:15A little bit of background here, relavent
-
3:15 - 3:18to our populations. The NRC, National
-
3:18 - 3:21Research Council for the U.S.A., a book
-
3:21 - 3:24that was published in 2000, called"How
-
3:24 - 3:28People Learn", a wonderful book, even more
-
3:28 - 3:30wonderful in that it has just 3 key findings:
-
3:30 - 3:34kind of succinct! The first of those key
-
3:34 - 3:37findings is this one: Students don't come
-
3:37 - 3:43into classrooms with an empty head, they
-
3:43 - 3:46have preconceptions about how the world
-
3:46 - 3:49works. It happens to be the case, how
-
3:49 - 3:52Mathematics works. You've got to deal with
-
3:52 - 3:54that right at the start, or they won't
-
3:54 - 3:56pick up the new concepts that you're trying
-
3:56 - 3:59to explain to them, and the information
-
3:59 - 4:02which you are giving! Or, which often
-
4:02 - 4:05happens, they may learn them just for the
-
4:05 - 4:08purposes of the test, maybe just read them,
-
4:08 - 4:10but only for 24 hours, and then go back to
-
4:10 - 4:13their original learnings and
-
4:13 - 4:15preconceptions, that they brought to the
-
4:15 - 4:20classrooms. This, in fact was known , in
-
4:20 - 4:23a really good research paper in a
-
4:23 - 4:30monograph from Chicago way back in 1925:
-
4:30 - 4:33When we learn something that is new to us,
-
4:33 - 4:38what we learn is a ~dominant entry~ to the
-
4:38 - 4:41brain. Quite often when we are teaching
-
4:41 - 4:43maths, we've got to do some unteaching.
-
4:43 - 4:46It's one of the very good reasons for using
-
4:46 - 4:50manipulatives and visual images, to
-
4:50 - 4:52try and create new images for teaching. So,
-
4:52 - 4:56that's the "so what?" of this. We're not
-
4:56 - 4:59just starting from scratch, we often have
-
4:59 - 5:02to work with things that are, maybe, not
-
5:02 - 5:07right. Then maths has an emotive, an
-
5:07 - 5:10emotional side. There aren't many subjects
-
5:10 - 5:14where people write books about anxiety. You
-
5:14 - 5:16get books on maths anxiety. I guess,
-
5:16 - 5:19less books on geography anxiety, art
-
5:19 - 5:25anxiety. A lot of the times this is down to
-
5:25 - 5:28children giving an illusion of learning.
-
5:28 - 5:31Children with good memories often start off
-
5:31 - 5:34doing quite well in maths. But then as that
-
5:34 - 5:37load on memory gets greater and greater,
-
5:37 - 5:41they tend not to fair as well, because the
-
5:41 - 5:44memory often stops them from understanding,
-
5:44 - 5:48as the understanding then helps the memory.
-
5:48 - 5:50This is one of the strengths for
-
5:50 - 5:54dyslexics. When I lecture to teachers, as
-
5:54 - 5:57Fernette said, I do this all around the
-
5:57 - 6:02world, I often ask a vague question, that
-
6:02 - 6:06is, "At what age do enough children give up
-
6:06 - 6:11on maths in your classes for you to notice?
-
6:11 - 6:14The most frequent, popular answer is 7
-
6:14 - 6:19years old. That is a big worry, and I'm not
-
6:19 - 6:23just talking about Dyslexics in this area,
-
6:23 - 6:28an International problem for many children.
-
6:28 - 6:31I think we have to look in maths at the
-
6:31 - 6:34role of failure and getting things wrong
-
6:34 - 6:40because maths is incredibly judgemental. My
-
6:40 - 6:42experience in working with children, and
-
6:42 - 6:46students whatever age, is that failure
-
6:46 - 6:51rarely motivates. People maneuver to avoid
-
6:51 - 6:54failure in a number of ways. One of the
-
6:54 - 6:56good ways of avoiding failure is giving up.
-
6:56 - 6:59not trying.
-
6:59 - 7:02But, people will make mistakes, and we have
-
7:02 - 7:05to look at that, and we have to deal with
-
7:05 - 7:08that, within the lessons.
-
7:08 - 7:10So what we are trying to do is to build
-
7:10 - 7:14security. One of the things, in general,
-
7:14 - 7:17when we learn when we are right--it helps
-
7:17 - 7:22us feel secure, is the role of consistency.
-
7:22 - 7:25When things are consistent, means we can
-
7:25 - 7:28often see patterns--it helps us to be secure
-
7:28 - 7:30and when we ar secure, we tend to learn
-
7:30 - 7:32better. We often think
-
7:32 - 7:35that maths is a pretty consistent subject,
-
7:35 - 7:38but like spelling of the English language,
-
7:38 - 7:40which has some rules, but huge numbers of
-
7:40 - 7:48exceptions. Let's look at that right now:
-
7:48 - 7:50That's my first break, so if you have any
-
7:50 - 7:53questions...If you can't hear me, or
-
7:53 - 7:57whatever, lets have a little time now
-
7:57 - 8:00before I launch into the next little bit
-
8:00 - 8:04of the talk, now. I'm going to get going.
-
8:04 - 8:09So, how do we write numbers? Because,
-
8:09 - 8:11ultimately, children have got to learn to
-
8:11 - 8:16write numbers. My grandson, when he was
-
8:16 - 8:21age 2 and 3, was able to say numbers. There
-
8:21 - 8:24is a big difference between saying numbers
-
8:24 - 8:28and writing them. The symbols we use to
-
8:28 - 8:30represent numbers are going to be very
-
8:30 - 8:33important. Particularly for children who
-
8:33 - 8:36have difficulty with the information, as
-
8:36 - 8:39presented...something I guess that dyslexics
-
8:39 - 8:43have in common. I want just to introduce
-
8:43 - 8:46you to the numbers and the symbols to 9.
-
8:46 - 8:49These are symbols for single digit numbers,
-
8:49 - 8:53just one digit to represent 9. I'm going
-
8:53 - 8:56to start in this section via illustrating
-
8:56 - 8:59one of the things that's very important for
-
8:59 - 9:03me, in the philosophy/principles behind
-
9:03 - 9:06the way I teach--that's the question I ask
-
9:06 - 9:10myself: What else am I teaching? What else
-
9:10 - 9:12are my student's learning? Lots of times
-
9:12 - 9:16the actual maths might be stuff these
-
9:16 - 9:19learners have done many years ago. What
-
9:19 - 9:21I am looking for is the way I can teach
-
9:21 - 9:24that in a way that makes it acceptable and
-
9:24 - 9:26understandable to older learners, who might
-
9:26 - 9:29have failed at learning this stuff many
-
9:29 - 9:32times before. So what else am I teaching
-
9:32 - 9:35what else are people learning,
-
9:35 - 9:37when I'm dealing with any topic
-
9:37 - 9:43in maths? I also want to try to make people
-
9:43 - 9:47be more comfortable with numbers. One of
-
9:47 - 9:50the things I'm going to show you in this
-
9:50 - 9:52next section 'what else are you teaching',
-
9:52 - 9:54is showing people how to 'see numbers' in
-
9:54 - 9:57numbers. I'm going to explain that in a
-
9:57 - 10:01moment. So here is a question, you don't
-
10:01 - 10:05have to answer, it's rhetorical...but it is
-
10:05 - 10:08a very key question, in terms of the number
-
10:08 - 10:11system, we use. And that is, what is
-
10:11 - 10:18nothing? What is zero?...Here we go!
-
10:18 - 10:24There's a slide--nothing more. What's very
-
10:24 - 10:28important in a number system is that we
-
10:28 - 10:31actually have a symbol for nothing. There
-
10:31 - 10:37it is..Zero. That's a key symbol, and I'll
-
10:37 - 10:40explain why it is key, and why it is the
-
10:40 - 10:44root of many misconceptions in Mathematics.
-
10:44 - 10:48If I want to generate problems in arithmetic
-
10:48 - 10:53calculations, I put zeroes in the numbers.
-
10:53 - 10:55It really makes things more challenging. So
-
10:55 - 10:57let's look at the next symbols and do a
-
10:57 - 11:00little bit about what else we are teaching.
-
11:00 - 11:04That's the symbol '1' for one thing, the symbol
-
11:04 - 11:08'2' for two things, the symbol '3' for three
-
11:08 - 11:13things. I'm using the red color for the
-
11:13 - 11:16odd numbers, and blue for the even numbers.
-
11:16 - 11:19I'm not going to deal with that right now,
-
11:19 - 11:21but it would be a 'what else are you
-
11:21 - 11:25teaching' topic. So, I've got to four. It
-
11:25 - 11:29would depend on the learners--I might say,
-
11:29 - 11:33"Now look at this four. It's two plus two.
