< Return to Video

Teaching Math to Dyslexic Students - Dr Steve Chinn

  • 0:00 - 0:03
    What an incredible delight today to have
  • 0:03 - 0:05
    Steve Chinn with us, he is one of the
  • 0:05 - 0:07
    worlds authorities on teaching Math to
  • 0:07 - 0:10
    Dyslexic students. He has a number of books
  • 0:10 - 0:12
    that are available to Amazon and other
  • 0:12 - 0:14
    places that I think many of you will find
  • 0:14 - 0:19
    very useful. Steve has his doctorate in
  • 0:19 - 0:22
    Applied Physics, but has had many years as
  • 0:22 - 0:26
    teaching headmaster of Mathematics and
  • 0:26 - 0:28
    Science, it's a real pleasure to welcome
  • 0:28 - 0:30
    him today, and we look forward to hearing
  • 0:30 - 0:32
    everything he has to tell us about teaching
  • 0:32 - 0:33
    Math.
  • 0:33 - 0:36
    Thank you for that, let me see if I can do
  • 0:36 - 0:41
    this...A quick overview: I am not, in 45
  • 0:41 - 0:45
    minutes be able to deal with everything,
  • 0:45 - 0:47
    but I want to give you an overview, give
  • 0:47 - 0:49
    you some practical examples and
  • 0:49 - 0:52
    illustrations of what I think are the
  • 0:52 - 0:56
    principals of teaching Math to dyslexic
  • 0:56 - 0:59
    students. It's no good trying to use the
  • 0:59 - 1:03
    traditional methods, whatever they are.
  • 1:03 - 1:06
    In my experience, there are no quick fixes,
  • 1:06 - 1:11
    as there aren't any for reading dyslexia.
  • 1:11 - 1:15
    In many cases, it's about starting the math
  • 1:15 - 1:19
    in the place where the problems become
  • 1:19 - 1:22
    apparent, and tracking back quite a long
  • 1:22 - 1:25
    way before you get to a place where they
  • 1:25 - 1:29
    all disappear. Or often in children that
  • 1:29 - 1:32
    miss some of the few concepts, I'll talk
  • 1:32 - 1:34
    about that today. Of course we have to
  • 1:34 - 1:38
    remember that all students are individuals.
  • 1:38 - 1:41
    I've looked at the factors that are
  • 1:41 - 1:44
    involved, I've build them into the talk
  • 1:44 - 1:46
    rather than just list them at the start,
  • 1:46 - 1:48
    things that might create barriers to
  • 1:48 - 1:49
    learning & I'll break at various points so
  • 1:49 - 1:53
    that you can ask questions. I'll do my best
  • 1:53 - 1:55
    to answer. If I think it's a long answer, I
  • 1:55 - 1:59
    might put you off until the end of the
  • 1:59 - 2:03
    session. So, let's get going, and see if I
  • 2:03 - 2:06
    can start at the beginning with some
  • 2:06 - 2:09
    guiding principles and quotes. Let me
  • 2:09 - 2:13
    start with a lovely saying of Margaret
  • 2:13 - 2:16
    Rawson, a pioneer in the studying of
  • 2:16 - 2:19
    Dyslexia, from the U.S.A., a lady who
  • 2:19 - 2:21
    lived to be 104.
  • 2:21 - 2:24
    "Teach the subject as it is to the child as
  • 2:24 - 2:26
    she (or he) is."
  • 2:26 - 2:29
    Just such wise words, in a sense,
  • 2:29 - 2:33
    summarizes what I am trying to do, today.
  • 2:33 - 2:34
    Something I once said:
  • 2:34 - 2:38
    "It's quite complicated making things simple."
  • 2:38 - 2:42
    Quite often, people don't realize what
  • 2:42 - 2:46
    they think is simple, can be complicated to
  • 2:46 - 2:48
    make it simple, to do.
  • 2:48 - 2:50
    So once that's said,
  • 2:50 - 2:52
    "It's usually best to start at the beginning
  • 2:52 - 2:54
    and to be aware of where this stuff
  • 2:54 - 2:56
    is heading." ..."Stuff" is a technical
  • 2:56 - 2:58
    term I'll use for mathematics.
  • 2:58 - 3:00
    I want to look at the
  • 3:00 - 3:02
    development trajectory in maths, which
  • 3:02 - 3:06
    sometimes means you start looking back,
  • 3:06 - 3:08
    and sometimes means you go forward.
  • 3:08 - 3:10
    You have do both of those things.
  • 3:10 - 3:15
    A little bit of background here, relavent
  • 3:15 - 3:18
    to our populations. The NRC, National
  • 3:18 - 3:21
    Research Council for the U.S.A., a book
  • 3:21 - 3:24
    that was published in 2000, called"How
  • 3:24 - 3:28
    People Learn", a wonderful book, even more
  • 3:28 - 3:30
    wonderful in that it has just 3 key findings:
  • 3:30 - 3:34
    kind of succinct! The first of those key
  • 3:34 - 3:37
    findings is this one: Students don't come
  • 3:37 - 3:43
    into classrooms with an empty head, they
  • 3:43 - 3:46
    have preconceptions about how the world
  • 3:46 - 3:49
    works. It happens to be the case, how
  • 3:49 - 3:52
    Mathematics works. You've got to deal with
  • 3:52 - 3:54
    that right at the start, or they won't
  • 3:54 - 3:56
    pick up the new concepts that you're trying
  • 3:56 - 3:59
    to explain to them, and the information
  • 3:59 - 4:02
    which you are giving! Or, which often
  • 4:02 - 4:05
    happens, they may learn them just for the
  • 4:05 - 4:08
    purposes of the test, maybe just read them,
  • 4:08 - 4:10
    but only for 24 hours, and then go back to
  • 4:10 - 4:13
    their original learnings and
  • 4:13 - 4:15
    preconceptions, that they brought to the
  • 4:15 - 4:20
    classrooms. This, in fact was known , in
  • 4:20 - 4:23
    a really good research paper in a
  • 4:23 - 4:30
    monograph from Chicago way back in 1925:
  • 4:30 - 4:33
    When we learn something that is new to us,
  • 4:33 - 4:38
    what we learn is a ~dominant entry~ to the
  • 4:38 - 4:41
    brain. Quite often when we are teaching
  • 4:41 - 4:43
    maths, we've got to do some unteaching.
  • 4:43 - 4:46
    It's one of the very good reasons for using
  • 4:46 - 4:50
    manipulatives and visual images, to
  • 4:50 - 4:52
    try and create new images for teaching. So,
  • 4:52 - 4:56
    that's the "so what?" of this. We're not
  • 4:56 - 4:59
    just starting from scratch, we often have
  • 4:59 - 5:02
    to work with things that are, maybe, not
  • 5:02 - 5:07
    right. Then maths has an emotive, an
  • 5:07 - 5:10
    emotional side. There aren't many subjects
  • 5:10 - 5:14
    where people write books about anxiety. You
  • 5:14 - 5:16
    get books on maths anxiety. I guess,
  • 5:16 - 5:19
    less books on geography anxiety, art
  • 5:19 - 5:25
    anxiety. A lot of the times this is down to
  • 5:25 - 5:28
    children giving an illusion of learning.
  • 5:28 - 5:31
    Children with good memories often start off
  • 5:31 - 5:34
    doing quite well in maths. But then as that
  • 5:34 - 5:37
    load on memory gets greater and greater,
  • 5:37 - 5:41
    they tend not to fair as well, because the
  • 5:41 - 5:44
    memory often stops them from understanding,
  • 5:44 - 5:48
    as the understanding then helps the memory.
  • 5:48 - 5:50
    This is one of the strengths for
  • 5:50 - 5:54
    dyslexics. When I lecture to teachers, as
  • 5:54 - 5:57
    Fernette said, I do this all around the
  • 5:57 - 6:02
    world, I often ask a vague question, that
  • 6:02 - 6:06
    is, "At what age do enough children give up
  • 6:06 - 6:11
    on maths in your classes for you to notice?
  • 6:11 - 6:14
    The most frequent, popular answer is 7
  • 6:14 - 6:19
    years old. That is a big worry, and I'm not
  • 6:19 - 6:23
    just talking about Dyslexics in this area,
  • 6:23 - 6:28
    an International problem for many children.
  • 6:28 - 6:31
    I think we have to look in maths at the
  • 6:31 - 6:34
    role of failure and getting things wrong
  • 6:34 - 6:40
    because maths is incredibly judgemental. My
  • 6:40 - 6:42
    experience in working with children, and
  • 6:42 - 6:46
    students whatever age, is that failure
  • 6:46 - 6:51
    rarely motivates. People maneuver to avoid
  • 6:51 - 6:54
    failure in a number of ways. One of the
  • 6:54 - 6:56
    good ways of avoiding failure is giving up.
  • 6:56 - 6:59
    not trying.
  • 6:59 - 7:02
    But, people will make mistakes, and we have
  • 7:02 - 7:05
    to look at that, and we have to deal with
  • 7:05 - 7:08
    that, within the lessons.
  • 7:08 - 7:10
    So what we are trying to do is to build
  • 7:10 - 7:14
    security. One of the things, in general,
  • 7:14 - 7:17
    when we learn when we are right--it helps
  • 7:17 - 7:22
    us feel secure, is the role of consistency.
  • 7:22 - 7:25
    When things are consistent, means we can
  • 7:25 - 7:28
    often see patterns--it helps us to be secure
  • 7:28 - 7:30
    and when we ar secure, we tend to learn
  • 7:30 - 7:32
    better. We often think
  • 7:32 - 7:35
    that maths is a pretty consistent subject,
  • 7:35 - 7:38
    but like spelling of the English language,
  • 7:38 - 7:40
    which has some rules, but huge numbers of
  • 7:40 - 7:48
    exceptions. Let's look at that right now:
  • 7:48 - 7:50
    That's my first break, so if you have any
  • 7:50 - 7:53
    questions...If you can't hear me, or
  • 7:53 - 7:57
    whatever, lets have a little time now
  • 7:57 - 8:00
    before I launch into the next little bit
  • 8:00 - 8:04
    of the talk, now. I'm going to get going.
