-
Okay, now we're back and we're going to
be talking about how
-
some of these biological dispositions
might be related to
-
what we've already learned
about classical conditioning.
-
Okay so now we're touched on this
just a little bit at the beginning
-
when we talked about factors influencing
classical conditioning
-
we did talk about this concept
of belongingness.
-
We're going to introduce this new term,
which is sometimes referred to as
-
conditioned stimulus-unconditioned
stimulus relevance,
-
and the basic idea is that some stimuli
connect to each other,
-
belong to each other more naturally.
-
So the conditioned stimulus-unconditioned
stimulus relevance has to do with
-
this concept that some stimuli
are more natural,
-
there's a more natural connection
they tend to belong together.
-
Now we did talk about this when we
talked about classical conditioning.
-
Remember our Garcia and Koelling
"Bright Noisy Tasty Water" study.
-
Now remember with that study they had
the groups of-of rats
-
that were learning to avoid and they had
different stimuli
-
and remember that they found that
taste and nausea
-
were easy to connect together, perhaps
there was that CS-US relevance
-
and also lights and noise were easily
connected with shock
-
but when the sti-the conditioned stimuli,
-
taste wasn't as easily connected
with shock
-
and lights and noise were not as easily
connected with nausea.
-
That was that concept of belongingness
and that some stimuli
-
were more naturally connected
to each other.
-
Now we also know that belongingness
impacts the way that we
-
develop certain kinds of fears and we
talked a lot about fears
-
with classical conditioning and developing
fears of insects, and snakes,
-
and perhaps heights, and water.
-
Well again from an evolutionary
perspective it would make sense
-
that we're going to learn to avoid things
that could really cause us harm.
-
You know snakes can-can bite us
and kill us and so it kind of makes sense
-
for us to avoid those creatures,
and it would make sense for us to learn
-
to fear them quickly.
-
And the same thing with insects, um,
and then heights and water
-
are again things that could bring about
our demise,
-
and so from an evolutionary perspective
it's going to be easier for us
-
to learn these kinds of fears
-
than to learn fears for objects that don't
have that kind of connection to, um, our
-
our general welfare.
-
So we're not as likely to develop
strong fears
-
for, um, inanimate objects; for a ball,
for a telephone, for a desk.
-
Because within the species history,
you know, we haven't had to
-
watch out for a ball, or a telephone,
or a desk.
-
So we have talked about this belongingness
already this CS-US relevance.
-
We're going to talk a little bit more
about this with respect to foods
-
because again we're focusing on
an appetitive stimuli
-
and behaviors today.
-
We're going to be talking
a little bit more
-
about taste diversions and other kinds
of classical conditioning learning
-
and specifically how when we develop
these strong taste aversions,
-
often times this is a little bit different
-
than other kinds of
classical conditioning.
-
So we're going to focus on differences
and taste aversions
-
and how those differences might be
attributed to the evolution of our species
-
and how perhaps taste aversions are
just different
-
from other kinds of
classical conditioning.
-
So with respect to taste aversions,
-
remember how perhaps
we've had a bad experience
-
with something, something was associated
with making us sick
-
and now we can't taste that food we can't
smell that food
-
because we've had this bad experience.
Well one of the difference-
-
one of the differences
with taste aversions
-
and other kinds of classical conditioning
-
has to do with how quickly
we learn these aversions.
-
Now, taste aversions, often times we
can learn in just one trial.
-
We can have a bad experience with tequila
and only has to happen once
-
and now we are going to do everything
possible to avoid the taste of,
-
or the smell of tequila.
Now that's really pretty different
-
than most of the other examples
of classical conditioning.
-
So if you remember back to
the very beginning of the term
-
we talked about Pavlov, and the dog,
and the bell.
-
Most of the time with classical
conditioning several trials are needed
-
to learn to, um, make the conditioned
response.
-
Now that's really different, you don't
have to get sick multiple times
-
to be able to learn to avoid something
with respect to taste.
-
Right, and again that makes sense
from an evolutionary perspective.
-
How many times can we consume
something that's going to make us sick
-
and learn to, you know and live.
So,
-
for the- you know, the-the evolution
of the species it really makes some sense
-
that we can learn quickly,
through just a single trial.