-
11:33 - 11:36It's two times two. That's two operations
-
11:36 - 11:38I've talked about; addition and
-
11:38 - 11:41multiplication. It's a square. If it's an
-
11:41 - 11:45older learner, I might say that we represent
-
11:45 - 11:47four as two, with a small two up here,
-
11:47 - 11:51meaning 2 squared. Wouldn't do that with a
-
11:51 - 11:55six year old. This is how you can take
-
11:55 - 11:58basic information, support all principles,
-
11:58 - 12:02early learning principles and make them
-
12:02 - 12:07more age appropriate for children. Let's
-
12:07 - 12:12get 5 up there. You'll notice, I've
-
12:12 - 12:14used a pattern, and it's one that's probably
-
12:14 - 12:18familiar because it's a pattern on dice,
-
12:18 - 12:22playing cards, dominoes. I might also just
-
12:22 - 12:25use this as a random pattern to just check
-
12:25 - 12:29out other issues with recognizing quantity.
-
12:29 - 12:33Right now my agenda is to show a couple of
-
12:33 - 12:38specific points. The pattern I'm using,
-
12:38 - 12:41here is the difference. This is not the
-
12:41 - 12:44pattern you see on cards, dominoes, and
-
12:44 - 12:48dice. This "What else you are teaching here"
-
12:48 - 12:52is I'm showing that 6, the number within
-
12:52 - 12:58numbers, is made up of 5 and 1. I was
-
12:58 - 13:02also showing that 3 is made up of 2 and 1.
-
13:02 - 13:07A lot of the time learners who don't have
-
13:07 - 13:09good number sense, I'm going to build that
-
13:09 - 13:13number sense by using key numbers.
-
13:13 - 13:17One, two, and five. Then, I'm going to look
-
13:17 - 13:20for the key numbers in other numbers. So
-
13:20 - 13:25that was my 6: it's 5 and 1. My 7 is 5
-
13:25 - 13:31and 2. Eight, is 5 and 3. If I move this
-
13:31 - 13:36counter (I would use real counters...get
-
13:36 - 13:39some tactile experience)...If I move this
-
13:39 - 13:43one to ~here~, I would have 2 and 2 and 2
-
13:43 - 13:47and 2. So that 2 plus 2 plus 2 plus 2, we
-
13:47 - 13:51have 8. (Eight up there...) I would have 4
-
13:51 - 13:55plus 4. I would have 2 cubed, if I was
-
13:55 - 13:58working with older learners. Four times two
-
13:58 - 14:032 x 2 x 2 x 2. So again, it is using numbers
-
14:03 - 14:06where hopefully the numbers are not a
-
14:06 - 14:08barrier to illustrate some of the
-
14:08 - 14:10mathematical operations, some of the
-
14:10 - 14:14principals. Eight...now we want to get 9.
-
14:14 - 14:18There's 9. I'm still in single digits. I
-
14:18 - 14:21haven't got to 10 yet. What I've done, I've
-
14:21 - 14:26introduced the learner to all 10 symbols we
-
14:26 - 14:32use for making any number. I've done that by
-
14:32 - 14:35by counting forwards and adding one on
-
14:35 - 14:38every time. I'll move through this next bit
-
14:38 - 14:43a little quicker. Quite often we focus on
-
14:43 - 14:46counting forward, and therefore develop
-
14:46 - 14:48additions skills, which tends to be the
-
14:48 - 14:51default operation. For learners who are
-
14:51 - 14:54struggling with maths.But shouldn't we also
-
14:54 - 14:57introduce counting backwards because when
-
14:57 - 15:00we count backwards, we learn (let's keep
-
15:00 - 15:03that gentleman for a moment)...we learn
-
15:03 - 15:07about taking away. This flexibility and
-
15:07 - 15:12seeing numbers in numbers, in interrelated
-
15:12 - 15:16addition and subtraction...that's a skill that
-
15:16 - 15:19some people have almost intuitively. They
-
15:19 - 15:22have an intuitive number sense. In some
-
15:22 - 15:25research I did when I worked in the states
-
15:25 - 15:28back in the 80's, we, my colleagues
-
15:28 - 15:30Dwight Knox and John Bath,
-
15:30 - 15:34called those people grasshoppers. People
-
15:34 - 15:37who take numbers very literally-- they see
-
15:37 - 15:409 as 9, they see 4 as 4--they don't see it
-
15:40 - 15:46as 1 less than 5...we call those inchworms.
-
15:46 - 15:48I just popped this little slide in here,
-
15:48 - 15:51this grasshopper is scratching his head,
-
15:51 - 15:55just to give you a little image, we are trying to get
-
15:55 - 15:58some intuitive thinking, some good number
-
15:58 - 16:02sense, some grasshopper skills. So to come
-
16:02 - 16:06back, we take away one every time. We are
-
16:06 - 16:11introducing subtraction. The counting goal
-
16:11 - 16:14was introducing addition, adding one
-
16:14 - 16:16every time, we want to get better at
-
16:16 - 16:19adding one. Subtraction we are introducing
-
16:19 - 16:23as taking a whole number away every time,
-
16:23 - 16:26so in this particular example the quantity
-
16:26 - 16:29is getting smaller, and we are returning to
-
16:29 - 16:34zero. Now with some examples of what else
-
16:34 - 16:37we are teaching. What I have done, is count
-
16:37 - 16:41to 9 and come back again.
-
16:41 - 16:46In unsophisticated use of quantity, you can
-
16:46 - 16:50use tallies. If you see learners using lots
-
16:50 - 16:53and lots of tallies, that's worrying,
-
16:53 - 16:56because it means they are not understanding
-
16:56 - 17:00how to group numbers, they are only seeing
-
17:00 - 17:04numbers in ones, and that is very limiting.
-
17:04 - 17:07Their mathematics is not going to progress.
-
17:07 - 17:09So you see something like this, just
-
17:09 - 17:12a string of tallies, very rarely are they
-
17:12 - 17:17grouped...just strings of tallies. The less
-
17:17 - 17:21you'll remember, you won't remember how
-
17:21 - 17:24many are there just by looking. Back to
-
17:24 - 17:26Forty three just happened to be
-
17:26 - 17:29how many I could squeeze in on this
-
17:29 - 17:34particular side. Now I'm going to show you
-
17:34 - 17:3932 tallies. What I've done is grouped in 5
-
17:39 - 17:43and 5 and 5 and 5 and 5 and 5, I'm going to
-
17:43 - 17:47group them again, in 10's. ~Now~ I can see
-
17:47 - 17:5510-20-30 and 2. So number systems will
-
17:55 - 17:59eventually group ones into quantities. More
-
17:59 - 18:03often than not those quantities are tens,
-
18:03 - 18:07then we have a two units left over,
-
18:07 - 18:09and that's 32.
-
18:09 - 18:17Why do we use 10 as this grouping quantity?
-
18:17 - 18:20Remember, the answer is here: we have 10
-
18:20 - 18:25fingers on two hands. Five and five. So
-
18:25 - 18:30hands go '1', for one digit, '2', for two
-
18:30 - 18:34hands, '5' for the five fingers, and the 10
-
18:34 - 18:38-the core key facts on which we conclude
-
18:38 - 18:42other numbers. To be a little mischievous,
-
18:42 - 18:45let me just ask you a question, that I'm
-
18:45 - 18:47not going to answer. Some of you do
-
18:47 - 18:52research, and when you do, where did 12
-
18:52 - 18:56come from? It's rather used for time!
-
18:56 - 19:00Some of you do research! So this
-
19:00 - 19:03idea of clustering numbers into groups to
-
19:03 - 19:06make counting and manipulation of numbers
-
19:06 - 19:10easier-- well, 3000 years ago the Egyptians
-
19:10 - 19:13used this symbol [image] to represent
-
19:13 - 19:1710 things. They used this symbol [image]
-
19:17 - 19:20to represent 100 things. A couple of
-
19:20 - 19:24thousand years ago, the Romans used an 'x'
-
19:24 - 19:28to represent 10, and a 'C' to represent 100,
-
19:28 - 19:31and we still see 'C' in century and cent.
-
19:31 - 19:35We still see that today.
-
19:35 - 19:38In the number system we use, the Hindu-
-
19:38 - 19:41Arabic system, you're familiar with seeing
-
19:41 - 19:44one-zero (10), and one-zero-zero (100).