  • 8:04 - 8:09
    So, how do we write numbers? Because,
  • 8:09 - 8:11
    ultimately, children have got to learn to
  • 8:11 - 8:16
    write numbers. My grandson, when he was
  • 8:16 - 8:21
    age 2 and 3, was able to say numbers. There
  • 8:21 - 8:24
    is a big difference between saying numbers
  • 8:24 - 8:28
    and writing them. The symbols we use to
  • 8:28 - 8:30
    represent numbers are going to be very
  • 8:30 - 8:33
    important. Particularly for children who
  • 8:33 - 8:36
    have difficulty with the information, as
  • 8:36 - 8:39
    presented...something I guess that dyslexics
  • 8:39 - 8:43
    have in common. I want just to introduce
  • 8:43 - 8:46
    you to the numbers and the symbols to 9.
  • 8:46 - 8:49
    These are symbols for single digit numbers,
  • 8:49 - 8:53
    just one digit to represent 9. I'm going
  • 8:53 - 8:56
    to start in this section via illustrating
  • 8:56 - 8:59
    one of the things that's very important for
  • 8:59 - 9:03
    me, in the philosophy/principles behind
  • 9:03 - 9:06
    the way I teach--that's the question I ask
  • 9:06 - 9:10
    myself: What else am I teaching? What else
  • 9:10 - 9:12
    are my student's learning? Lots of times
  • 9:12 - 9:16
    the actual maths might be stuff these
  • 9:16 - 9:19
    learners have done many years ago. What
  • 9:19 - 9:21
    I am looking for is the way I can teach
  • 9:21 - 9:24
    that in a way that makes it acceptable and
  • 9:24 - 9:26
    understandable to older learners, who might
  • 9:26 - 9:29
    have failed at learning this stuff many
  • 9:29 - 9:32
    times before. So what else am I teaching
  • 9:32 - 9:35
    what else are people learning,
  • 9:35 - 9:37
    when I'm dealing with any topic
  • 9:37 - 9:43
    in maths? I also want to try to make people
  • 9:43 - 9:47
    be more comfortable with numbers. One of
  • 9:47 - 9:50
    the things I'm going to show you in this
  • 9:50 - 9:52
    next section 'what else are you teaching',
  • 9:52 - 9:54
    is showing people how to 'see numbers' in
  • 9:54 - 9:57
    numbers. I'm going to explain that in a
  • 9:57 - 10:01
    moment. So here is a question, you don't
  • 10:01 - 10:05
    have to answer, it's rhetorical...but it is
  • 10:05 - 10:08
    a very key question, in terms of the number
  • 10:08 - 10:11
    system, we use. And that is, what is
  • 10:11 - 10:18
    nothing? What is zero?...Here we go!
  • 10:18 - 10:24
    There's a slide--nothing more. What's very
  • 10:24 - 10:28
    important in a number system is that we
  • 10:28 - 10:31
    actually have a symbol for nothing. There
  • 10:31 - 10:37
    it is..Zero. That's a key symbol, and I'll
  • 10:37 - 10:40
    explain why it is key, and why it is the
  • 10:40 - 10:44
    root of many misconceptions in Mathematics.
  • 10:44 - 10:48
    If I want to generate problems in arithmetic
  • 10:48 - 10:53
    calculations, I put zeroes in the numbers.
  • 10:53 - 10:55
    It really makes things more challenging. So
  • 10:55 - 10:57
    let's look at the next symbols and do a
  • 10:57 - 11:00
    little bit about what else we are teaching.
  • 11:00 - 11:04
    That's the symbol '1' for one thing, the symbol
  • 11:04 - 11:08
    '2' for two things, the symbol '3' for three
  • 11:08 - 11:13
    things. I'm using the red color for the
  • 11:13 - 11:16
    odd numbers, and blue for the even numbers.
  • 11:16 - 11:19
    I'm not going to deal with that right now,
  • 11:19 - 11:21
    but it would be a 'what else are you
  • 11:21 - 11:25
    teaching' topic. So, I've got to four. It
  • 11:25 - 11:29
    would depend on the learners--I might say,
  • 11:29 - 11:33
    "Now look at this four. It's two plus two.
  • 11:33 - 11:36
    It's two times two. That's two operations
  • 11:36 - 11:38
    I've talked about; addition and
  • 11:38 - 11:41
    multiplication. It's a square. If it's an
  • 11:41 - 11:45
    older learner, I might say that we represent
  • 11:45 - 11:47
    four as two, with a small two up here,
  • 11:47 - 11:51
    meaning 2 squared. Wouldn't do that with a
  • 11:51 - 11:55
    six year old. This is how you can take
  • 11:55 - 11:58
    basic information, support all principles,
  • 11:58 - 12:02
    early learning principles and make them
  • 12:02 - 12:07
    more age appropriate for children. Let's
  • 12:07 - 12:12
    get 5 up there. You'll notice, I've
  • 12:12 - 12:14
    used a pattern, and it's one that's probably
  • 12:14 - 12:18
    familiar because it's a pattern on dice,
  • 12:18 - 12:22
    playing cards, dominoes. I might also just
  • 12:22 - 12:25
    use this as a random pattern to just check
  • 12:25 - 12:29
    out other issues with recognizing quantity.
  • 12:29 - 12:33
    Right now my agenda is to show a couple of
  • 12:33 - 12:38
    specific points. The pattern I'm using,
  • 12:38 - 12:41
    here is the difference. This is not the
  • 12:41 - 12:44
    pattern you see on cards, dominoes, and
  • 12:44 - 12:48
    dice. This "What else you are teaching here"
  • 12:48 - 12:52
    is I'm showing that 6, the number within
  • 12:52 - 12:58
    numbers, is made up of 5 and 1. I was
  • 12:58 - 13:02
    also showing that 3 is made up of 2 and 1.
  • 13:02 - 13:07
    A lot of the time learners who don't have
  • 13:07 - 13:09
    good number sense, I'm going to build that
  • 13:09 - 13:13
    number sense by using key numbers.
  • 13:13 - 13:17
    One, two, and five. Then, I'm going to look
  • 13:17 - 13:20
    for the key numbers in other numbers. So
  • 13:20 - 13:25
    that was my 6: it's 5 and 1. My 7 is 5
  • 13:25 - 13:31
    and 2. Eight, is 5 and 3. If I move this
  • 13:31 - 13:36
    counter (I would use real counters...get
  • 13:36 - 13:39
    some tactile experience)...If I move this
  • 13:39 - 13:43
    one to ~here~, I would have 2 and 2 and 2
  • 13:43 - 13:47
    and 2. So that 2 plus 2 plus 2 plus 2, we
  • 13:47 - 13:51
    have 8. (Eight up there...) I would have 4
  • 13:51 - 13:55
    plus 4. I would have 2 cubed, if I was
  • 13:55 - 13:58
    working with older learners. Four times two
  • 13:58 - 14:03
    2 x 2 x 2 x 2. So again, it is using numbers
  • 14:03 - 14:06
    where hopefully the numbers are not a
  • 14:06 - 14:08
    barrier to illustrate some of the
  • 14:08 - 14:10
    mathematical operations, some of the
  • 14:10 - 14:14
    principals. Eight...now we want to get 9.
  • 14:14 - 14:18
    There's 9. I'm still in single digits. I
  • 14:18 - 14:21
    haven't got to 10 yet. What I've done, I've
  • 14:21 - 14:26
    introduced the learner to all 10 symbols we
  • 14:26 - 14:32
    use for making any number. I've done that by
  • 14:32 - 14:35
    by counting forwards and adding one on
  • 14:35 - 14:38
    every time. I'll move through this next bit
  • 14:38 - 14:43
    a little quicker. Quite often we focus on
  • 14:43 - 14:46
    counting forward, and therefore develop
  • 14:46 - 14:48
    additions skills, which tends to be the
  • 14:48 - 14:51
    default operation. For learners who are
  • 14:51 - 14:54
    struggling with maths.But shouldn't we also
  • 14:54 - 14:57
    introduce counting backwards because when
  • 14:57 - 15:00
    we count backwards, we learn (let's keep
  • 15:00 - 15:03
    that gentleman for a moment)...we learn
  • 15:03 - 15:07
    about taking away. This flexibility and
  • 15:07 - 15:12
    seeing numbers in numbers, in interrelated
  • 15:12 - 15:16
    addition and subtraction...that's a skill that
  • 15:16 - 15:19
    some people have almost intuitively. They
  • 15:19 - 15:22
    have an intuitive number sense. In some
  • 15:22 - 15:25
    research I did when I worked in the states
  • 15:25 - 15:28
    back in the 80's, we, my colleagues
  • 15:28 - 15:30
    Dwight Knox and John Bath,
  • 15:30 - 15:34
    called those people grasshoppers. People
  • 15:34 - 15:37
    who take numbers very literally-- they see
  • 15:37 - 15:40
    9 as 9, they see 4 as 4--they don't see it
  • 15:40 - 15:46
    as 1 less than 5...we call those inchworms.
  • 15:46 - 15:48
    I just popped this little slide in here,
  • 15:48 - 15:51
    this grasshopper is scratching his head,
  • 15:51 - 15:55
    just to give you a little image, we are trying to get
  • 15:55 - 15:58
    some intuitive thinking, some good number
  • 15:58 - 16:02
    sense, some grasshopper skills. So to come
  • 16:02 - 16:06
    back, we take away one every time. We are
  • 16:06 - 16:11
    introducing subtraction. The counting goal
  • 16:11 - 16:14
    was introducing addition, adding one
  • 16:14 - 16:16
    every time, we want to get better at
  • 16:16 - 16:19
    adding one. Subtraction we are introducing
  • 16:19 - 16:23
    as taking a whole number away every time,
  • 16:23 - 16:26
    so in this particular example the quantity
  • 16:26 - 16:29
    is getting smaller, and we are returning to
  • 16:29 - 16:34
    zero. Now with some examples of what else
  • 16:34 - 16:37
    we are teaching. What I have done, is count
  • 16:37 - 16:41
    to 9 and come back again.
  • 16:41 - 16:46
    In unsophisticated use of quantity, you can
  • 16:46 - 16:50
    use tallies. If you see learners using lots
  • 16:50 - 16:53
    and lots of tallies, that's worrying,
  • 16:53 - 16:56
    because it means they are not understanding
  • 16:56 - 17:00
    how to group numbers, they are only seeing
  • 17:00 - 17:04
    numbers in ones, and that is very limiting.
  • 17:04 - 17:07
    Their mathematics is not going to progress.