-
Another major difference with respect
to taste aversions and other forms of
-
classical conditioning is the timing
of the conditioned stimulus
-
and the unconditioned stimulus.
Now with taste aversions often times
-
there's a very long delay between
conditioned stimulus,
-
tasting something or smelling something,
and then the unconditioned stimulus
-
that-that experience of being sick.
-
It could be 30 minutes,
it could be an hour,
-
it could be anywhere up to 24 hours
-
between when we eat something
and when we have that sick feeling.
-
Now that's not typical for
classical conditioning, other forms of
-
classical conditioning, whoops
wrong direction,
-
we've got to come back to that idea
of continuity,
-
that the conditioned stimulus
-
and the unconditioned stimulus
must be close together.
-
Now again thinking about the
Pavlov Paradigm.
-
The bell and the meat were presented
within seconds of each other
-
and for learning to occur there's got to
be an easy connection between
-
that conditioned stimulus and the
unconditioned stimulus.
-
So we're talking about things typically
being connected
-
within seconds of each but with taste
aversions,
-
often times we consume something
-
and we don't feel sick for 30 minutes,
an hour, 24 hours.
-
For other kinds of learning this
isn't going to occur.
-
We're not going to have the connection
but with taste aversions we are able
-
to still make that connection, um,
despite the delay
-
between the conditioned stimulus
and the unconditioned stimulus.
-
And again this is something that-that
kind of makes sense
-
from an evolutionary perspective, again it
would be important for us
-
to be able to connect that feeling of
nausea with what we tasted
-
even though there has been a really long
delay between consuming the food
-
and getting sick later because again,
in order to be able to survive
-
we have to be able to remember what it was
that made us sick
-
and make that connection.
-
Now moving in the other direction, we've
talked a lot about, uh,
-
taste aversions but the same could be said
for taste preferences.
-
So, from an evolutionary perspective there
may be some belongingness components too.
-
So we've already mentioned that we
are going to have this predisposition
-
to seek out and try to make connections
with salty and sweet kinds of tastes.
-
And so the book even talks about,
you know, if we want to get kids to-
-
to like vegetables more w-we would be
well served to try to get them
-
um, connected with sweeter
tasting vegetables.
-
So sprinkling sugar on broccoli might be
-
the way to increase
preferences for broccoli.
-
Um, I would also argue th-that a lot of
vegetables have,
-
um, a bit of a bitter taste to them
-
and so we might have
this predisposition to avoid
-
certain kinds of, uh, vegetables that
have that kind of a bitter taste.
-
I know a lot of super tasters,
often times avoid vegetables
-
and it's probably likely
because of th-that
-
taste of bitter is very enhanced for them.
-
They can't get the sweet taste
out of th- the vegetables.
-
So we're going to prefer, we have this
predisposition to seek out salty and sweet
-
kinds of foods.
I would also argue that we've got
-
some very strong conditioned
flavor preferences
-
and there's something that's know as
"The Medicine Effect".
-
So we're going to talk a little bit about
that and how that fits
-
with our biological predispositions.
So we talked about how if we have a food
-
that we've connected with getting sick,
we've eaten something and then later
-
we get sick.
We tend to avoid that kind of a food,
-
but the medicine effect actually works
in the opposite direction.
-
Any kind of food that we connect with
feeling better, starting to feel better
-
we might start to connect that food
with that feeling of, um, wellness.
-
So here's how this might work with
classical conditioning:
-
if we have an illness that's dissipating,
we're starting to get better.
-
So the unconditioned stimulus of the
illness starting to go away,
-
is making us feel better,
-
any kind of food that we're
pairing with that feeling better
-
can also start to take on
those properties.
-
So the classic is, um, consuming
chicken soup, or chicken noodle soup.
-
Right, so having chicken noodle soup
as you're starting to-
-
as an illness is starting to go away.
If the illness is starting to go away
-
we start to feel better.
So chicken soup being paired
-
with the illness starting to go away,
the chicken soup
-
can also get us to just feel better
and this can happen even when
-
we're not really sick.
So anything that's paired with an illness
-
going away and our body
starting to feel better,
-
can start to take on those properties and
also elicit that same kind of a response.
-
And so I'd like to get you to think
a little bit about:
-
what are the things that you tend to
think of as comfort foods?