-
19:44 - 19:47Where these symbols are quite distinct, we
-
19:47 - 19:51are only using 2 of those symbols are
-
19:51 - 19:53introduced while we are on this session,
-
19:53 - 19:56one followed by zero, and a one followed by
-
19:56 - 19:58two zeroes. What seems to be common, is
-
19:58 - 20:03that the tally is I, and the one unit is
-
20:03 - 20:06[inaudible]
-
20:06 - 20:10So, I've introduced the idea of using one
-
20:10 - 20:14tally. We have also introduced the idea of
-
20:14 - 20:219 single digit numbers, symbols of 1,2,3,4,
-
20:21 - 20:265,6,7,8, 9. The key moment when we are
-
20:26 - 20:30teaching mathematics, is this change from
-
20:30 - 20:35single digit, 9- to double digits, 10. Here is
-
20:35 - 20:39the 10, two 5's....here the digits go from
-
20:39 - 20:459 to 10. Beneath the key moment is when we
-
20:45 - 20:47erase that process, and we go back
-
20:47 - 20:52from 10 to 9. That's going to help children
-
20:52 - 20:55do more complicated addition problems,
-
20:55 - 20:59more complicated subtraction problems, and
-
20:59 - 21:02thus, we lead to multiplication. That's a
-
21:02 - 21:05key moment, and it's a very conceptual
-
21:05 - 21:07moment, because we are using one symbol,
-
21:07 - 21:10then we are using two. That's kind of
-
21:10 - 21:14interesting. What makes the Hindu-Arabic
-
21:14 - 21:19system clever is that the digits, the
-
21:19 - 21:26symbols hold places. Here is 9 units, we
-
21:26 - 21:35write 9. When we get to 10, we can't write
-
21:35 - 21:39one, because we've used one in the units.
-
21:39 - 21:41We've got to come up with something that
-
21:41 - 21:48distinguishes one unit from one ten. We use
-
21:48 - 21:541-0, we use 2 symbols. This means we have
-
21:54 - 21:58one 10, it means we have zero units. The
-
21:58 - 22:02implication there is that we have to get there
-
22:02 - 22:04using the right place. If we put a zero
-
22:04 - 22:08here, and a one here, that would mean
-
22:08 - 22:11zero 10's and one unit. It's the order, the
-
22:11 - 22:15sequencing. Children who have problems
-
22:15 - 22:20with sequencing, this can be an issue.
-
22:20 - 22:25Maths isn't always friendly if the learners
-
22:25 - 22:30have issues like sequencing issues. Now
-
22:30 - 22:34you can guess that eleven is going to be
-
22:34 - 22:39one 10, one unit. The symbol allows us to
-
22:39 - 22:49bring the numbers. Unfortunately, learning
-
22:49 - 22:53numbers in English is not a good thing.
-
22:53 - 23:00There are some real vocabulary problems.
-
23:00 - 23:03If I was learning these early numbers in
-
23:03 - 23:07Cantonese, I wouldn't be saying eleven, I'd
-
23:07 - 23:11be saying one ten, one unit. Twelve--
-
23:11 - 23:14one ten, two units. We don't, we say
-
23:14 - 23:18eleven and twelve. The language, vocabulary
-
23:18 - 23:22does not support symbols, does not
-
23:22 - 23:25support the concept. Children learn these
-
23:25 - 23:28things because they practice them, their
-
23:28 - 23:31parents practice them with children, and
-
23:31 - 23:35they give this illusion of one, but they
-
23:35 - 23:40are not learning this key issue of place
-
23:40 - 23:46value. This one, is different than the place
-
23:46 - 23:49value of this one. In terms of what it
-
23:49 - 23:53conveys, mathematically...that's bad
-
23:53 - 23:58enough, but then we get to the teen numbers
-
23:58 - 24:03and the vocabulary is telling us 3, not
-
24:03 - 24:06very well, and 10, not in the language,
-
24:06 - 24:08but it is suggesting three
-
24:08 - 24:14and ten, but children have to write 1 and 3.
-
24:14 - 24:16Four and ten, children have to have to write
-
24:16 - 24:201 and 4. When children come to practice
-
24:20 - 24:24two digit numbers for the first time, the
-
24:24 - 24:27vocabulary does not support the symbols
-
24:27 - 24:31that they want. A lot of children will have
-
24:31 - 24:35difficulty grasping the concept because
-
24:35 - 24:38language, our medium of communication,
-
24:38 - 24:42is not supporting the concept. This needs to
-
24:42 - 24:45be modeled with materials and visual
-
24:45 - 24:53images, which are not.[inaudible]
-
24:53 - 24:58Let me get to twenty--well, twenty is not
-
24:58 - 25:03too bad it, it suggests 2 in the tens, but
-
25:03 - 25:06then we do say, twenty-one, twenty-two,
-
25:06 - 25:08twenty-three. At least the vocabulary is
-
25:08 - 25:12supporting the concept. I sometimes think
-
25:12 - 25:16maybe we should never teach children these
-
25:16 - 25:19until we teach them 21, 22, 23.
-
25:19 - 25:23That's a little different.
-
25:23 - 25:26So, if you are scared of big numbers, look
-
25:26 - 25:28away now. I want to show you how this
-
25:28 - 25:31system .........[inaudible] just clever,
-
25:31 - 25:35and how, for older learners, the same
-
25:35 - 25:38pattern. It goes on and on...so if you
-
25:38 - 25:40didn't get it in the first learning, you
-
25:40 - 25:42are not going to get the
-
25:42 - 25:44extension of the maths.
-
25:44 - 25:49So, the unit is the base. Ten is 10 units,
-
25:49 - 25:53the next place value. One hundred is 100
-
25:53 - 25:56units. Now, we've moved up to third place
-
25:56 - 25:59value...we have a 3 digit number, a 2
-
25:59 - 26:02digit number, and a 1. As we go up in value,
-
26:02 - 26:05increasing the number of digits. Again,
-
26:05 - 26:07I've modeled this in base 10 which is more
-
26:07 - 26:11likely, these blocks very good material.
-
26:11 - 26:14Then we get to 1000, and 1000 is kind of
-
26:14 - 26:17like a watershed. Up to now, we've done 10
-
26:17 - 26:22units, 100 units, 1000 units. Now, for the
-
26:22 - 26:24~bigger~ numbers, I'm going to work in
-
26:24 - 26:28thousands. So I am going to do 10 thousand,
-
26:28 - 26:32a 100 thousand, and a thousand
-
26:32 - 26:35thousand. So I am using this ten-hundred-
-
26:35 - 26:42thousand sequence--I've got to 1000 again,
-
26:42 - 26:45we have to have a new word, that's a
-
26:45 - 26:49million. Now I will carry on using
-
26:49 - 26:5310 million, 100 million, 1000 million,
-
26:53 - 26:58which, as you know, is a billion. You can
-
26:58 - 27:01work out what a trillion will be. There is
-
27:01 - 27:07a logic in this form. Just to show you how,
-
27:07 - 27:11working with all the learners, I want to
-
27:11 - 27:14use very basic maths, illustrating complex
-
27:14 - 27:19symbols and complex representations. What
-
27:19 - 27:23maths tries to do is represent complicated
-
27:23 - 27:27ideas presciently, and sometimes called
-
27:27 - 27:31very elegant. A hundred is 10 times 10,
-
27:31 - 27:35ten lots of ten. So it is 2 tens multiplied
-
27:35 - 27:39together. Mathmeticians symbolize this with
-
27:39 - 27:43this 10, with this little 2 tucked up here in the
-
27:43 - 27:47top right hand corner, 10 squared. This is
-
27:47 - 27:51very difficult for children with dyspraxia,
-
27:51 - 27:55with untidy writing, sequencing issues,
-
27:55 - 27:59it's making all those demands. This too,
-
27:59 - 28:02has to be small, otherwise it will be 102.
-
28:02 - 28:06It's critical. These are points of danger
-
28:06 - 28:09for learners, and points that teachers have
-
28:09 - 28:11to pre-empt by demonstrating them and
-
28:11 - 28:15making them very clear. A thousand is
-
28:15 - 28:2010 x 10 x10, three times only, three tens
-
28:20 - 28:24all together, so we write it with a 3 up here.
-
28:24 - 28:27There's a logic, there's a pattern. Don't
-
28:27 - 28:30just teach by rote. Ten Thousand is 10 to
-
28:30 - 28:34the 4th, skip a hundred thousand, go on to
-
28:34 - 28:35a million, which is
-
28:35 - 28:4010 x 10 x 10 x 10x 10 x 10. Instead of
-
28:40 - 28:44writing all that, mathematicians write 10
-
28:44 - 28:46to the 6th, and the 6 tells you, there are
-
28:46 - 28:51six 10's multiplied together. Those of you
-
28:51 - 28:54that are looking for patterns, what do you
-
28:54 - 28:57think a mathematician might write,
-
28:57 - 29:02that doesn't [inaudible] might write for 10?
-
29:02 - 29:08Think of the sequence. Four, three, two,
-
29:08 - 29:11the answer is, they write 10 to the
-
29:11 - 29:15power of 1. If you really want to be
-
29:15 - 29:19confused, but there is a logic, for a 1, we
-
29:19 - 29:23write 10 to the power of 0. That's the
-
29:23 - 29:27older learners, the ones doing the [inaudible].
-
29:27 - 29:31Here's another natural break time, I'm
-
29:31 - 29:34ready for some questions if you have them,
-
29:34 - 29:47but equally, I'm ready to go on if you don't.