  • 17:07 - 17:09
    So you see something like this, just
  • 17:09 - 17:12
    a string of tallies, very rarely are they
  • 17:12 - 17:17
    grouped...just strings of tallies. The less
  • 17:17 - 17:21
    you'll remember, you won't remember how
  • 17:21 - 17:24
    many are there just by looking. Back to
  • 17:24 - 17:26
    Forty three just happened to be
  • 17:26 - 17:29
    how many I could squeeze in on this
  • 17:29 - 17:34
    particular side. Now I'm going to show you
  • 17:34 - 17:39
    32 tallies. What I've done is grouped in 5
  • 17:39 - 17:43
    and 5 and 5 and 5 and 5 and 5, I'm going to
  • 17:43 - 17:47
    group them again, in 10's. ~Now~ I can see
  • 17:47 - 17:55
    10-20-30 and 2. So number systems will
  • 17:55 - 17:59
    eventually group ones into quantities. More
  • 17:59 - 18:03
    often than not those quantities are tens,
  • 18:03 - 18:07
    then we have a two units left over,
  • 18:07 - 18:09
    and that's 32.
  • 18:09 - 18:17
    Why do we use 10 as this grouping quantity?
  • 18:17 - 18:20
    Remember, the answer is here: we have 10
  • 18:20 - 18:25
    fingers on two hands. Five and five. So
  • 18:25 - 18:30
    hands go '1', for one digit, '2', for two
  • 18:30 - 18:34
    hands, '5' for the five fingers, and the 10
  • 18:34 - 18:38
    -the core key facts on which we conclude
  • 18:38 - 18:42
    other numbers. To be a little mischievous,
  • 18:42 - 18:45
    let me just ask you a question, that I'm
  • 18:45 - 18:47
    not going to answer. Some of you do
  • 18:47 - 18:52
    research, and when you do, where did 12
  • 18:52 - 18:56
    come from? It's rather used for time!
  • 18:56 - 19:00
    Some of you do research! So this
  • 19:00 - 19:03
    idea of clustering numbers into groups to
  • 19:03 - 19:06
    make counting and manipulation of numbers
  • 19:06 - 19:10
    easier-- well, 3000 years ago the Egyptians
  • 19:10 - 19:13
    used this symbol [image] to represent
  • 19:13 - 19:17
    10 things. They used this symbol [image]
  • 19:17 - 19:20
    to represent 100 things. A couple of
  • 19:20 - 19:24
    thousand years ago, the Romans used an 'x'
  • 19:24 - 19:28
    to represent 10, and a 'C' to represent 100,
  • 19:28 - 19:31
    and we still see 'C' in century and cent.
  • 19:31 - 19:35
    We still see that today.
  • 19:35 - 19:38
    In the number system we use, the Hindu-
  • 19:38 - 19:41
    Arabic system, you're familiar with seeing
  • 19:41 - 19:44
    one-zero (10), and one-zero-zero (100).
  • 19:44 - 19:47
    Where these symbols are quite distinct, we
  • 19:47 - 19:51
    are only using 2 of those symbols are
  • 19:51 - 19:53
    introduced while we are on this session,
  • 19:53 - 19:56
    one followed by zero, and a one followed by
  • 19:56 - 19:58
    two zeroes. What seems to be common, is
  • 19:58 - 20:03
    that the tally is I, and the one unit is
  • 20:03 - 20:06
    [inaudible]
  • 20:06 - 20:10
    So, I've introduced the idea of using one
  • 20:10 - 20:14
    tally. We have also introduced the idea of
  • 20:14 - 20:21
    9 single digit numbers, symbols of 1,2,3,4,
  • 20:21 - 20:26
    5,6,7,8, 9. The key moment when we are
  • 20:26 - 20:30
    teaching mathematics, is this change from
  • 20:30 - 20:35
    single digit, 9- to double digits, 10. Here is
  • 20:35 - 20:39
    the 10, two 5's....here the digits go from
  • 20:39 - 20:45
    9 to 10. Beneath the key moment is when we
  • 20:45 - 20:47
    erase that process, and we go back
  • 20:47 - 20:52
    from 10 to 9. That's going to help children
  • 20:52 - 20:55
    do more complicated addition problems,
  • 20:55 - 20:59
    more complicated subtraction problems, and
  • 20:59 - 21:02
    thus, we lead to multiplication. That's a
  • 21:02 - 21:05
    key moment, and it's a very conceptual
  • 21:05 - 21:07
    moment, because we are using one symbol,
  • 21:07 - 21:10
    then we are using two. That's kind of
  • 21:10 - 21:14
    interesting. What makes the Hindu-Arabic
  • 21:14 - 21:19
    system clever is that the digits, the
  • 21:19 - 21:26
    symbols hold places. Here is 9 units, we
  • 21:26 - 21:35
    write 9. When we get to 10, we can't write
  • 21:35 - 21:39
    one, because we've used one in the units.
  • 21:39 - 21:41
    We've got to come up with something that
  • 21:41 - 21:48
    distinguishes one unit from one ten. We use
  • 21:48 - 21:54
    1-0, we use 2 symbols. This means we have
  • 21:54 - 21:58
    one 10, it means we have zero units. The
  • 21:58 - 22:02
    implication there is that we have to get there
  • 22:02 - 22:04
    using the right place. If we put a zero
  • 22:04 - 22:08
    here, and a one here, that would mean
  • 22:08 - 22:11
    zero 10's and one unit. It's the order, the
  • 22:11 - 22:15
    sequencing. Children who have problems
  • 22:15 - 22:20
    with sequencing, this can be an issue.
  • 22:20 - 22:25
    Maths isn't always friendly if the learners
  • 22:25 - 22:30
    have issues like sequencing issues. Now
  • 22:30 - 22:34
    you can guess that eleven is going to be
  • 22:34 - 22:39
    one 10, one unit. The symbol allows us to
  • 22:39 - 22:49
    bring the numbers. Unfortunately, learning
  • 22:49 - 22:53
    numbers in English is not a good thing.
  • 22:53 - 23:00
    There are some real vocabulary problems.
  • 23:00 - 23:03
    If I was learning these early numbers in
  • 23:03 - 23:07
    Cantonese, I wouldn't be saying eleven, I'd
  • 23:07 - 23:11
    be saying one ten, one unit. Twelve--
  • 23:11 - 23:14
    one ten, two units. We don't, we say
  • 23:14 - 23:18
    eleven and twelve. The language, vocabulary
  • 23:18 - 23:22
    does not support symbols, does not
  • 23:22 - 23:25
    support the concept. Children learn these
  • 23:25 - 23:28
    things because they practice them, their
  • 23:28 - 23:31
    parents practice them with children, and
  • 23:31 - 23:35
    they give this illusion of one, but they
  • 23:35 - 23:40
    are not learning this key issue of place
  • 23:40 - 23:46
    value. This one, is different than the place
  • 23:46 - 23:49
    value of this one. In terms of what it
  • 23:49 - 23:53
    conveys, mathematically...that's bad
  • 23:53 - 23:58
    enough, but then we get to the teen numbers
  • 23:58 - 24:03
    and the vocabulary is telling us 3, not
  • 24:03 - 24:06
    very well, and 10, not in the language,
  • 24:06 - 24:08
    but it is suggesting three
  • 24:08 - 24:14
    and ten, but children have to write 1 and 3.
  • 24:14 - 24:16
    Four and ten, children have to have to write
  • 24:16 - 24:20
    1 and 4. When children come to practice
  • 24:20 - 24:24
    two digit numbers for the first time, the
  • 24:24 - 24:27
    vocabulary does not support the symbols
  • 24:27 - 24:31
    that they want. A lot of children will have
  • 24:31 - 24:35
    difficulty grasping the concept because
  • 24:35 - 24:38
    language, our medium of communication,
  • 24:38 - 24:42
    is not supporting the concept. This needs to
  • 24:42 - 24:45
    be modeled with materials and visual
  • 24:45 - 24:53
    images, which are not.[inaudible]
  • 24:53 - 24:58
    Let me get to twenty--well, twenty is not
  • 24:58 - 25:03
    too bad it, it suggests 2 in the tens, but
  • 25:03 - 25:06
    then we do say, twenty-one, twenty-two,
  • 25:06 - 25:08
    twenty-three. At least the vocabulary is
  • 25:08 - 25:12
    supporting the concept. I sometimes think
  • 25:12 - 25:16
    maybe we should never teach children these
  • 25:16 - 25:19
    until we teach them 21, 22, 23.
  • 25:19 - 25:23
    That's a little different.
  • 25:23 - 25:26
    So, if you are scared of big numbers, look
  • 25:26 - 25:28
    away now. I want to show you how this
  • 25:28 - 25:31
    system .........[inaudible] just clever,
  • 25:31 - 25:35
    and how, for older learners, the same
  • 25:35 - 25:38
    pattern. It goes on and on...so if you
  • 25:38 - 25:40
    didn't get it in the first learning, you
  • 25:40 - 25:42
    are not going to get the
  • 25:42 - 25:44
    extension of the maths.
  • 25:44 - 25:49
    So, the unit is the base. Ten is 10 units,
  • 25:49 - 25:53
    the next place value. One hundred is 100
  • 25:53 - 25:56
    units. Now, we've moved up to third place
  • 25:56 - 25:59
    value...we have a 3 digit number, a 2
  • 25:59 - 26:02
    digit number, and a 1. As we go up in value,
  • 26:02 - 26:05
    increasing the number of digits. Again,
  • 26:05 - 26:07
    I've modeled this in base 10 which is more
  • 26:07 - 26:11
    likely, these blocks very good material.