-
I know for my kids comfort foods tend to
have lots of carbs
-
and so they were happy to help me pick out
this picture,
-
of I think it's chicken-fried steak
-
and we have maybe some mash potatoes,
and some bread, and some corn, um,
-
and the joke in our family
is that they eat this
-
and they have their little carb coma.
-
So they go to sleep.
-
But, um, and then over here we've got
I think this is some kind of a
-
mac and cheese too but the idea here
and what I want you to think about
-
are the foods that you think of
as comfort foods.
-
Are these foods that in the past have been
associated with you feeling better,
-
being paired with something else
and perhaps, um,
-
that could have happened through
the process of classical conditioning.
-
So I want to give you one more example of
this
-
and this is one from my own childhood.
-
So I always saw my grandma as being
somebody who, um, was associated with
-
feeling good, feeling safe, feeling
comfortable with my grandma.
-
And then consistently my grandma made
cookies, mmm.
-
Now again, we've got this biological
predisposition for sweets
-
but if I consistently pair being with
my grandma
-
who elicits this, these feeling of joy
and comfort with cookies.
-
Since she made cookies,
cookies and grandma
-
now the idea of being exposed to cookies
-
is also going to elicit these feelings
of joy and comfort.
-
So if you have certain desserts, certain
kinds of treats that you associated
-
with, um, experiences, stimuli that
elicit these really positive feelings.
-
You know, whenever you get together
with your family you always have
-
these special kinds of desserts,
-
it's likely that those desserts
are going to also take on
-
those, um, those properties that
are associated with, uh,
-
the unconditioned stimulus.
So feeling good, feeling positive,
-
feeling comfortable, feeling joy,
alright.
-
Um, and again like we said if you've got,
if the foods have a salty or a sweet
-
um, component to them we're already going
to have that biological predisposition
-
to get connected more easily.
-
Alright, so let's shift gears and talk
a little bit about biological dispositions
-
and how they could be connected
to operant conditioning.
-
So before we get to operant conditioning,
let's briefly talk about
-
instrumental conditioning.
-
So as you remember, instrumental
conditioning is associated with Thorndike
-
and Thorndike did propose this
law of belongingness and remember
-
with the law of belongingness, he said
that certain stimulus response connections
-
are more natural.
So now remembering back,
-
remember Thorndike had the puzzle box
and the cat.
-
And he proposed this law of belongingness
to explain
-
why certain connections could be developed
rather than others.
-
Now remember, Thorndike wanted to train
the cat to get out of the puzzle box by
-
pushing a lever and he found that he could
very readily get the cat
-
to scratch and push a lever to get out
of the puzzle box
-
but despite all of his efforts he could
not get the cat to learn to groom himself.
-
So the cat couldn't be trained to groom
himself, herself to get out of the box
-
but the cat could be trained to push a
lever or to stra- to scratch to get out
-
and Thorndike talked about this
with respect to belongingness.
-
So scratching and pushing a lever
or a more natural connection
-
to the escape response where as, grooming
was not a natural connection,
-
not a natural, um, response to connect
with, um, escape.
-
And so law of belongingness would say
that some stimulus response connections
-
are more natural, so that's definitely
getting to this idea of
-
a biological predisposition.
-
Now with respect to operant conditioning,
I want you to try to remember
-
when we talk about Skinner, remember
Skinner created the skinner boxes
-
and the whole point of creating a
skinner box was he was trying to create
-
an environment that would avoid
fixed action patterns,
-
would avoid reflexes.
-
So Skinner was trying to do everything
possible to eliminate these
-
biological dispositions.
He created a little world and tried to, um
-
use responses that were within the
organism's repertoire, something that
-
they could do but not something that they
would normally do because he wanted
-
to avoid those reflexes.
He wanted to avoid those, those stimuli
-
that were naturally
connected to responses.
-
So now he tried to do that
-
and he tried to train his students
to do that but
-
what we're going to see is t-that
y-you know, w-we can't completely ignore
-
what our biological predispositions are
and there are, there is to some extent
-
a sense of belongingness with certain
kinds of reinforcers.
-
There is a biological predisposition to
-
act a certain way with
certain reinforcers.
-
So we're going to talk a little bit about
some of Skinner's students.