-
29:47 - 29:51Okay, I'm going on...
-
29:51 - 29:53I want to just look for a moment at number
-
29:53 - 29:56facts, what are sometimes called
-
29:56 - 29:59[inaudible] facts. A great idea that's
-
29:59 - 30:01that's come from some key researchers in
-
30:01 - 30:05the U.S.A., is not to call these facts, but
-
30:05 - 30:08to call them number combinations. When I
-
30:08 - 30:11started working with Dyslexics, thirty or
-
30:11 - 30:14more years ago, I discovered them because
-
30:14 - 30:18my learners taught me. What they didn't
-
30:18 - 30:21remember, they often worked out.
-
30:21 - 30:28If they knew 5 + 5 was 10; 5 + 6 was 11.
-
30:28 - 30:32That's what this number combination means.
-
30:32 - 30:36Children and adults use key parts, to work
-
30:36 - 30:42out all the facts. Back to my 1,2,5 and 10.
-
30:42 - 30:47This is a great skill because for some
-
30:47 - 30:50facts, there's no reason, unless you
-
30:50 - 30:52understand numbers very well why the
-
30:52 - 30:56answer repeats...what it is.
-
30:56 - 31:00Let me explain a little bit about numbers.
-
31:00 - 31:04The research on maths learning, Mathematics
-
31:04 - 31:07learning, what we call the numbers calculating,
-
31:07 - 31:10this is stuff the kid's lack confidence in.
-
31:10 - 31:15It seems to me, from the research, you can
-
31:15 - 31:18do endless practice on this, in some ways,
-
31:18 - 31:23and the entry to the brain, is pretty short
-
31:23 - 31:26lived. The thing I hear time and time again,
-
31:26 - 31:30from parents is:
-
31:30 - 31:32"He learned it last night, but he's
-
31:32 - 31:36forgotten it this morning. " That is very
-
31:36 - 31:39common. If you have problems with
-
31:39 - 31:42remembering facts, and I'm going to do
-
31:42 - 31:46Algebra problems, coming to that shortly,
-
31:46 - 31:48don't turn off, all those of you going to
-
31:48 - 31:51do Algebra, [inaudible].
-
31:51 - 31:54What you really want to learn are the number
-
31:54 - 31:59combinations that add up to ten. The reason
-
31:59 - 32:03they are useless to do lots with
-
32:03 - 32:06them, I'll try to be straight in a little bit,
-
32:06 - 32:10And the doubles, the numbers like 5 + 5,
-
32:10 - 32:13and 6 + 6, and facts, ......a bit of algebra
-
32:13 - 32:17in here. This is a number, any number, this
-
32:17 - 32:20is the same number. This is a way of
-
32:20 - 32:24generalizing, this could be 5 + 5, this could be
-
32:24 - 32:277 + 7, so in a sense that's a very good
-
32:27 - 32:31example of algebra we use to generalize,
-
32:31 - 32:37to show patterns, things are the same.
-
32:37 - 32:39This is some manipulatives,
-
32:39 - 32:44again, to try and help people see what
-
32:44 - 32:47numbers, what combination of numbers,
-
32:47 - 32:50make 10. I'm going to start with 10 on this
-
32:50 - 32:53side, and none on this side, so zero--this
-
32:53 - 32:57is a combination that makes 10. If I move
-
32:57 - 33:01one over, I've still got 10, I conserved
-
33:01 - 33:04the 10, that's an important concept,
-
33:04 - 33:07this side now, I have only 9, I
-
33:07 - 33:15have 1 on this side. If I move 2 over, I
-
33:15 - 33:25now have 8 and 2; 8 on the left side, and 2
-
33:25 - 33:28on this side. I can keep moving one over,
-
33:28 - 33:337 and 3, 6 and 4, until I reach double key
-
33:33 - 33:37number. If the number combination for 10
-
33:37 - 33:45is key, this particular 5 + 5, is super key,
-
33:45 - 33:48mega-key, whatever you want to call it.
-
33:48 - 33:51It's the 5 fingers on one hand, 5 fingers
-
33:51 - 33:53on the other.
-
33:53 - 33:57It's a key number. If I move over again,
-
33:57 - 34:02moving one over, I've now got 4 and 6.
-
34:02 - 34:04Instead of 6 and 4, which we had,
-
34:04 - 34:08we've now got 4 and 6: still 10. An
-
34:08 - 34:12important rule for you here...if two
-
34:12 - 34:16numbers, 4 and 6 add up to make 10, then
-
34:16 - 34:19those numbers in the other rule, 6 and 4,
-
34:19 - 34:23will also make 10. Sometimes people want to
-
34:23 - 34:26know why 7 and 6 makes 13, but they also
-
34:26 - 34:29do know, because I've pointed it out to them,
-
34:29 - 34:33that 6 and 7 makes 13. This of course,
-
34:33 - 34:35goes on with 3 and 7, and 2 and 8, until
-
34:35 - 34:40you end up with 0 and 10. So those are the
-
34:40 - 34:44number bonds group. Why are they grouped?
-
34:44 - 34:48This might be something I would do with an
-
34:48 - 34:53older learner, not a 5,6,7 year old. I want
-
34:53 - 34:57to show you again how there are links,
-
34:57 - 35:00patterns. If you know these facts, you'll
-
35:00 - 35:04know your combinations for 100. For an older
-
35:04 - 35:07child whose doing decimals, they'll know--
-
35:07 - 35:09they'll be able to work out their number
-
35:09 - 35:14combinations for decimals. So, 1 and 9
-
35:14 - 35:17make 10, 10 and 90 make 100. Practicing
-
35:17 - 35:20place value and your two digit plus two
-
35:20 - 35:23digit makes three digit in this particular
-
35:23 - 35:26example. Here I'm practicing decimals, but
-
35:26 - 35:30look: the 1, the 2, the 3, the 4...the 1,
-
35:30 - 35:32the 2, the 3, the 4...the 1, the 2, the 3,
-
35:32 - 35:35the 4, combined with the 9,8,7,6...
-
35:35 - 35:38the 9,8,7,6...the 9,8,7,6, are there in
-
35:38 - 35:41in all these examples, just are different
-
35:41 - 35:46place values. Again, there are lovely key
-
35:46 - 35:50points and I might go here, and here
-
35:50 - 35:52and for reason, and
-
35:52 - 35:545 and 5 makes 10, and 50 and 50 makes a
-
35:54 - 35:59hundred. This isn't hard at all. So again, you
-
35:59 - 36:02as the teacher or parent look at the child,
-
36:02 - 36:06You look at that, and it's working [inaudible]
-
36:06 - 36:10and that's kind of important. Here I've
-
36:10 - 36:13used symbols, again, I've used
-
36:13 - 36:20manipulatives. [inaudible] Okay...
-
36:20 - 36:23Algebra...let's just play around a litle
-
36:23 - 36:26bit here before we get to Algebra, let's
-
36:26 - 36:30be patient...in moments...
-
36:30 - 36:32In this particular slide I'm starting off by
-
36:32 - 36:34showing you there is a relationship
-
36:34 - 36:38between addition and subtraction. Those
-
36:38 - 36:42inchworm learners, those original learners
-
36:42 - 36:45that I've talked about, don't see that
-
36:45 - 36:49connection unless it is pointed out. I do
-
36:49 - 36:52want to point it out, so here's a little
-
36:52 - 36:56problem: 100 minus 0 is what? What would
-
36:56 - 36:59I put in that little space there, what
-
36:59 - 37:05would be my answer? It's 100. Let me
-
37:05 - 37:08now make that an addition problem. What
-
37:08 - 37:10would I add on to 0, what would I put in
-
37:10 - 37:13this little box here to make 100? Again, I
-
37:13 - 37:16hope you are seeing, that's 100. So I am
-
37:16 - 37:19relating subtraction and addition, using
-
37:19 - 37:23addition facts to make subtraction facts.
-
37:23 - 37:31Let's do-what would I put in the box, for
-
37:31 - 37:34this subtraction? I would put 80, same as
-
37:34 - 37:38I would put here in this box. The question
-
37:38 - 37:41is, what happens when I subtract 20
-
37:41 - 37:45from 100? I take 20 away from 100, again
-
37:45 - 37:49the calvery comes in, I get 80. What do I
-
37:49 - 37:53add on to 20 to get 100? I add 80.
-
37:53 - 37:58Fifty, the same deal. Seventy, same deal.
-
37:58 - 38:03Relating addition to subtraction.
-
38:03 - 38:06You need to understand that
-
38:06 - 38:09is a law, because if you don't you're not
-
38:09 - 38:14going to get the Algebra. I've subtly
-
38:14 - 38:17introduced Algebra, because I've asked you,
-
38:17 - 38:20what number goes in here, to make this
-
38:20 - 38:25side equal that side. Let's put that square
-
38:25 - 38:31away for a moment, and let's replace it,
-
38:31 - 38:36with an 'x'. ~All~ I've done is taken the
-
38:36 - 38:39square away, and replaced it, with an 'x'.