  • 26:11 - 26:14
    Then we get to 1000, and 1000 is kind of
  • 26:14 - 26:17
    like a watershed. Up to now, we've done 10
  • 26:17 - 26:22
    units, 100 units, 1000 units. Now, for the
  • 26:22 - 26:24
    ~bigger~ numbers, I'm going to work in
  • 26:24 - 26:28
    thousands. So I am going to do 10 thousand,
  • 26:28 - 26:32
    a 100 thousand, and a thousand
  • 26:32 - 26:35
    thousand. So I am using this ten-hundred-
  • 26:35 - 26:42
    thousand sequence--I've got to 1000 again,
  • 26:42 - 26:45
    we have to have a new word, that's a
  • 26:45 - 26:49
    million. Now I will carry on using
  • 26:49 - 26:53
    10 million, 100 million, 1000 million,
  • 26:53 - 26:58
    which, as you know, is a billion. You can
  • 26:58 - 27:01
    work out what a trillion will be. There is
  • 27:01 - 27:07
    a logic in this form. Just to show you how,
  • 27:07 - 27:11
    working with all the learners, I want to
  • 27:11 - 27:14
    use very basic maths, illustrating complex
  • 27:14 - 27:19
    symbols and complex representations. What
  • 27:19 - 27:23
    maths tries to do is represent complicated
  • 27:23 - 27:27
    ideas presciently, and sometimes called
  • 27:27 - 27:31
    very elegant. A hundred is 10 times 10,
  • 27:31 - 27:35
    ten lots of ten. So it is 2 tens multiplied
  • 27:35 - 27:39
    together. Mathmeticians symbolize this with
  • 27:39 - 27:43
    this 10, with this little 2 tucked up here in the
  • 27:43 - 27:47
    top right hand corner, 10 squared. This is
  • 27:47 - 27:51
    very difficult for children with dyspraxia,
  • 27:51 - 27:55
    with untidy writing, sequencing issues,
  • 27:55 - 27:59
    it's making all those demands. This too,
  • 27:59 - 28:02
    has to be small, otherwise it will be 102.
  • 28:02 - 28:06
    It's critical. These are points of danger
  • 28:06 - 28:09
    for learners, and points that teachers have
  • 28:09 - 28:11
    to pre-empt by demonstrating them and
  • 28:11 - 28:15
    making them very clear. A thousand is
  • 28:15 - 28:20
    10 x 10 x10, three times only, three tens
  • 28:20 - 28:24
    all together, so we write it with a 3 up here.
  • 28:24 - 28:27
    There's a logic, there's a pattern. Don't
  • 28:27 - 28:30
    just teach by rote. Ten Thousand is 10 to
  • 28:30 - 28:34
    the 4th, skip a hundred thousand, go on to
  • 28:34 - 28:35
    a million, which is
  • 28:35 - 28:40
    10 x 10 x 10 x 10x 10 x 10. Instead of
  • 28:40 - 28:44
    writing all that, mathematicians write 10
  • 28:44 - 28:46
    to the 6th, and the 6 tells you, there are
  • 28:46 - 28:51
    six 10's multiplied together. Those of you
  • 28:51 - 28:54
    that are looking for patterns, what do you
  • 28:54 - 28:57
    think a mathematician might write,
  • 28:57 - 29:02
    that doesn't [inaudible] might write for 10?
  • 29:02 - 29:08
    Think of the sequence. Four, three, two,
  • 29:08 - 29:11
    the answer is, they write 10 to the
  • 29:11 - 29:15
    power of 1. If you really want to be
  • 29:15 - 29:19
    confused, but there is a logic, for a 1, we
  • 29:19 - 29:23
    write 10 to the power of 0. That's the
  • 29:23 - 29:27
    older learners, the ones doing the [inaudible].
  • 29:27 - 29:31
    Here's another natural break time, I'm
  • 29:31 - 29:34
    ready for some questions if you have them,
  • 29:34 - 29:47
    but equally, I'm ready to go on if you don't.
  • 29:47 - 29:51
    Okay, I'm going on...
  • 29:51 - 29:53
    I want to just look for a moment at number
  • 29:53 - 29:56
    facts, what are sometimes called
  • 29:56 - 29:59
    [inaudible] facts. A great idea that's
  • 29:59 - 30:01
    that's come from some key researchers in
  • 30:01 - 30:05
    the U.S.A., is not to call these facts, but
  • 30:05 - 30:08
    to call them number combinations. When I
  • 30:08 - 30:11
    started working with Dyslexics, thirty or
  • 30:11 - 30:14
    more years ago, I discovered them because
  • 30:14 - 30:18
    my learners taught me. What they didn't
  • 30:18 - 30:21
    remember, they often worked out.
  • 30:21 - 30:28
    If they knew 5 + 5 was 10; 5 + 6 was 11.
  • 30:28 - 30:32
    That's what this number combination means.
  • 30:32 - 30:36
    Children and adults use key parts, to work
  • 30:36 - 30:42
    out all the facts. Back to my 1,2,5 and 10.
  • 30:42 - 30:47
    This is a great skill because for some
  • 30:47 - 30:50
    facts, there's no reason, unless you
  • 30:50 - 30:52
    understand numbers very well why the
  • 30:52 - 30:56
    answer repeats...what it is.
  • 30:56 - 31:00
    Let me explain a little bit about numbers.
  • 31:00 - 31:04
    The research on maths learning, Mathematics
  • 31:04 - 31:07
    learning, what we call the numbers calculating,
  • 31:07 - 31:10
    this is stuff the kid's lack confidence in.
  • 31:10 - 31:15
    It seems to me, from the research, you can
  • 31:15 - 31:18
    do endless practice on this, in some ways,
  • 31:18 - 31:23
    and the entry to the brain, is pretty short
  • 31:23 - 31:26
    lived. The thing I hear time and time again,
  • 31:26 - 31:30
    from parents is:
  • 31:30 - 31:32
    "He learned it last night, but he's
  • 31:32 - 31:36
    forgotten it this morning. " That is very
  • 31:36 - 31:39
    common. If you have problems with
  • 31:39 - 31:42
    remembering facts, and I'm going to do
  • 31:42 - 31:46
    Algebra problems, coming to that shortly,
  • 31:46 - 31:48
    don't turn off, all those of you going to
  • 31:48 - 31:51
    do Algebra, [inaudible].
  • 31:51 - 31:54
    What you really want to learn are the number
  • 31:54 - 31:59
    combinations that add up to ten. The reason
  • 31:59 - 32:03
    they are useless to do lots with
  • 32:03 - 32:06
    them, I'll try to be straight in a little bit,
  • 32:06 - 32:10
    And the doubles, the numbers like 5 + 5,
  • 32:10 - 32:13
    and 6 + 6, and facts, ......a bit of algebra
  • 32:13 - 32:17
    in here. This is a number, any number, this
  • 32:17 - 32:20
    is the same number. This is a way of
  • 32:20 - 32:24
    generalizing, this could be 5 + 5, this could be
  • 32:24 - 32:27
    7 + 7, so in a sense that's a very good
  • 32:27 - 32:31
    example of algebra we use to generalize,
  • 32:31 - 32:37
    to show patterns, things are the same.
  • 32:37 - 32:39
    This is some manipulatives,
  • 32:39 - 32:44
    again, to try and help people see what
  • 32:44 - 32:47
    numbers, what combination of numbers,
  • 32:47 - 32:50
    make 10. I'm going to start with 10 on this
  • 32:50 - 32:53
    side, and none on this side, so zero--this
  • 32:53 - 32:57
    is a combination that makes 10. If I move
  • 32:57 - 33:01
    one over, I've still got 10, I conserved
  • 33:01 - 33:04
    the 10, that's an important concept,
  • 33:04 - 33:07
    this side now, I have only 9, I
  • 33:07 - 33:15
    have 1 on this side. If I move 2 over, I
  • 33:15 - 33:25
    now have 8 and 2; 8 on the left side, and 2
  • 33:25 - 33:28
    on this side. I can keep moving one over,
  • 33:28 - 33:33
    7 and 3, 6 and 4, until I reach double key
  • 33:33 - 33:37
    number. If the number combination for 10
  • 33:37 - 33:45
    is key, this particular 5 + 5, is super key,
  • 33:45 - 33:48
    mega-key, whatever you want to call it.
  • 33:48 - 33:51
    It's the 5 fingers on one hand, 5 fingers
  • 33:51 - 33:53
    on the other.
  • 33:53 - 33:57
    It's a key number. If I move over again,
  • 33:57 - 34:02
    moving one over, I've now got 4 and 6.
  • 34:02 - 34:04
    Instead of 6 and 4, which we had,
  • 34:04 - 34:08
    we've now got 4 and 6: still 10. An
  • 34:08 - 34:12
    important rule for you here...if two
  • 34:12 - 34:16
    numbers, 4 and 6 add up to make 10, then
  • 34:16 - 34:19
    those numbers in the other rule, 6 and 4,
  • 34:19 - 34:23
    will also make 10. Sometimes people want to
  • 34:23 - 34:26
    know why 7 and 6 makes 13, but they also
  • 34:26 - 34:29
    do know, because I've pointed it out to them,
  • 34:29 - 34:33
    that 6 and 7 makes 13. This of course,
  • 34:33 - 34:35
    goes on with 3 and 7, and 2 and 8, until
  • 34:35 - 34:40
    you end up with 0 and 10. So those are the
  • 34:40 - 34:44
    number bonds group. Why are they grouped?
  • 34:44 - 34:48
    This might be something I would do with an
  • 34:48 - 34:53
    older learner, not a 5,6,7 year old. I want
  • 34:53 - 34:57
    to show you again how there are links,
  • 34:57 - 35:00
    patterns. If you know these facts, you'll
  • 35:00 - 35:04
    know your combinations for 100. For an older
  • 35:04 - 35:07
    child whose doing decimals, they'll know--
  • 35:07 - 35:09
    they'll be able to work out their number
  • 35:09 - 35:14
    combinations for decimals. So, 1 and 9
  • 35:14 - 35:17
    make 10, 10 and 90 make 100. Practicing
  • 35:17 - 35:20
    place value and your two digit plus two
  • 35:20 - 35:23
    digit makes three digit in this particular
  • 35:23 - 35:26
    example. Here I'm practicing decimals, but
  • 35:26 - 35:30
    look: the 1, the 2, the 3, the 4...the 1,
  • 35:30 - 35:32
    the 2, the 3, the 4...the 1, the 2, the 3,
  • 35:32 - 35:35
    the 4, combined with the 9,8,7,6...
  • 35:35 - 35:38
    the 9,8,7,6...the 9,8,7,6, are there in
  • 35:38 - 35:41
    in all these examples, just are different
  • 35:41 - 35:46
    place values. Again, there are lovely key
  • 35:46 - 35:50
    points and I might go here, and here
  • 35:50 - 35:52
    and for reason, and
  • 35:52 - 35:54
    5 and 5 makes 10, and 50 and 50 makes a
  • 35:54 - 35:59
    hundred. This isn't hard at all. So again, you
  • 35:59 - 36:02
    as the teacher or parent look at the child,
  • 36:02 - 36:06
    You look at that, and it's working [inaudible]
  • 36:06 - 36:10
    and that's kind of important. Here I've
  • 36:10 - 36:13
    used symbols, again, I've used
  • 36:13 - 36:20
    manipulatives. [inaudible] Okay...