-
We're going to talk about the Brelands,
so this is Keller Breland
-
and this is his wife Marion Breland and the
Brelands were students of Skinner and, um,
-
they learned principles of operant
conditioning and shaping
-
and they went on to have a very successful
career and they spent a lot of their time
-
using techniques of shaping and operant
conditioning to train animals
-
to engage in different kinds of responses.
So here you've got a picture of an animal
-
learning to play a piano, but what the
Brelands found from all of their time
-
working with animals, and we'll talk more
about it, is that there is this
-
biological predisposition towards certain
kinds of reinforcers.
-
And so even though shaping could be used
to learn a set of responses, um,
-
the animals that they worked with
tended to revert back to some of their
-
natural tendencies, specifically with
food reinforcers.
-
Now the Brelands are famous for writing
this article,
-
"The Misbehavior of Organisms"
-
and this came out in 1961 and if you're
not as familiar with the literature
-
i-i-the name of the article might not make
a whole lot of sense to you.
-
So clearly they're talking about
misbehavior, and animal doing things
-
that they weren't trained to do but to
some extent this was poking fun at
-
their professor, Skinner, because Skinner
had earlier written a book entitled,
-
"The Behavior of Organisms", that was one
of his most famous works
-
and so what they're writing about in this
article, the misbehavior,
-
is they're kind of poking fun
at what Skinner taught them.
-
So lets talk a little bit about
-
what they meant by this misbehavior of
organisms and how they came to learn
-
what they did and how this relates back
to our topic for the day.
-
So I want to encourage you after you're
done with this segment to go and, uh,
-
click on this particular web page,
it's links to Moodle.
-
And this is the webpage for something that
the Brelands created
-
which is referred to as the "IQ Zoo".
So here's an advertisement for the IQ Zoo.
-
And so what, um, the IQ Zoo was, it was a
tourist attraction
-
that was open in the 1950's and it
operated continuously through the 1990's
-
and this was something that Keller
and Marion Breland, um,
-
created as an opportunity for the public
to see what animals could do, um.
-
They wanted to get animals familiar- they
wanted the public to see the skills
-
that animals have and how they can be
-
trained to do a wide range of
different things
-
and they were really interested in, um,
having-having people see how smart
-
animals were but also what they were
capable of doing based on
-
the principles of operant conditioning.
So in the 1940's the-the Belands,
-
the Keller and Marion Breland had started,
uh, training animals to do
-
these different kinds of tricks to help
General Mills sell farm feed
-
and then what they found was that they
could train animals to do a wide range
-
of different things and so then in the
1950's they opened up this IQ Zoo.
-
And the animals that they trained the-they
trained a wide range of different animals
-
and as is says in the advertisement here
their work has appeared on TV,
-
they appeared on the Ed Sullivan show,
on Animal Kingdom, um you might want to
-
ask your parents or ask your grandparents
if they've ever heard of the IQ Zoo,
-
or saw any of these kinds of animals, um,
it was super impressive
-
and like I said this is, uh, this is, um
a tourist attraction that ran from
-
the 1950's through the 1990's, it was
very well known in-in Arkansas.
-
So what did they train animals to do?
And if you go to t he webpage
-
it will show you lots of these different
behaviors.
-
They had a duck who could play a song on
the guitar,
-
they had trained hamsters to, um,
-
swing on trapeze, and this example we
have a pig that's been trained to, um,
-
pull down a watering can and the watering
can comes down and gives t-the pig
-
a little bit of, uh, bath.
Um, these are just small little snippets.
-
If you go to the IQ Zoo web page,
you'll see that they have, um,
-
20 or 30 different kinds of animals that
have been trained to do these
-
really interesting and entertaining kinds
of behaviors
-
and they used principles of
Operant Conditioning to do this
-
but so how does this relate to what we're
talking about right now
-
in misbehavior of organisms?
Well as I mentioned the Brelands
-
spent all this time training animals to
do these interesting things
-
but they ran into difficulties
along the way
-
and they wrote about those difficulties
-
in their article, "The Misbehavior of
Organisms".
-
And so we're going to talk about two of
their examples
-
and ultimately it comes down to this idea
of instinctive drift
-
and with instinctive drift
-
it's the concept th-that
organisms, animals,
-
but humans are the same, we tend to revert
back to our biologically based
-
response patterns.
So even though we can use shaping
-
and principles of operant conditioning
to learn different kinds of behaviors
-
we have this predisposition to go back
-
to what is part of our
innate response pattern.