-
38:39 - 38:44Now my question is, what number would I
-
38:44 - 38:49write instead of 'x' to make this equation
-
38:49 - 38:53the same on both sides. So what number
-
38:53 - 38:56do I add to 20, to make 80 ?
-
38:56 - 38:59In order to get 'x', you have to tell me,
-
38:59 - 39:02what number 'x' represents. In maths we
-
39:02 - 39:06often use the instruction, find 'x'. I've
-
39:06 - 39:08seen the classic cartoon where the child
-
39:08 - 39:10looked all around and said,
-
39:10 - 39:13says,"Here it is!". That's not what we meant.
-
39:13 - 39:18Let's do it again, with the next set. Just
-
39:18 - 39:22get rid of the box, let's put in the
-
39:22 - 39:25letter, this time use 'y'. What is the
-
39:25 - 39:29value of 'y' that makes this side 100,
-
39:29 - 39:32make it equal to that side.
-
39:32 - 39:35If you don't understand the numbers,
-
39:35 - 39:41you don't understand the Algebra. If I was
-
39:41 - 39:44teaching Algebra, I might want to go back
-
39:44 - 39:48to the early numbers, that is important.
-
39:48 - 39:52Sorry, Coleen, fractions, we're not going
-
39:52 - 39:56to have time for that tonite, but I will
-
39:56 - 40:02mention them, not very constructively.
-
40:02 - 40:08With the four operations we use: addition,
-
40:08 - 40:16subtraction, multiplication, division...
-
40:16 - 40:21they are all linked. There is a hugely
-
40:21 - 40:24confusing language around addition and
-
40:24 - 40:27subtraction. Sometimes with subtraction,
-
40:27 - 40:29we say 'take away', sometimes 'what's the
-
40:29 - 40:33difference', sometimes we say 'subtract'.
-
40:33 - 40:37Just as a quick illustration here, I want
-
40:37 - 40:39to use these number combinations of 10
-
40:39 - 40:42again. What is the difference between a
-
40:42 - 40:48thousand, and 438? I've drawn a number
-
40:48 - 40:53line, so here's my 1000, here's my 438--
-
40:53 - 40:55this is the difference. What is this bit
-
40:55 - 40:59here? That's what I want to know.
-
40:59 - 41:02I'm going to do that, by adding. I'm
-
41:02 - 41:07going to add, first of all, 2 to the 8, to
-
41:07 - 41:13make it 40, that little tiny bit there,
-
41:13 - 41:17headed towards a thousand. What do I add
-
41:17 - 41:21on to 440, to make the hundred? Sixty.
-
41:21 - 41:23These are my number bonds for 10.
-
41:23 - 41:31I've got to 500. What do I add on to 500
-
41:31 - 41:35to make 1000? Five hundred. Now I've gone
-
41:35 - 41:37from there to there, I've found this
-
41:37 - 41:40difference by addition. That's the way,
-
41:40 - 41:43since we can't do money much today, that's
-
41:43 - 41:45the way when I was a boy, we used to make
-
41:45 - 41:48change. We could make it by adding on from
-
41:48 - 41:50the purchase price to the money that the
-
41:50 - 41:53customer gave us. So, that's the answer.
-
41:53 - 41:56I've done it in using number bonds of 10,
-
41:56 - 41:59a 100, and a 1000, and I"ve done it in
-
41:59 - 42:08stages to help with that. If you want to do
-
42:08 - 42:13the take away, 1000 minus 438, is so
-
42:13 - 42:17difficult, when you do it as a take away.
-
42:17 - 42:21Can I suggest you get on google or whatever
-
42:21 - 42:23search engine you use, and search for
-
42:23 - 42:26'Tom Lehrer and New Math'. Tom Lehrer was
-
42:26 - 42:30a math professor at Harvard and a great
-
42:30 - 42:34song writer, a tremendously, often black,
-
42:34 - 42:37sense of humor, and see how he explains
-
42:37 - 42:40how you do this type of calculation. It's
-
42:40 - 42:44wonderful, much more entertaining than I
-
42:44 - 42:49can be. This "what else are you teaching?"
-
42:49 - 42:51this principal, and then I'll move on to a
-
42:51 - 42:57new topic in a moment..."9". Grasshoppers,
-
42:57 - 43:01these intuitive thinkers, love 9, because
-
43:01 - 43:04it's close to 10, they don't see 9,
-
43:04 - 43:09they see "9" as unique. Inchworms see 9
-
43:09 - 43:12as lots of fingers, they DON'T see it next
-
43:12 - 43:17to 10, they see it very, very literally.
-
43:17 - 43:21They add 9 by adding 1,2,3,...til they get
-
43:21 - 43:23to 9. And they add 99 in the same way,
-
43:23 - 43:25they see it singly.
-
43:25 - 43:28Shops, yet again, a bit about money. Shops
-
43:28 - 43:32use 99 cents or 99 pence at the end.
-
43:32 - 43:35Something is priced at 14 dollars and 99
-
43:35 - 43:37cents [$14.99] because they know
-
43:37 - 43:39psychologically a lot of people see the 14
-
43:39 - 43:42dollars, they don't see the ".99", they
-
43:42 - 43:46don't see it as close to 15 dollars. I'd
-
43:46 - 43:50want to teach that you have 10, you round
-
43:50 - 43:55up as a way of avoiding the psychological
-
43:55 - 43:59price. Inchworms, again, you might convince
-
43:59 - 44:03them that adding 10 can happen, but then
-
44:03 - 44:05they have trouble, knowing they have to
-
44:05 - 44:08subtract. Why? Because it's addition, and
-
44:08 - 44:10they don't interrelate operations and
-
44:10 - 44:15numbers. These are skills we are teaching
-
44:15 - 44:17to make people more comfortable
-
44:17 - 44:20with number in everyday life, often,
-
44:20 - 44:22with money.
-
44:22 - 44:26Estimation is what grasshoppers do
-
44:26 - 44:28naturally. They are good at that, they have
-
44:28 - 44:30great number sense. They may not be great
-
44:30 - 44:33at other things, but that's one of their
-
44:33 - 44:35great skills. Inchworms, not natural
-
44:35 - 44:40estimators. With this 9, I am teaching
-
44:40 - 44:43estimation, inter-relating numbers and
-
44:43 - 44:46operation, rounding up to the nearest 10
-
44:46 - 44:48and so on. I am also teaching people to
-
44:48 - 44:52appraise their answer. After it all, I want
-
44:52 - 44:54them to know, is your estimate bigger or
-
44:54 - 44:57smaller than the real answer. Estimation
-
44:57 - 45:01is taking away that pressure of decision,
-
45:01 - 45:06and introducing a new idea upon them.
-
45:06 - 45:08I need to get moving, I've been talking
-
45:08 - 45:12for a long time. Any questions again at
-
45:12 - 45:27this break?...[pause] I'll move on to
-
45:27 - 45:32conceptualizing. Money is quite good
-
45:32 - 45:35for introducing conceptualization, it
-
45:35 - 45:40combines two things there. In terms of
-
45:40 - 45:45place value, the money in the U.S.A, you
-
45:45 - 45:49have this coin called a quarter, which
-
45:49 - 45:52is not as good for maths, for place values,
-
45:52 - 45:55kind of good for fractions, not as good
-
45:55 - 46:01for regular mathematics. In our currency,
-
46:01 - 46:04and in lots of other countries we have a
-
46:04 - 46:091 cent, 2 cent, 5 cent, 10 cent, 20 cent,
-
46:09 - 46:1250 cent...there is a good pattern for
-
46:12 - 46:16breaking down numbers. But you can use
-
46:16 - 46:20trading with money, so this crossing from
-
46:20 - 46:25units to tens. If you get 10 single cents, you
-
46:25 - 46:30can trade them for a dime. You can trade
-
46:30 - 46:3510 dimes for a dollar. In the U.K., it
-
46:35 - 46:38would be 10 pennies for a ten pence coin,
-
46:38 - 46:43and 10 ten pence coins for a pound. You
-
46:43 - 46:46can use coins to relate when transforming
-
46:46 - 46:47and conceptualizing.
-
46:47 - 46:50Let me motor on because I am talking too
-
46:50 - 46:56much which I always do. Again, BIG research
-
46:56 - 46:58Children/Adults don't know their
-
46:58 - 47:02multiplication facts, their times table
-
47:02 - 47:05facts. This, again, when I ask teachers,
-
47:05 - 47:08BIG problem, 50 to 70% of children age 10
-
47:08 - 47:13in the U.K. don't master this task. Again,
-
47:13 - 47:16what you need to learn, what you've got to
-
47:16 - 47:19learn are 0's, 1's, 2's, 5's and 10's. So
-
47:19 - 47:25what I'll do, is to try to show you how
-
47:25 - 47:31these can be done...got to keep working.