  • 36:20 - 36:23
    Algebra...let's just play around a litle
  • 36:23 - 36:26
    bit here before we get to Algebra, let's
  • 36:26 - 36:30
    be patient...in moments...
  • 36:30 - 36:32
    In this particular slide I'm starting off by
  • 36:32 - 36:34
    showing you there is a relationship
  • 36:34 - 36:38
    between addition and subtraction. Those
  • 36:38 - 36:42
    inchworm learners, those original learners
  • 36:42 - 36:45
    that I've talked about, don't see that
  • 36:45 - 36:49
    connection unless it is pointed out. I do
  • 36:49 - 36:52
    want to point it out, so here's a little
  • 36:52 - 36:56
    problem: 100 minus 0 is what? What would
  • 36:56 - 36:59
    I put in that little space there, what
  • 36:59 - 37:05
    would be my answer? It's 100. Let me
  • 37:05 - 37:08
    now make that an addition problem. What
  • 37:08 - 37:10
    would I add on to 0, what would I put in
  • 37:10 - 37:13
    this little box here to make 100? Again, I
  • 37:13 - 37:16
    hope you are seeing, that's 100. So I am
  • 37:16 - 37:19
    relating subtraction and addition, using
  • 37:19 - 37:23
    addition facts to make subtraction facts.
  • 37:23 - 37:31
    Let's do-what would I put in the box, for
  • 37:31 - 37:34
    this subtraction? I would put 80, same as
  • 37:34 - 37:38
    I would put here in this box. The question
  • 37:38 - 37:41
    is, what happens when I subtract 20
  • 37:41 - 37:45
    from 100? I take 20 away from 100, again
  • 37:45 - 37:49
    the calvery comes in, I get 80. What do I
  • 37:49 - 37:53
    add on to 20 to get 100? I add 80.
  • 37:53 - 37:58
    Fifty, the same deal. Seventy, same deal.
  • 37:58 - 38:03
    Relating addition to subtraction.
  • 38:03 - 38:06
    You need to understand that
  • 38:06 - 38:09
    is a law, because if you don't you're not
  • 38:09 - 38:14
    going to get the Algebra. I've subtly
  • 38:14 - 38:17
    introduced Algebra, because I've asked you,
  • 38:17 - 38:20
    what number goes in here, to make this
  • 38:20 - 38:25
    side equal that side. Let's put that square
  • 38:25 - 38:31
    away for a moment, and let's replace it,
  • 38:31 - 38:36
    with an 'x'. ~All~ I've done is taken the
  • 38:36 - 38:39
    square away, and replaced it, with an 'x'.
  • 38:39 - 38:44
    Now my question is, what number would I
  • 38:44 - 38:49
    write instead of 'x' to make this equation
  • 38:49 - 38:53
    the same on both sides. So what number
  • 38:53 - 38:56
    do I add to 20, to make 80 ?
  • 38:56 - 38:59
    In order to get 'x', you have to tell me,
  • 38:59 - 39:02
    what number 'x' represents. In maths we
  • 39:02 - 39:06
    often use the instruction, find 'x'. I've
  • 39:06 - 39:08
    seen the classic cartoon where the child
  • 39:08 - 39:10
    looked all around and said,
  • 39:10 - 39:13
    says,"Here it is!". That's not what we meant.
  • 39:13 - 39:18
    Let's do it again, with the next set. Just
  • 39:18 - 39:22
    get rid of the box, let's put in the
  • 39:22 - 39:25
    letter, this time use 'y'. What is the
  • 39:25 - 39:29
    value of 'y' that makes this side 100,
  • 39:29 - 39:32
    make it equal to that side.
  • 39:32 - 39:35
    If you don't understand the numbers,
  • 39:35 - 39:41
    you don't understand the Algebra. If I was
  • 39:41 - 39:44
    teaching Algebra, I might want to go back
  • 39:44 - 39:48
    to the early numbers, that is important.
  • 39:48 - 39:52
    Sorry, Coleen, fractions, we're not going
  • 39:52 - 39:56
    to have time for that tonite, but I will
  • 39:56 - 40:02
    mention them, not very constructively.
  • 40:02 - 40:08
    With the four operations we use: addition,
  • 40:08 - 40:16
    subtraction, multiplication, division...
  • 40:16 - 40:21
    they are all linked. There is a hugely
  • 40:21 - 40:24
    confusing language around addition and
  • 40:24 - 40:27
    subtraction. Sometimes with subtraction,
  • 40:27 - 40:29
    we say 'take away', sometimes 'what's the
  • 40:29 - 40:33
    difference', sometimes we say 'subtract'.
  • 40:33 - 40:37
    Just as a quick illustration here, I want
  • 40:37 - 40:39
    to use these number combinations of 10
  • 40:39 - 40:42
    again. What is the difference between a
  • 40:42 - 40:48
    thousand, and 438? I've drawn a number
  • 40:48 - 40:53
    line, so here's my 1000, here's my 438--
  • 40:53 - 40:55
    this is the difference. What is this bit
  • 40:55 - 40:59
    here? That's what I want to know.
  • 40:59 - 41:02
    I'm going to do that, by adding. I'm
  • 41:02 - 41:07
    going to add, first of all, 2 to the 8, to
  • 41:07 - 41:13
    make it 40, that little tiny bit there,
  • 41:13 - 41:17
    headed towards a thousand. What do I add
  • 41:17 - 41:21
    on to 440, to make the hundred? Sixty.
  • 41:21 - 41:23
    These are my number bonds for 10.
  • 41:23 - 41:31
    I've got to 500. What do I add on to 500
  • 41:31 - 41:35
    to make 1000? Five hundred. Now I've gone
  • 41:35 - 41:37
    from there to there, I've found this
  • 41:37 - 41:40
    difference by addition. That's the way,
  • 41:40 - 41:43
    since we can't do money much today, that's
  • 41:43 - 41:45
    the way when I was a boy, we used to make
  • 41:45 - 41:48
    change. We could make it by adding on from
  • 41:48 - 41:50
    the purchase price to the money that the
  • 41:50 - 41:53
    customer gave us. So, that's the answer.
  • 41:53 - 41:56
    I've done it in using number bonds of 10,
  • 41:56 - 41:59
    a 100, and a 1000, and I"ve done it in
  • 41:59 - 42:08
    stages to help with that. If you want to do
  • 42:08 - 42:13
    the take away, 1000 minus 438, is so
  • 42:13 - 42:17
    difficult, when you do it as a take away.
  • 42:17 - 42:21
    Can I suggest you get on google or whatever
  • 42:21 - 42:23
    search engine you use, and search for
  • 42:23 - 42:26
    'Tom Lehrer and New Math'. Tom Lehrer was
  • 42:26 - 42:30
    a math professor at Harvard and a great
  • 42:30 - 42:34
    song writer, a tremendously, often black,
  • 42:34 - 42:37
    sense of humor, and see how he explains
  • 42:37 - 42:40
    how you do this type of calculation. It's
  • 42:40 - 42:44
    wonderful, much more entertaining than I
  • 42:44 - 42:49
    can be. This "what else are you teaching?"
  • 42:49 - 42:51
    this principal, and then I'll move on to a
  • 42:51 - 42:57
    new topic in a moment..."9". Grasshoppers,
  • 42:57 - 43:01
    these intuitive thinkers, love 9, because
  • 43:01 - 43:04
    it's close to 10, they don't see 9,
  • 43:04 - 43:09
    they see "9" as unique. Inchworms see 9
  • 43:09 - 43:12
    as lots of fingers, they DON'T see it next
  • 43:12 - 43:17
    to 10, they see it very, very literally.
  • 43:17 - 43:21
    They add 9 by adding 1,2,3,...til they get
  • 43:21 - 43:23
    to 9. And they add 99 in the same way,
  • 43:23 - 43:25
    they see it singly.
  • 43:25 - 43:28
    Shops, yet again, a bit about money. Shops
  • 43:28 - 43:32
    use 99 cents or 99 pence at the end.
  • 43:32 - 43:35
    Something is priced at 14 dollars and 99
  • 43:35 - 43:37
    cents [$14.99] because they know
  • 43:37 - 43:39
    psychologically a lot of people see the 14
  • 43:39 - 43:42
    dollars, they don't see the ".99", they
  • 43:42 - 43:46
    don't see it as close to 15 dollars. I'd
  • 43:46 - 43:50
    want to teach that you have 10, you round
  • 43:50 - 43:55
    up as a way of avoiding the psychological
  • 43:55 - 43:59
    price. Inchworms, again, you might convince
  • 43:59 - 44:03
    them that adding 10 can happen, but then
  • 44:03 - 44:05
    they have trouble, knowing they have to
  • 44:05 - 44:08
    subtract. Why? Because it's addition, and
  • 44:08 - 44:10
    they don't interrelate operations and
  • 44:10 - 44:15
    numbers. These are skills we are teaching
  • 44:15 - 44:17
    to make people more comfortable
  • 44:17 - 44:20
    with number in everyday life, often,
  • 44:20 - 44:22
    with money.
  • 44:22 - 44:26
    Estimation is what grasshoppers do
  • 44:26 - 44:28
    naturally. They are good at that, they have
  • 44:28 - 44:30
    great number sense. They may not be great
  • 44:30 - 44:33
    at other things, but that's one of their
  • 44:33 - 44:35
    great skills. Inchworms, not natural
  • 44:35 - 44:40
    estimators. With this 9, I am teaching
  • 44:40 - 44:43
    estimation, inter-relating numbers and
  • 44:43 - 44:46
    operation, rounding up to the nearest 10
  • 44:46 - 44:48
    and so on. I am also teaching people to
  • 44:48 - 44:52
    appraise their answer. After it all, I want
  • 44:52 - 44:54
    them to know, is your estimate bigger or
  • 44:54 - 44:57
    smaller than the real answer. Estimation
  • 44:57 - 45:01
    is taking away that pressure of decision,
  • 45:01 - 45:06
    and introducing a new idea upon them.