-
So we're going to talk
about two examples here
-
and you've probably heard about these
-
in other classes but I think it-it's
worth while to talk about here.
-
So this is the example with the pig
and essentially they used
-
principles of shaping to get a pig to
pick up a coin, you can kind of see it
-
in this picture, the pig has the coin in
it's mouth.
-
The pig had to learn to pick up the coin
carry it in its mouth
-
and deposit it into, and it's kind of hard
to see in this picture
-
but this was like a little
metal piggy bank.
-
So they can use principals of shaping
-
remember with shaping it's reinforcing
closer approximations
-
of the correct response, they could get
the pig to do this, um,
-
and the pig did it for awhile, but what
they started to notice was the pig
-
started to engage in different kinds
of behaviors along the way.
-
So even after the pig had learned a
response pattern,
-
what the pig was supposed to do, it would
start to pick up the coin
-
and then toss it up in the air, and then
roll it around on the ground.
-
And these are not behaviors that have been
part of the original learning sequence.
-
But, um, as the Brelands paid more
attention to this and then
-
they paid more attention to pigs
and what pigs do.
-
If you've spent any time with pigs, um,
what you may know is that pigs
-
tend to root their food, then if they are
presented with food
-
they tend to roll it around of the ground
they may throw it up in the air
-
but this is a response patter that is
innate and part of what
-
pigs typically do when they're engaging
with food.
-
Now the coin wasn't food but they were
given a food reinforcer
-
at the end of completing the sequence
and what they started to do
-
was engage in that food sequence with
the coin.
-
So again they refer to this as that idea
of instinctive drift.
-
The pig was reverting back to its
biologically based response pattern.
-
It was acting upon the coin
-
as it would act upon the food
that it was going to receive.
-
So even though they could train the pig to
engage in the right kinds of responses
-
eventually instinctive drift
would kick back in.
-
So now here in this other picture
we've got the raccoon
-
and i'll have to explain what's going on
here with this particular picture
-
they've trained a raccoon to pick up a
ball and deposit it into
-
a little basketball net, right,
pretty cool.
-
What they had actually tried to do earlier
-
and they had to shift to this particular
behavior was they tried to get a raccoon
-
to do kind of a similar thing as the pig.
The raccoon had been trained to,
-
with shaping and food reinforcers, to pick
up a coin and put the coin into a bank
-
and they could get t-the raccoon
to do this with shaping but eventually
-
the raccoons started to engage in some odd
behaviors too.
-
So the raccoon would pick up the coin
and then it would kind of do this little
-
up and down motion with the coin
and that wasn't part of the, um,
-
original chained sequence of behaviors,
-
and then they gave the
raccoon another coin
-
and the raccoon would take the two coins
and kind of rub them together
-
in this miserly fashion and then take the
coins and then kind of put them
-
up and down into the, the piggy bank.
And again these were not behaviors that
-
had been reinforced, they were not hadn't
been shaped
-
and the raccoon was able to do the correct
responses but then across time
-
started engaging in these other kinds
of odd behaviors.
-
Now again, stepping back and paying
attention to raccoons, um,
-
if you've watching raccoons, if you know
anything about raccoons,
-
raccoons wash their food and so what the
Brelands noticed was happening was that
-
the raccoon was starting to act upon
the coins the same way
-
it would act upon its food.
-
So it would often times the raccoons will
take their food,
-
they'll dip it into a stream,
-
they'll kind of rub it around and, um,
the behaviors that they saw
-
the raccoon engaging with the coin were
really similar to the kinds of behaviors
-
that a raccoon would engage in it th-they
the raccoon was washing its food.
-
So, um, even though they hadn't trained
the raccoon to do this,
-
the raccoon kind of reverted back to those
biologically based response patterns.
-
So, then whats up with the deal with the
picture with the basketball?
-
So what the Brelands had to do was stop
using, uh, stop using stimuli
-
that were small with raccoons, so they
had to shift gears
-
and try to train the raccoon to use the
basketball because the basketball
-
was too big to be used for the same way
that they would normally, uh,
-
the same kinds of responses
that they would use with the food.
-
So they could get the raccoon to pick up
a basketball and put it into the hoop
-
because it was too different from the
kinds of, um, stimuli that raccoon
-
would normally use as far as eating.