-
47:31 - 47:34Here we go...what else are you teaching?
-
47:34 - 47:36Rote learning is not good for a lot of
-
47:36 - 47:40dyslexic students. I nearly destroyed my
-
47:40 - 47:43relationship with my first bunch of
-
47:43 - 47:46dyslexic students by trying to enforce rote
-
47:46 - 47:49learning, the times table facts upon them.
-
47:49 - 47:53This can be an advantage because if you
-
47:53 - 47:58don't rote learn them, you have to learn
-
47:58 - 48:06them in other ways, so I go back to the
-
48:06 - 48:10motto, "Teach the subject as it is, to the
-
48:10 - 48:12child as he is." If rote learning doesn't
-
48:12 - 48:15work, let's go for something deeper. Let's
-
48:15 - 48:18make a demotivating experience a positive
-
48:18 - 48:20experience. We need to know what
-
48:20 - 48:23multiplication is: let's just remind
-
48:23 - 48:25ourselves that addition is about adding
-
48:25 - 48:29together numbers, multiplication is about
-
48:29 - 48:33adding "lots of" the same number. Now,
-
48:33 - 48:36lots of sometimes means ~many~ in everyday
-
48:36 - 48:40life. Lots of in maths can be 2 lots of,
-
48:40 - 48:43or 3 lots of, it doesn't have to be mega
-
48:43 - 48:46numbers. Here is 3 "lots of " 4, added
-
48:46 - 48:52together. I like that it is 3 'x' 4,
-
48:52 - 48:55three times four [inaudible]
-
48:55 - 49:00Again, symbols are communitive property[??]
-
49:00 - 49:02I wouldn't expect a child to add
-
49:02 - 49:04all of these together, that would be very
-
49:04 - 49:06time consuming, as someone mentioned,
-
49:06 - 49:09working memory, it certainly swamped
-
49:09 - 49:11the working memory. This is a tough fact,
-
49:11 - 49:14it's one of the facts that is very hard to
-
49:14 - 49:18learn. I don't actually know why, but it
-
49:18 - 49:21is incredibly hard for children. Let me
-
49:21 - 49:25give you a quick lesson on how to help
-
49:25 - 49:28with this, and teach something else
-
49:28 - 49:31at the same time. So here is me,
-
49:31 - 49:36representing 4 and 4 and 4 visually. I
-
49:36 - 49:40could get an early estimate by popping this
-
49:40 - 49:44square in here, that square in here...that
-
49:44 - 49:46would give me two fives, and give me 12.
-
49:46 - 49:48That's not what I want to do right now.
-
49:48 - 49:51What I want to do right now is take this
-
49:51 - 49:56image of 4, and change it into a new image,
-
49:56 - 50:01all in a line. Two reasons: Here is
-
50:01 - 50:043 x 4 I'm doing visually. I could put them
-
50:04 - 50:08all together and have 3 x 4, or I can
-
50:08 - 50:12seperate them, and have 2 lots of 4, and
-
50:12 - 50:15have 1 lot of 4. Two easy facts, rather
-
50:15 - 50:22than one hard fact. Similarly, for 7 x 6, I
-
50:22 - 50:27can line up 7 separate 6's, or I can make
-
50:27 - 50:34it an area, I've got 7 lots of 6, or I could
-
50:34 - 50:39break it up into 2 parts, five 6's and
-
50:39 - 50:43two 6's, trying to introduce this idea of
-
50:43 - 50:46a multiple that you might know, and a
-
50:46 - 50:49multiple that you might know, and putting
-
50:49 - 50:53them together. That's part of the deal.
-
50:53 - 50:56In symbols, I'm showing you here how to
-
50:56 - 51:02use a partial products to work out the
-
51:02 - 51:05facts they don't know. Sometimes, you
-
51:05 - 51:07might want to make a few notes to support
-
51:07 - 51:11the working memory, but what we are
-
51:11 - 51:14teaching here is the basics of long
-
51:14 - 51:19multiplication. That's good, we aren't just
-
51:19 - 51:21wasting time, restraining [???] the child
-
51:21 - 51:24with rote learning that doesn't work.
-
51:24 - 51:26Once you know this principal, looking for
-
51:26 - 51:31easy numbers, 2 and 1 are 3, 5 and 2 are 7.
-
51:31 - 51:34Twelve breaks down into 10 and 2. I used
-
51:34 - 51:37to teach the 12x's tables, but we don't
-
51:37 - 51:40use them very often anymore. But I know my
-
51:40 - 51:4210's and I know my 2 facts, and I combine
-
51:42 - 51:46them. Fifteen---I don't teach that, but
-
51:46 - 51:51I know my 10, and I know 5's, I know
-
51:51 - 51:54that 5 is half of 10, so I can use this
-
51:54 - 51:59strategy to go by. When we go up to the
-
51:59 - 52:02cheat-sheet, yeah, I think sometimes "just
-
52:02 - 52:04give them the cheat-sheet" so we can
-
52:04 - 52:07concentrate on the concept and not lose
-
52:07 - 52:09out all the time if I'm not getting these
-
52:09 - 52:13facts. I kind of like the idea of spending
-
52:13 - 52:16some time on teaching this method because
-
52:16 - 52:18it is a conceptual lesson, and it's not
-
52:18 - 52:21just teaching how to access the fact--it's
-
52:21 - 52:24teaching the principals of multiplication,
-
52:24 - 52:28and indeed, division. What I'm saying here
-
52:28 - 52:31is adding each separate number is a many
-
52:31 - 52:36step procedure. This, the cheat-sheet
-
52:36 - 52:39method or rote learning is the one step
-
52:39 - 52:42method. I'm kind of offering an alternative
-
52:42 - 52:45in between, the two-step method. You can't
-
52:45 - 52:49do this in one, can you do it in two?
-
52:49 - 52:54It's not as much as the many, it's the two.
-
52:54 - 52:57I try to make the two easy, and these are
-
52:57 - 53:02automatic, low load on working memory.
-
53:02 - 53:05I want to get people off this total
-
53:05 - 53:08reliance on rote learning, because it is
-
53:08 - 53:11so hard. That's why an alternative to the
-
53:11 - 53:15cheat-sheets in some situations--These are
-
53:15 - 53:18the core facts, you combine them to make
-
53:18 - 53:22other facts. So I combine 'this' and 'this'
-
53:22 - 53:26one 8 and five 8 are six 8. Conception,
-
53:26 - 53:29one can understand that combination. Five
-
53:29 - 53:328's and two 8's make seven 8's. Two 8's and
-
53:32 - 53:36ten 8's to make twelve 8's. What I'm
-
53:36 - 53:39trying to do is give strategies that are
-
53:39 - 53:43consistent. Combining facts that you know,
-
53:43 - 53:48using facts that you know to extend the
-
53:48 - 53:51concepts, to extend the facts you do.
-
53:51 - 53:54A little bit of Algebra coming up here
-
53:54 - 53:57slowly. So I'm doing six lots, six lots
-
53:57 - 54:01of 6, transferring that into five lots and
-
54:01 - 54:05one lot. Six lots of 7, six lots of 8, six
-
54:05 - 54:09lots of 9. Same split every time into five
-
54:09 - 54:12lots of 6, one lot of 6;
-
54:12 - 54:19five lots of 7 and one lot of 7,
-
54:19 - 54:22five lots of 8, one of 8; five lots of 9,
-
54:22 - 54:28one of 9. This is not a quick fix, but
-
54:28 - 54:32you are wasting less learning time, because
-
54:32 - 54:35you are teaching concepts. This is
-
54:35 - 54:37conceptual, this is about teaching
-
54:37 - 54:42mathematics, what is facts.
-
54:42 - 54:46Let's put this now into symbols, and what
-
54:46 - 54:49I've now done, is left those visual images
-
54:49 - 54:53behind, and focused on the symbols.
-
54:53 - 54:586 lots of 6, 5 lots and 1; 6 lots of 7,
-
54:58 - 55:035 lots and 1; 6 lots of 8, 5 lots and 1;
-
55:03 - 55:076 lots of 9, 5 lots and 1. It's going to
-
55:07 - 55:11work for any number: 6 '21's", 5 "21's"
-
55:11 - 55:17and 1 "21's". Oh, there's a hand in there,
-
55:17 - 55:21and I don't know why...This is meant to be
-
55:21 - 55:245 lots of any number you want to write...
-
55:24 - 55:276 lots of any number you want to write,
-
55:27 - 55:305 lots of any number and 1 lot of any
-
55:30 - 55:35number. Instead of this hand, I can use
-
55:35 - 55:40an 'n'. I can use an 'n', I can use a hand.
-
55:40 - 55:436 lots of any number: 5 lots of any
-
55:43 - 55:45number plus 1 of any number.