  • 45:06 - 45:08
    I need to get moving, I've been talking
  • 45:08 - 45:12
    for a long time. Any questions again at
  • 45:12 - 45:27
    this break?...[pause] I'll move on to
  • 45:27 - 45:32
    conceptualizing. Money is quite good
  • 45:32 - 45:35
    for introducing conceptualization, it
  • 45:35 - 45:40
    combines two things there. In terms of
  • 45:40 - 45:45
    place value, the money in the U.S.A, you
  • 45:45 - 45:49
    have this coin called a quarter, which
  • 45:49 - 45:52
    is not as good for maths, for place values,
  • 45:52 - 45:55
    kind of good for fractions, not as good
  • 45:55 - 46:01
    for regular mathematics. In our currency,
  • 46:01 - 46:04
    and in lots of other countries we have a
  • 46:04 - 46:09
    1 cent, 2 cent, 5 cent, 10 cent, 20 cent,
  • 46:09 - 46:12
    50 cent...there is a good pattern for
  • 46:12 - 46:16
    breaking down numbers. But you can use
  • 46:16 - 46:20
    trading with money, so this crossing from
  • 46:20 - 46:25
    units to tens. If you get 10 single cents, you
  • 46:25 - 46:30
    can trade them for a dime. You can trade
  • 46:30 - 46:35
    10 dimes for a dollar. In the U.K., it
  • 46:35 - 46:38
    would be 10 pennies for a ten pence coin,
  • 46:38 - 46:43
    and 10 ten pence coins for a pound. You
  • 46:43 - 46:46
    can use coins to relate when transforming
  • 46:46 - 46:47
    and conceptualizing.
  • 46:47 - 46:50
    Let me motor on because I am talking too
  • 46:50 - 46:56
    much which I always do. Again, BIG research
  • 46:56 - 46:58
    Children/Adults don't know their
  • 46:58 - 47:02
    multiplication facts, their times table
  • 47:02 - 47:05
    facts. This, again, when I ask teachers,
  • 47:05 - 47:08
    BIG problem, 50 to 70% of children age 10
  • 47:08 - 47:13
    in the U.K. don't master this task. Again,
  • 47:13 - 47:16
    what you need to learn, what you've got to
  • 47:16 - 47:19
    learn are 0's, 1's, 2's, 5's and 10's. So
  • 47:19 - 47:25
    what I'll do, is to try to show you how
  • 47:25 - 47:31
    these can be done...got to keep working.
  • 47:31 - 47:34
    Here we go...what else are you teaching?
  • 47:34 - 47:36
    Rote learning is not good for a lot of
  • 47:36 - 47:40
    dyslexic students. I nearly destroyed my
  • 47:40 - 47:43
    relationship with my first bunch of
  • 47:43 - 47:46
    dyslexic students by trying to enforce rote
  • 47:46 - 47:49
    learning, the times table facts upon them.
  • 47:49 - 47:53
    This can be an advantage because if you
  • 47:53 - 47:58
    don't rote learn them, you have to learn
  • 47:58 - 48:06
    them in other ways, so I go back to the
  • 48:06 - 48:10
    motto, "Teach the subject as it is, to the
  • 48:10 - 48:12
    child as he is." If rote learning doesn't
  • 48:12 - 48:15
    work, let's go for something deeper. Let's
  • 48:15 - 48:18
    make a demotivating experience a positive
  • 48:18 - 48:20
    experience. We need to know what
  • 48:20 - 48:23
    multiplication is: let's just remind
  • 48:23 - 48:25
    ourselves that addition is about adding
  • 48:25 - 48:29
    together numbers, multiplication is about
  • 48:29 - 48:33
    adding "lots of" the same number. Now,
  • 48:33 - 48:36
    lots of sometimes means ~many~ in everyday
  • 48:36 - 48:40
    life. Lots of in maths can be 2 lots of,
  • 48:40 - 48:43
    or 3 lots of, it doesn't have to be mega
  • 48:43 - 48:46
    numbers. Here is 3 "lots of " 4, added
  • 48:46 - 48:52
    together. I like that it is 3 'x' 4,
  • 48:52 - 48:55
    three times four [inaudible]
  • 48:55 - 49:00
    Again, symbols are communitive property[??]
  • 49:00 - 49:02
    I wouldn't expect a child to add
  • 49:02 - 49:04
    all of these together, that would be very
  • 49:04 - 49:06
    time consuming, as someone mentioned,
  • 49:06 - 49:09
    working memory, it certainly swamped
  • 49:09 - 49:11
    the working memory. This is a tough fact,
  • 49:11 - 49:14
    it's one of the facts that is very hard to
  • 49:14 - 49:18
    learn. I don't actually know why, but it
  • 49:18 - 49:21
    is incredibly hard for children. Let me
  • 49:21 - 49:25
    give you a quick lesson on how to help
  • 49:25 - 49:28
    with this, and teach something else
  • 49:28 - 49:31
    at the same time. So here is me,
  • 49:31 - 49:36
    representing 4 and 4 and 4 visually. I
  • 49:36 - 49:40
    could get an early estimate by popping this
  • 49:40 - 49:44
    square in here, that square in here...that
  • 49:44 - 49:46
    would give me two fives, and give me 12.
  • 49:46 - 49:48
    That's not what I want to do right now.
  • 49:48 - 49:51
    What I want to do right now is take this
  • 49:51 - 49:56
    image of 4, and change it into a new image,
  • 49:56 - 50:01
    all in a line. Two reasons: Here is
  • 50:01 - 50:04
    3 x 4 I'm doing visually. I could put them
  • 50:04 - 50:08
    all together and have 3 x 4, or I can
  • 50:08 - 50:12
    seperate them, and have 2 lots of 4, and
  • 50:12 - 50:15
    have 1 lot of 4. Two easy facts, rather
  • 50:15 - 50:22
    than one hard fact. Similarly, for 7 x 6, I
  • 50:22 - 50:27
    can line up 7 separate 6's, or I can make
  • 50:27 - 50:34
    it an area, I've got 7 lots of 6, or I could
  • 50:34 - 50:39
    break it up into 2 parts, five 6's and
  • 50:39 - 50:43
    two 6's, trying to introduce this idea of
  • 50:43 - 50:46
    a multiple that you might know, and a
  • 50:46 - 50:49
    multiple that you might know, and putting
  • 50:49 - 50:53
    them together. That's part of the deal.
  • 50:53 - 50:56
    In symbols, I'm showing you here how to
  • 50:56 - 51:02
    use a partial products to work out the
  • 51:02 - 51:05
    facts they don't know. Sometimes, you
  • 51:05 - 51:07
    might want to make a few notes to support
  • 51:07 - 51:11
    the working memory, but what we are
  • 51:11 - 51:14
    teaching here is the basics of long
  • 51:14 - 51:19
    multiplication. That's good, we aren't just
  • 51:19 - 51:21
    wasting time, restraining [???] the child
  • 51:21 - 51:24
    with rote learning that doesn't work.
  • 51:24 - 51:26
    Once you know this principal, looking for
  • 51:26 - 51:31
    easy numbers, 2 and 1 are 3, 5 and 2 are 7.
  • 51:31 - 51:34
    Twelve breaks down into 10 and 2. I used
  • 51:34 - 51:37
    to teach the 12x's tables, but we don't
  • 51:37 - 51:40
    use them very often anymore. But I know my
  • 51:40 - 51:42
    10's and I know my 2 facts, and I combine
  • 51:42 - 51:46
    them. Fifteen---I don't teach that, but
  • 51:46 - 51:51
    I know my 10, and I know 5's, I know
  • 51:51 - 51:54
    that 5 is half of 10, so I can use this
  • 51:54 - 51:59
    strategy to go by. When we go up to the
  • 51:59 - 52:02
    cheat-sheet, yeah, I think sometimes "just
  • 52:02 - 52:04
    give them the cheat-sheet" so we can
  • 52:04 - 52:07
    concentrate on the concept and not lose
  • 52:07 - 52:09
    out all the time if I'm not getting these
  • 52:09 - 52:13
    facts. I kind of like the idea of spending
  • 52:13 - 52:16
    some time on teaching this method because
  • 52:16 - 52:18
    it is a conceptual lesson, and it's not
  • 52:18 - 52:21
    just teaching how to access the fact--it's
  • 52:21 - 52:24
    teaching the principals of multiplication,
  • 52:24 - 52:28
    and indeed, division. What I'm saying here
  • 52:28 - 52:31
    is adding each separate number is a many
  • 52:31 - 52:36
    step procedure. This, the cheat-sheet
  • 52:36 - 52:39
    method or rote learning is the one step
  • 52:39 - 52:42
    method. I'm kind of offering an alternative
  • 52:42 - 52:45
    in between, the two-step method. You can't
  • 52:45 - 52:49
    do this in one, can you do it in two?
  • 52:49 - 52:54
    It's not as much as the many, it's the two.
  • 52:54 - 52:57
    I try to make the two easy, and these are
  • 52:57 - 53:02
    automatic, low load on working memory.
  • 53:02 - 53:05
    I want to get people off this total
  • 53:05 - 53:08
    reliance on rote learning, because it is
  • 53:08 - 53:11
    so hard. That's why an alternative to the
  • 53:11 - 53:15
    cheat-sheets in some situations--These are
  • 53:15 - 53:18
    the core facts, you combine them to make
  • 53:18 - 53:22
    other facts. So I combine 'this' and 'this'
  • 53:22 - 53:26
    one 8 and five 8 are six 8. Conception,
  • 53:26 - 53:29
    one can understand that combination. Five
  • 53:29 - 53:32
    8's and two 8's make seven 8's. Two 8's and
  • 53:32 - 53:36
    ten 8's to make twelve 8's. What I'm
  • 53:36 - 53:39
    trying to do is give strategies that are
  • 53:39 - 53:43
    consistent. Combining facts that you know,
  • 53:43 - 53:48
    using facts that you know to extend the
  • 53:48 - 53:51
    concepts, to extend the facts you do.
  • 53:51 - 53:54
    A little bit of Algebra coming up here
  • 53:54 - 53:57
    slowly. So I'm doing six lots, six lots
  • 53:57 - 54:01
    of 6, transferring that into five lots and
  • 54:01 - 54:05
    one lot. Six lots of 7, six lots of 8, six
  • 54:05 - 54:09
    lots of 9. Same split every time into five
  • 54:09 - 54:12
    lots of 6, one lot of 6;
  • 54:12 - 54:19
    five lots of 7 and one lot of 7,
  • 54:19 - 54:22
    five lots of 8, one of 8; five lots of 9,
  • 54:22 - 54:28
    one of 9. This is not a quick fix, but
  • 54:28 - 54:32
    you are wasting less learning time, because
  • 54:32 - 54:35
    you are teaching concepts. This is
  • 54:35 - 54:37
    conceptual, this is about teaching
  • 54:37 - 54:42
    mathematics, what is facts.
  • 54:42 - 54:46
    Let's put this now into symbols, and what
  • 54:46 - 54:49
    I've now done, is left those visual images
  • 54:49 - 54:53
    behind, and focused on the symbols.
  • 54:53 - 54:58
    6 lots of 6, 5 lots and 1; 6 lots of 7,
  • 54:58 - 55:03
    5 lots and 1; 6 lots of 8, 5 lots and 1;
  • 55:03 - 55:07
    6 lots of 9, 5 lots and 1. It's going to
  • 55:07 - 55:11
    work for any number: 6 '21's", 5 "21's"
  • 55:11 - 55:17
    and 1 "21's". Oh, there's a hand in there,
  • 55:17 - 55:21
    and I don't know why...This is meant to be
  • 55:21 - 55:24
    5 lots of any number you want to write...
  • 55:24 - 55:27
    6 lots of any number you want to write,
  • 55:27 - 55:30
    5 lots of any number and 1 lot of any
  • 55:30 - 55:35
    number. Instead of this hand, I can use
  • 55:35 - 55:40
    an 'n'. I can use an 'n', I can use a hand.
  • 55:40 - 55:43
    6 lots of any number: 5 lots of any
  • 55:43 - 55:45
    number plus 1 of any number.
  • 55:45 - 55:48
    And, as you know, in Algebra, we don't use
  • 55:48 - 55:56
    times. This question, Christine, about
  • 55:56 - 55:58
    dyslexic student's that taught the methods
  • 55:58 - 56:01
    to themselves. Yep, that's where I learnt
  • 56:01 - 56:04
    it, I learnt it from my dyslexic students.
  • 56:04 - 56:08
    Just kind of streamlined it a bit, little
  • 56:08 - 56:10
    bit of order [??] A generation of dyslexic
  • 56:10 - 56:14
    students taught me all this stuff.
  • 56:14 - 56:18
    Okay, what is it about division, the tail
  • 56:18 - 56:23
    end of my session. I'm only doing this as
  • 56:23 - 56:26
    an illustration, again of the complexity
  • 56:26 - 56:29
    of maths, and how we can help, to some
  • 56:29 - 56:34
    extent, so this optimal [inaudible]
  • 56:34 - 56:36
    can do. I'll just give you a little bit of
  • 56:36 - 56:38
    data from the U.K., this is some data from
  • 56:38 - 56:44
    the standardized tests. In the U.K., at ten
  • 56:44 - 56:48
    years old, about 3/4 of the students
  • 56:48 - 56:52
    across the U.K. completed this. This
  • 56:52 - 56:56
    division, less than 1/4 of the ten year
  • 56:56 - 56:59
    olds can do. At thirteen, it's still there
  • 56:59 - 57:06
    about 3/4 to just over 40%. At 15, it's
  • 57:06 - 57:10
    gone up to 83%, still only 44% . Division
  • 57:10 - 57:19
    is tough, learning it is complicated.
  • 57:19 - 57:22
    Nearly finished, for those of you that are
  • 57:22 - 57:25
    listening. Division and fractions,
  • 57:25 - 57:28
    again, when I ask teachers around the
  • 57:28 - 57:30
    world what are the subjects that kids
  • 57:30 - 57:34
    find hard, these are the top two.
  • 57:34 - 57:37
    Fractions, of course, incorporate division.
  • 57:37 - 57:39
    It's a culture, the culture is that these
  • 57:39 - 57:42
    two topics are hard, so it helps people
  • 57:42 - 57:45
    pre-empt their feelings of failure,
  • 57:45 - 57:48
    because we know it's tough, and that's
  • 57:48 - 57:52
    the beginning. Why is it hard? Because
  • 57:52 - 57:55
    our language is. We say "sixteen
  • 57:55 - 57:58
    divided by 3". We also say, "How many
  • 57:58 - 58:02
    threes in sixteen?" That reversal of the
  • 58:02 - 58:06
    numbers involved will confuse some children
  • 58:06 - 58:09
    before they even begin. Again, I want this
  • 58:09 - 58:14
    to be conceptual. I want it to be
  • 58:14 - 58:18
    understandable. Link things together.
  • 58:18 - 58:20
    Addition is about adding together numbers,
  • 58:20 - 58:22
    subtraction is about taking away. They are
  • 58:22 - 58:24
    inverse, they are opposites.
  • 58:24 - 58:27
    Multiplication is about adding 'lots of'
  • 58:27 - 58:30
    the ~same~ number. We can guess, that
  • 58:30 - 58:34
    division is about subtracting/taking away
  • 58:34 - 58:42
    lots of the same number. About 5 slides left.
  • 58:42 - 58:44
    Sixteen divided by three-- how many lots
  • 58:44 - 58:47
    of 3 in 16? So, I'm using multiplication
  • 58:47 - 58:50
    language to relate the concept.
  • 58:50 - 58:54
    So let's just take away one 3...we've got 13
  • 58:54 - 58:58
    Another 3, we've got 10. Another 3, we've
  • 58:58 - 59:02
    got 7, another 3, we've got 4...so what
  • 59:02 - 59:06
    I've done is taken away 5 lots of 3, and
  • 59:06 - 59:12
    I've got 1 left over, 1 remainder. Let me
  • 59:12 - 59:16
    just rearrange that for you, and I'm going
  • 59:16 - 59:19
    to do the same again, "How many 3's in 16?"
  • 59:19 - 59:25
    Take away 3, and another 3, and another 3,
  • 59:25 - 59:28
    the fourth 3, and the fifth 3. I've taken
  • 59:28 - 59:32
    away five 3's, and I've got 1 left over.
  • 59:32 - 59:36
    That is the way we present short division
  • 59:36 - 59:41
    in the U.K. How many 3's in 16? Five, and
  • 59:41 - 59:45
    1 left over. This also relates to a
  • 59:45 - 59:49
    rectangles, again, what else am I teaching.
  • 59:49 - 59:50
    I am trying to
  • 59:50 - 59:53
    teach, this is a rectangle. You see
  • 59:53 - 59:56
    it's 3 on one side of the rectangle, and
  • 59:56 - 59:59
    you see it's 5, linking multiplication
  • 59:59 - 60:00
    and division. Again, I am
  • 60:00 - 60:04
    doing this very quickly, but I'm trying to
  • 60:04 - 60:07
    illustrate these with the visuals, and the
  • 60:07 - 60:09
    icon. Einstein said, among
  • 60:09 - 60:13
    many other things, "Everything should be
  • 60:13 - 60:17
    made as simple as possible, but not
  • 60:17 - 60:20
    simpler." I'm just delighted and very
  • 60:20 - 60:24
    honored [inaudible] that you are here, thank
  • 60:24 - 60:28
    you. Let's just quickly look at what children
  • 60:28 - 60:32
    are often taught, and why this is so hard.
  • 60:32 - 60:36
    "How many 23's in 16?" There aren't any.
  • 60:36 - 60:42
    "How many 23's in 168? " How the heck do I
  • 60:42 - 60:46
    know? It's so difficult, so that's the
  • 60:46 - 60:50
    barrier; already, some kids are gone.
  • 60:50 - 60:53
    What we do is, let's say, we work out a
  • 60:53 - 60:57
    answer of 7. We write the 7 here. How do I
  • 60:57 - 61:00
    know that? I don't know, so much the child
  • 61:00 - 61:05
    has to take in faith. We take away that, we
  • 61:05 - 61:07
    get 7, we bring down the 3, whatever that
  • 61:07 - 61:13
    means. How many 23's in 73? What is it?
  • 61:13 - 61:18
    Take away. We bring down the 6. SO MUCH
  • 61:18 - 61:20
    to remember in that sequence. Make 1
  • 61:20 - 61:27
    mistake, and your out! There's another way,
  • 61:27 - 61:30
    where I'll use a different example,
  • 61:30 - 61:33
    which relates to multiplication, relates to
  • 61:33 - 61:39
    that times tables. So again, only doing the
  • 61:39 - 61:43
    same thing again and again and again. What
  • 61:43 - 61:46
    you might do is link the two. Instead of
  • 61:46 - 61:48
    having to find out how many 17's in
  • 61:48 - 61:51
    something, what we are going to do here is
  • 61:51 - 61:53
    use repeated subtraction. Let's find
  • 61:53 - 61:55
    something to take away. Our initial
  • 61:55 - 61:58
    jottings might be, well, whats a hundred
  • 61:58 - 62:02
    times 17? That would be 1700, that's too
  • 62:02 - 62:05
    big, so we do some reappraising. I go to my
  • 62:05 - 62:09
    next easy multiple, and halve that, 50
  • 62:09 - 62:13
    lots, 850. At least I'm started, I've got the
  • 62:13 - 62:16
    layer started. I've taken away the 50
  • 62:16 - 62:21
    times 17, and got 357 left. Then we take
  • 62:21 - 62:25
    away twenty 17's, using that multiplication
  • 62:25 - 62:29
    again...and then what? You've still got to
  • 62:29 - 62:32
    subtract, you've still got to organize your
  • 62:32 - 62:35
    work in space, but I've made it more
  • 62:35 - 62:39
    conceptually manageable. And I'm using
  • 62:39 - 62:43
    key multiples in the subtraction, something
  • 62:43 - 62:48
    that's I've learned from [inaudible],
  • 62:48 - 62:52
    many years ago. Total numbers are there.