-
55:45 - 55:48And, as you know, in Algebra, we don't use
-
55:48 - 55:56times. This question, Christine, about
-
55:56 - 55:58dyslexic student's that taught the methods
-
55:58 - 56:01to themselves. Yep, that's where I learnt
-
56:01 - 56:04it, I learnt it from my dyslexic students.
-
56:04 - 56:08Just kind of streamlined it a bit, little
-
56:08 - 56:10bit of order [??] A generation of dyslexic
-
56:10 - 56:14students taught me all this stuff.
-
56:14 - 56:18Okay, what is it about division, the tail
-
56:18 - 56:23end of my session. I'm only doing this as
-
56:23 - 56:26an illustration, again of the complexity
-
56:26 - 56:29of maths, and how we can help, to some
-
56:29 - 56:34extent, so this optimal [inaudible]
-
56:34 - 56:36can do. I'll just give you a little bit of
-
56:36 - 56:38data from the U.K., this is some data from
-
56:38 - 56:44the standardized tests. In the U.K., at ten
-
56:44 - 56:48years old, about 3/4 of the students
-
56:48 - 56:52across the U.K. completed this. This
-
56:52 - 56:56division, less than 1/4 of the ten year
-
56:56 - 56:59olds can do. At thirteen, it's still there
-
56:59 - 57:06about 3/4 to just over 40%. At 15, it's
-
57:06 - 57:10gone up to 83%, still only 44% . Division
-
57:10 - 57:19is tough, learning it is complicated.
-
57:19 - 57:22Nearly finished, for those of you that are
-
57:22 - 57:25listening. Division and fractions,
-
57:25 - 57:28again, when I ask teachers around the
-
57:28 - 57:30world what are the subjects that kids
-
57:30 - 57:34find hard, these are the top two.
-
57:34 - 57:37Fractions, of course, incorporate division.
-
57:37 - 57:39It's a culture, the culture is that these
-
57:39 - 57:42two topics are hard, so it helps people
-
57:42 - 57:45pre-empt their feelings of failure,
-
57:45 - 57:48because we know it's tough, and that's
-
57:48 - 57:52the beginning. Why is it hard? Because
-
57:52 - 57:55our language is. We say "sixteen
-
57:55 - 57:58divided by 3". We also say, "How many
-
57:58 - 58:02threes in sixteen?" That reversal of the
-
58:02 - 58:06numbers involved will confuse some children
-
58:06 - 58:09before they even begin. Again, I want this
-
58:09 - 58:14to be conceptual. I want it to be
-
58:14 - 58:18understandable. Link things together.
-
58:18 - 58:20Addition is about adding together numbers,
-
58:20 - 58:22subtraction is about taking away. They are
-
58:22 - 58:24inverse, they are opposites.
-
58:24 - 58:27Multiplication is about adding 'lots of'
-
58:27 - 58:30the ~same~ number. We can guess, that
-
58:30 - 58:34division is about subtracting/taking away
-
58:34 - 58:42lots of the same number. About 5 slides left.
-
58:42 - 58:44Sixteen divided by three-- how many lots
-
58:44 - 58:47of 3 in 16? So, I'm using multiplication
-
58:47 - 58:50language to relate the concept.
-
58:50 - 58:54So let's just take away one 3...we've got 13
-
58:54 - 58:58Another 3, we've got 10. Another 3, we've
-
58:58 - 59:02got 7, another 3, we've got 4...so what
-
59:02 - 59:06I've done is taken away 5 lots of 3, and
-
59:06 - 59:12I've got 1 left over, 1 remainder. Let me
-
59:12 - 59:16just rearrange that for you, and I'm going
-
59:16 - 59:19to do the same again, "How many 3's in 16?"
-
59:19 - 59:25Take away 3, and another 3, and another 3,
-
59:25 - 59:28the fourth 3, and the fifth 3. I've taken
-
59:28 - 59:32away five 3's, and I've got 1 left over.
-
59:32 - 59:36That is the way we present short division
-
59:36 - 59:41in the U.K. How many 3's in 16? Five, and
-
59:41 - 59:451 left over. This also relates to a
-
59:45 - 59:49rectangles, again, what else am I teaching.
-
59:49 - 59:50I am trying to
-
59:50 - 59:53teach, this is a rectangle. You see
-
59:53 - 59:56it's 3 on one side of the rectangle, and
-
59:56 - 59:59you see it's 5, linking multiplication
-
59:59 - 60:00and division. Again, I am
-
60:00 - 60:04doing this very quickly, but I'm trying to
-
60:04 - 60:07illustrate these with the visuals, and the
-
60:07 - 60:09icon. Einstein said, among
-
60:09 - 60:13many other things, "Everything should be
-
60:13 - 60:17made as simple as possible, but not
-
60:17 - 60:20simpler." I'm just delighted and very
-
60:20 - 60:24honored [inaudible] that you are here, thank
-
60:24 - 60:28you. Let's just quickly look at what children
-
60:28 - 60:32are often taught, and why this is so hard.
-
60:32 - 60:36"How many 23's in 16?" There aren't any.
-
60:36 - 60:42"How many 23's in 168? " How the heck do I
-
60:42 - 60:46know? It's so difficult, so that's the
-
60:46 - 60:50barrier; already, some kids are gone.
-
60:50 - 60:53What we do is, let's say, we work out a
-
60:53 - 60:57answer of 7. We write the 7 here. How do I
-
60:57 - 61:00know that? I don't know, so much the child
-
61:00 - 61:05has to take in faith. We take away that, we
-
61:05 - 61:07get 7, we bring down the 3, whatever that
-
61:07 - 61:13means. How many 23's in 73? What is it?
-
61:13 - 61:18Take away. We bring down the 6. SO MUCH
-
61:18 - 61:20to remember in that sequence. Make 1
-
61:20 - 61:27mistake, and your out! There's another way,
-
61:27 - 61:30where I'll use a different example,
-
61:30 - 61:33which relates to multiplication, relates to
-
61:33 - 61:39that times tables. So again, only doing the
-
61:39 - 61:43same thing again and again and again. What
-
61:43 - 61:46you might do is link the two. Instead of
-
61:46 - 61:48having to find out how many 17's in
-
61:48 - 61:51something, what we are going to do here is
-
61:51 - 61:53use repeated subtraction. Let's find
-
61:53 - 61:55something to take away. Our initial
-
61:55 - 61:58jottings might be, well, whats a hundred
-
61:58 - 62:02times 17? That would be 1700, that's too
-
62:02 - 62:05big, so we do some reappraising. I go to my
-
62:05 - 62:09next easy multiple, and halve that, 50
-
62:09 - 62:13lots, 850. At least I'm started, I've got the
-
62:13 - 62:16layer started. I've taken away the 50
-
62:16 - 62:21times 17, and got 357 left. Then we take
-
62:21 - 62:25away twenty 17's, using that multiplication
-
62:25 - 62:29again...and then what? You've still got to
-
62:29 - 62:32subtract, you've still got to organize your
-
62:32 - 62:35work in space, but I've made it more
-
62:35 - 62:39conceptually manageable. And I'm using
-
62:39 - 62:43key multiples in the subtraction, something
-
62:43 - 62:48that's I've learned from [inaudible],
-
62:48 - 62:52many years ago. Total numbers are there.
-
62:52 - 62:55What this method does, and what it doesn't
-
62:55 - 62:58do is, it's logical, it's conceptual,and
-
62:58 - 63:01all the stuff we need. Nothing starts
-
63:01 - 63:03with [inaudible], nothing starts with
-
63:03 - 63:04[inaudible]. It's the
-
63:04 - 63:06inverse of the method that I teach for
-
63:06 - 63:09multiplication. There's less reliance on
-
63:09 - 63:12memory for procedures, because I'm using
-
63:12 - 63:13the procedure, that I use for
-
63:13 - 63:15multiplication, just the inversion,
-
63:15 - 63:19that's all. It uses core key fact multiples
-
63:19 - 63:21so you don't need a cheat sheet, which you
-
63:21 - 63:25don't have, by the way, for 17. It reduces
-
63:25 - 63:28the chance for errors , and that feeling of
-
63:28 - 63:30helplessness--"Can't do it! Not even going
-
63:30 - 63:33to start." You have to be able to subtract,
-
63:33 - 63:36do your spatial organization, and you do
-
63:36 - 63:43need a knowledge of place value.
-
63:43 - 63:45The devil is in the detail. I haven't done
-
63:45 - 63:48anything that's not mathematical. What
-
63:48 - 63:50I have done is focused on the details. So
-
63:50 - 63:53often the details will cause the children
-
63:53 - 63:59to fail.[inaudible] It's the details in
-
63:59 - 64:02the language you use, the images you use,
-
64:02 - 64:07the sanctions we often use. It's about
-
64:07 - 64:10knowing the math from a learners
-
64:10 - 64:13perspective, and that's why I said, my
-
64:13 - 64:18dyslexic ones taught me this. It's about
-
64:18 - 64:21preventing failure, getting to those
-
64:21 - 64:26emotional ideas before they become a
-
64:26 - 64:32block to learning. And....it's complicated!