  • 62:52 - 62:55
    What this method does, and what it doesn't
  • 62:55 - 62:58
    do is, it's logical, it's conceptual,and
  • 62:58 - 63:01
    all the stuff we need. Nothing starts
  • 63:01 - 63:03
    with [inaudible], nothing starts with
  • 63:03 - 63:04
    [inaudible]. It's the
  • 63:04 - 63:06
    inverse of the method that I teach for
  • 63:06 - 63:09
    multiplication. There's less reliance on
  • 63:09 - 63:12
    memory for procedures, because I'm using
  • 63:12 - 63:13
    the procedure, that I use for
  • 63:13 - 63:15
    multiplication, just the inversion,
  • 63:15 - 63:19
    that's all. It uses core key fact multiples
  • 63:19 - 63:21
    so you don't need a cheat sheet, which you
  • 63:21 - 63:25
    don't have, by the way, for 17. It reduces
  • 63:25 - 63:28
    the chance for errors , and that feeling of
  • 63:28 - 63:30
    helplessness--"Can't do it! Not even going
  • 63:30 - 63:33
    to start." You have to be able to subtract,
  • 63:33 - 63:36
    do your spatial organization, and you do
  • 63:36 - 63:43
    need a knowledge of place value.
  • 63:43 - 63:45
    The devil is in the detail. I haven't done
  • 63:45 - 63:48
    anything that's not mathematical. What
  • 63:48 - 63:50
    I have done is focused on the details. So
  • 63:50 - 63:53
    often the details will cause the children
  • 63:53 - 63:59
    to fail.[inaudible] It's the details in
  • 63:59 - 64:02
    the language you use, the images you use,
  • 64:02 - 64:07
    the sanctions we often use. It's about
  • 64:07 - 64:10
    knowing the math from a learners
  • 64:10 - 64:13
    perspective, and that's why I said, my
  • 64:13 - 64:18
    dyslexic ones taught me this. It's about
  • 64:18 - 64:21
    preventing failure, getting to those
  • 64:21 - 64:26
    emotional ideas before they become a
  • 64:26 - 64:32
    block to learning. And....it's complicated!
  • 64:32 - 64:36
    Thank you all for all your great comments,
  • 64:36 - 64:40
    I really have enjoyed talking to you in a
  • 64:40 - 64:44
    virtual way, and I hope the feeling is mutual.
  • 64:44 - 64:49
    Thanks, bye-bye. Any questions, does
  • 64:49 - 64:55
    anyone want to keep going? [Dr. Fernette
  • 64:55 - 64:57
    Eide] Absolutely awesome, thank you so
  • 64:57 - 65:22
    much! ...Fantastic!...Questions?... I know
  • 65:22 - 65:25
    you didn't get a chance to use fractions.
  • 65:25 - 65:28
    Anything, generally? Do you use
  • 65:28 - 65:35
    manipulatives? Do you do the pie thing?
  • 65:35 - 65:39
    [Dr. Chinn] Pies are pretty good...pizza's
  • 65:39 - 65:42
    are cruel because you are just going to put
  • 65:42 - 65:47
    children on pizza's! What I do use is
  • 65:47 - 65:52
    square paper, squares of paper, and I
  • 65:52 - 65:54
    get kids to fold them.
  • 65:54 - 65:57
    The reason I use squares, I can fold in two
  • 65:57 - 66:01
    directions. What I also do,
  • 66:01 - 66:04
    I premark the paper so the kids
  • 66:04 - 66:08
    aren't messing around trying to fold into
  • 66:08 - 66:13
    3 pieces or 5 pieces. I make it easier for
  • 66:13 - 66:17
    them to follow it. With the folding, I can
  • 66:17 - 66:20
    show that if you fold it 5 times you get
  • 66:20 - 66:23
    5 parts. If you fold it 3 times, you get
  • 66:23 - 66:27
    3 parts. So the third is bigger than the
  • 66:27 - 66:31
    fifth. You look to what are big conceptual
  • 66:31 - 66:35
    barriers. A big conceptual barrier is that
  • 66:35 - 66:38
    1 over 3 is bigger than 1 over 5. Language
  • 66:38 - 66:42
    again is a problem: a half, a third, a
  • 66:42 - 66:46
    quarter, don't tell you...a fifth, a sixth,
  • 66:46 - 66:49
    a seventh at least tell you something. It's
  • 66:49 - 66:52
    challenging, all the stuff that kids have
  • 66:52 - 66:56
    learned about numbers before you. When you
  • 66:56 - 67:00
    see 25, you know that the 2 is 2 tens, the
  • 67:00 - 67:04
    5 is 5 units. When you see 2 over 5, the
  • 67:04 - 67:09
    2 is 2 units, the 5 is 5 units, but that
  • 67:09 - 67:12
    sneaky line in the middle is telling you
  • 67:12 - 67:16
    the 2 is divided by 5. Twenty-five, you
  • 67:16 - 67:22
    know that the 2 is multiplied by 10.
  • 67:22 - 67:25
    There's hidden information in fractions,
  • 67:25 - 67:28
    it's challenging fact consistency. Now
  • 67:28 - 67:33
    that I mention it, it's a big topic to deal
  • 67:33 - 67:36
    with, but you've got to do it with visuals
  • 67:36 - 67:38
    aides and manipulation. Otherwise the
  • 67:38 - 67:43
    symbols are just gone, not getting constancy.
  • 67:43 - 68:03
    [discussion about tech details...]
  • 68:03 - 68:05
    [Fernette Eide] Do you have lesson plans
  • 68:05 - 68:08
    for teachers? [Dr. Chinn] Ha, it sounds
  • 68:08 - 68:10
    like my brother asked that question! I'm
  • 68:10 - 68:13
    working on that right now. In fact, I've
  • 68:13 - 68:15
    done a great deal of work on that, and I
  • 68:15 - 68:21
    hope to find a publisher. It's a huge
  • 68:21 - 68:27
    amount of work, hardly a mixture of
  • 68:27 - 68:30
    power-points like some of what I've done
  • 68:30 - 68:33
    today, which could be used as power-points
  • 68:33 - 68:39
    or guidance, so far as manipulatives, some
  • 68:39 - 68:45
    topic sheets, talking to teachers about
  • 68:45 - 68:49
    progress for learning, other resources,
  • 68:49 - 68:50
    like the squares I
  • 68:50 - 68:56
    mentioned. There's about 100 worksheets
  • 68:56 - 69:02
    that are criterion referenced,
  • 69:02 - 69:04
    and all have been posted with solutions.
  • 69:04 - 69:07
    so that teachers can use those worksheets
  • 69:07 - 69:09
    with a guide-master for them. The
  • 69:09 - 69:13
    worksheets are also set up to highlight
  • 69:13 - 69:16
    some error patterns, not trying to make
  • 69:16 - 69:23
    children go wrong, but to allow some other
  • 69:23 - 69:26
    patterns to happen. We can then deal with
  • 69:26 - 69:29
    those before they become embedded in
  • 69:29 - 69:33
    the kids, or the adults mind. I find that
  • 69:33 - 69:36
    working with adults,like some I mentioned-
  • 69:36 - 69:38
    just go back to that early stuff and
  • 69:38 - 69:42
    present it in a way that's acceptable to
  • 69:42 - 69:46
    adults. These are often the adults who, as
  • 69:46 - 69:50
    children, gave up at 7 and spent the last
  • 69:50 - 69:57
    10 years hating maths. It's very sad.
  • 69:57 - 70:01
    Many levels in mastery, sometimes you can
  • 70:01 - 70:06
    bypass lower levels. A lot of my students
  • 70:06 - 70:10
    often found Algebra easier than Arithmetic
  • 70:10 - 70:12
    not because school had gotten any easier,
  • 70:12 - 70:15
    but because their number facts are so poor.
  • 70:15 - 70:19
    There are less number fact demands in
  • 70:19 - 70:23
    Algebra--it's conceptual, and often my
  • 70:23 - 70:26
    students are very good conceptually, they
  • 70:26 - 70:29
    are just prone to error, maths is not
  • 70:29 - 70:32
    forgiving in that. The question about
  • 70:32 - 70:36
    mainstreaming, my experience has been what
  • 70:36 - 70:40
    works for dyslexics, works for anybody.
  • 70:40 - 70:44
    There is a group just north of Boston who
  • 70:44 - 70:48
    [inaudible] technology that right now is
  • 70:48 - 70:51
    talking about [inaudiable] people
  • 70:51 - 70:55
    like dyslexics, and computics. What we
  • 70:55 - 71:00
    are learning, work with these students,
  • 71:00 - 71:05
    these adults, can be mainstreamed. What
  • 71:05 - 71:07
    we are doing, I've said from the start, is
  • 71:07 - 71:10
    we are teaching the subject as it is, to
  • 71:10 - 71:12
    the child as it is, not messing with the
  • 71:12 - 71:15
    mathematics, implied to keep the
  • 71:15 - 71:17
    mathematics the same, but I'm trying to
  • 71:17 - 71:19
    make it more conceptual rather than rote.
  • 71:19 - 71:22
    [Fernette] We'll close now...Thanks, Steve,
  • 71:22 - 71:26
    this was incredibly helpful and awesome, I
  • 71:26 - 71:28
    wish I'd been taught math this way. I think
  • 71:28 - 71:30
    a lot of people are thinking about this
  • 71:30 - 71:32
    now. I really appreciate all the time
  • 71:32 - 71:35
    you've given us. You really have a genius
  • 71:35 - 71:37
    for all this kind of instruction...[We]
  • 71:37 - 71:40
    really appreciate it, thank you so much!
  • 71:40 - 71:41
    [clapping sounds]
  • 71:41 - 71:44
    [Dr. Chinn] Thank you, everyone, for being
  • 71:44 - 71:47
    so kind and for such good questions.
  • 71:47 - 71:49
    [ Dr.Fernette Eide] You are a rock star!
  • 71:49 - 71:51
    [laughter]
Title:
Teaching Math to Dyslexic Students - Dr Steve Chinn
Description:

Audio may be better at this link: http://bit.ly/157Oz9U
Dyslexic Advantage webinar with teaching strategies to help students with dyslexia learn math.

Dr Chinn uses animations, visuals, and simple patterns to address issues such as math facts, arithmetic, multiplication and division, and more complex topics such as algebra. If you have trouble with audio try this site: http://bit.ly/157Oz9U

This is the best short program we have ever seen for teaching math to students with dyslexia and dyscalculia. Thanks, Steve!

Join the movement at: http://dyslexicadvantage.com and like this video to spread the word. Thank you!

more » « less
Video Language:
English

English subtitles

Revisions