-
64:32 - 64:36Thank you all for all your great comments,
-
64:36 - 64:40I really have enjoyed talking to you in a
-
64:40 - 64:44virtual way, and I hope the feeling is mutual.
-
64:44 - 64:49Thanks, bye-bye. Any questions, does
-
64:49 - 64:55anyone want to keep going? [Dr. Fernette
-
64:55 - 64:57Eide] Absolutely awesome, thank you so
-
64:57 - 65:22much! ...Fantastic!...Questions?... I know
-
65:22 - 65:25you didn't get a chance to use fractions.
-
65:25 - 65:28Anything, generally? Do you use
-
65:28 - 65:35manipulatives? Do you do the pie thing?
-
65:35 - 65:39[Dr. Chinn] Pies are pretty good...pizza's
-
65:39 - 65:42are cruel because you are just going to put
-
65:42 - 65:47children on pizza's! What I do use is
-
65:47 - 65:52square paper, squares of paper, and I
-
65:52 - 65:54get kids to fold them.
-
65:54 - 65:57The reason I use squares, I can fold in two
-
65:57 - 66:01directions. What I also do,
-
66:01 - 66:04I premark the paper so the kids
-
66:04 - 66:08aren't messing around trying to fold into
-
66:08 - 66:133 pieces or 5 pieces. I make it easier for
-
66:13 - 66:17them to follow it. With the folding, I can
-
66:17 - 66:20show that if you fold it 5 times you get
-
66:20 - 66:235 parts. If you fold it 3 times, you get
-
66:23 - 66:273 parts. So the third is bigger than the
-
66:27 - 66:31fifth. You look to what are big conceptual
-
66:31 - 66:35barriers. A big conceptual barrier is that
-
66:35 - 66:381 over 3 is bigger than 1 over 5. Language
-
66:38 - 66:42again is a problem: a half, a third, a
-
66:42 - 66:46quarter, don't tell you...a fifth, a sixth,
-
66:46 - 66:49a seventh at least tell you something. It's
-
66:49 - 66:52challenging, all the stuff that kids have
-
66:52 - 66:56learned about numbers before you. When you
-
66:56 - 67:00see 25, you know that the 2 is 2 tens, the
-
67:00 - 67:045 is 5 units. When you see 2 over 5, the
-
67:04 - 67:092 is 2 units, the 5 is 5 units, but that
-
67:09 - 67:12sneaky line in the middle is telling you
-
67:12 - 67:16the 2 is divided by 5. Twenty-five, you
-
67:16 - 67:22know that the 2 is multiplied by 10.
-
67:22 - 67:25There's hidden information in fractions,
-
67:25 - 67:28it's challenging fact consistency. Now
-
67:28 - 67:33that I mention it, it's a big topic to deal
-
67:33 - 67:36with, but you've got to do it with visuals
-
67:36 - 67:38aides and manipulation. Otherwise the
-
67:38 - 67:43symbols are just gone, not getting constancy.
-
67:43 - 68:03[discussion about tech details...]
-
68:03 - 68:05[Fernette Eide] Do you have lesson plans
-
68:05 - 68:08for teachers? [Dr. Chinn] Ha, it sounds
-
68:08 - 68:10like my brother asked that question! I'm
-
68:10 - 68:13working on that right now. In fact, I've
-
68:13 - 68:15done a great deal of work on that, and I
-
68:15 - 68:21hope to find a publisher. It's a huge
-
68:21 - 68:27amount of work, hardly a mixture of
-
68:27 - 68:30power-points like some of what I've done
-
68:30 - 68:33today, which could be used as power-points
-
68:33 - 68:39or guidance, so far as manipulatives, some
-
68:39 - 68:45topic sheets, talking to teachers about
-
68:45 - 68:49progress for learning, other resources,
-
68:49 - 68:50like the squares I
-
68:50 - 68:56mentioned. There's about 100 worksheets
-
68:56 - 69:02that are criterion referenced,
-
69:02 - 69:04and all have been posted with solutions.
-
69:04 - 69:07so that teachers can use those worksheets
-
69:07 - 69:09with a guide-master for them. The
-
69:09 - 69:13worksheets are also set up to highlight
-
69:13 - 69:16some error patterns, not trying to make
-
69:16 - 69:23children go wrong, but to allow some other
-
69:23 - 69:26patterns to happen. We can then deal with
-
69:26 - 69:29those before they become embedded in
-
69:29 - 69:33the kids, or the adults mind. I find that
-
69:33 - 69:36working with adults,like some I mentioned-
-
69:36 - 69:38just go back to that early stuff and
-
69:38 - 69:42present it in a way that's acceptable to
-
69:42 - 69:46adults. These are often the adults who, as
-
69:46 - 69:50children, gave up at 7 and spent the last
-
69:50 - 69:5710 years hating maths. It's very sad.
-
69:57 - 70:01Many levels in mastery, sometimes you can
-
70:01 - 70:06bypass lower levels. A lot of my students
-
70:06 - 70:10often found Algebra easier than Arithmetic
-
70:10 - 70:12not because school had gotten any easier,
-
70:12 - 70:15but because their number facts are so poor.
-
70:15 - 70:19There are less number fact demands in
-
70:19 - 70:23Algebra--it's conceptual, and often my
-
70:23 - 70:26students are very good conceptually, they
-
70:26 - 70:29are just prone to error, maths is not
-
70:29 - 70:32forgiving in that. The question about
-
70:32 - 70:36mainstreaming, my experience has been what
-
70:36 - 70:40works for dyslexics, works for anybody.
-
70:40 - 70:44There is a group just north of Boston who
-
70:44 - 70:48[inaudible] technology that right now is
-
70:48 - 70:51talking about [inaudiable] people
-
70:51 - 70:55like dyslexics, and computics. What we
-
70:55 - 71:00are learning, work with these students,
-
71:00 - 71:05these adults, can be mainstreamed. What
-
71:05 - 71:07we are doing, I've said from the start, is
-
71:07 - 71:10we are teaching the subject as it is, to
-
71:10 - 71:12the child as it is, not messing with the
-
71:12 - 71:15mathematics, implied to keep the
-
71:15 - 71:17mathematics the same, but I'm trying to
-
71:17 - 71:19make it more conceptual rather than rote.
-
71:19 - 71:22[Fernette] We'll close now...Thanks, Steve,
-
71:22 - 71:26this was incredibly helpful and awesome, I
-
71:26 - 71:28wish I'd been taught math this way. I think
-
71:28 - 71:30a lot of people are thinking about this
-
71:30 - 71:32now. I really appreciate all the time
-
71:32 - 71:35you've given us. You really have a genius
-
71:35 - 71:37for all this kind of instruction...[We]
-
71:37 - 71:40really appreciate it, thank you so much!
-
71:40 - 71:41[clapping sounds]
-
71:41 - 71:44[Dr. Chinn] Thank you, everyone, for being
-
71:44 - 71:47so kind and for such good questions.
-
71:47 - 71:49[ Dr.Fernette Eide] You are a rock star!
-
71:49 - 71:51[laughter]
- Title:
- Teaching Math to Dyslexic Students - Dr Steve Chinn
- Description:
-
Audio may be better at this link: http://bit.ly/157Oz9U
Dyslexic Advantage webinar with teaching strategies to help students with dyslexia learn math.Dr Chinn uses animations, visuals, and simple patterns to address issues such as math facts, arithmetic, multiplication and division, and more complex topics such as algebra. If you have trouble with audio try this site: http://bit.ly/157Oz9U
This is the best short program we have ever seen for teaching math to students with dyslexia and dyscalculia. Thanks, Steve!
Join the movement at: http://dyslexicadvantage.com and like this video to spread the word. Thank you!
- Video Language:
- English
![]() |
Rose Walker edited English subtitles for Teaching Math to Dyslexic Students - Dr Steve Chinn | |
![]() |
Rose Walker edited English subtitles for Teaching Math to Dyslexic Students - Dr Steve Chinn | |
![]() |
Rose Walker edited English subtitles for Teaching Math to Dyslexic Students - Dr Steve Chinn | |
![]() |
Rose Walker edited English subtitles for Teaching Math to Dyslexic Students - Dr Steve Chinn | |
![]() |
Rose Walker edited English subtitles for Teaching Math to Dyslexic Students - Dr Steve Chinn | |
![]() |
Rose Walker edited English subtitles for Teaching Math to Dyslexic Students - Dr Steve Chinn | |
![]() |
Rose Walker edited English subtitles for Teaching Math to Dyslexic Students - Dr Steve Chinn | |
![]() |
Rose Walker edited English subtitles for Teaching Math to Dyslexic Students - Dr Steve Chinn |