-
So they could get the raccoon to engage
in the basketball,
-
picking up the basketball
-
and putting it into the hoop and the
instinctive drift didn't happen
-
with that particular stimulus because
-
it was too different to
what they were used to eating.
-
But again it kind of gets
back to this idea
-
of the misbehavior of organisms.
-
So even though operant conditioning can
be used, eventually we revert back
-
to these biologically based
response patterns.
-
So, thinking about this, we are all born
with these biological constraints,
-
these in born predispositions.
-
Certain kinds of learning are going to
be highly probably and easy
-
especially if they fit with what our, um,
response pattern is, our
-
inherited response pattern.
If we're trying to learn something
-
that is really different than what our
species has had to do in the past
-
it's going to be difficult and essentially
, you know, the more experiences
-
that we have, um, you know we-we
probably can learn it
-
based on shaping and principles of operant
conditioning but to some extent under-
-
under periods of stress we might start to
revert back to some of these
-
in born predispositions.
-
So you might be thinking, "Well, that's
that makes sense for raccoons,
-
that makes sense for pigs, what about
for humans?
-
Do we have any of the biological
predispositions
-
and you're certainly not going to see
much research on this
-
because humans don't like to think
of themselves as having, um,
-
instincts and these inborn predispositions
but let's talk about some possibilities.
-
Well, with humans, you know, you could-
you could argue the to some extent
-
the way that we respond to fearful stimuli
might have, um,
-
a biological predisposition too.
We talk about how some of our fears
-
make a little bit more sense, um, fears
of snakes, fears of-of, um,
-
animals that can harm us or insects that
could harm us, fears of heights, um,
-
we can say that we've got a biological
predisposition
-
to fear these kinds of things
-
and so even though, um, we may be able to
learn to work with snakes,
-
we may be able to learn to work at heights
if we have to work with heights, um,
-
these biological predispositions
were still there
-
and it might make it more challenging
to learn to work
-
under those kinds of circumstances
because we're fighting against
-
part of our biologically
based response pattern.
-
Recently a lot of people have talked about
-
how we might have a
biological predisposition
-
making it difficult for us to learn
technology, now that doesn't mean
-
that we can't learn technology but if you
think about it within the history
-
of our species working with computers, um,
hasn't been something
-
that we have had to in the past,
which is going to make it more challenging
-
for us to want to do it now.
Now that doesn't mean, again,
-
that we can't do it but because technology
hasn't been part
-
of our evolutionary history it might make
it more challenging for individuals
-
to learn technology, again, doesn't mean
you can't but it might make it
-
more difficult.
-
Uh, the last example I want to throw out
here
-
is having to do with eating behaviors.
-
So, you know, as a species we've had
to eat to stay alive, right?
-
If we don't eat, we don't stay alive.
-
And if we don't stay alive then we're not
going to be able to reproduce
-
and continue the species and so eating
serves a very strong biological function.
-
Now as humans, we also have
learned certain manners
-
that are associated with learning
-
and when we eat in a group setting,
in some kind of a public setting,
-
we probably have to adhere to following
those manners a little bit more
-
than we do when we're eating privately
but I want you to imagine
-
that you are super hungry and you are
in a position of having to eat
-
with others in a little bit more of a
formal setting.
-
So you're super hungry, and there's
food on the tables,
-
and there's everything in your mind
that's pushing you to grab that-
-
you know, to just kind of reach with
your hands and pick up
-
and start eating that food but you're in
a public setting and you're with people
-
and it's a little bit more formal and so,
you know, to some extent
-
you may have certain manners that you
have to follow.
-
Certain things that you have to follow
and at least in my household
-
one of the rules was you didn't
get to start eating
-
until everybody sat down to the table
-
and we also had to say some kind of prayer
or grace before every body
-
can jump in to eating and what
I'm saying is you still can maintain
-
those kinds of more formal rules but to
some extent you're fighting agianst
-
your biological predispositions which
would be grabbing that food
-
and just kind of picking
it up with your hands
-
and going to town eating, right.
-
So we have these biological, biologically
inborn predispositions to engage in
-
these kinds of responses, it doesn't mean
that we can't do the right thing
-
with respect to manners but we're fighting
against those biological predispositions,
-
especially if we're super hungry, right?
Alright.
-
So let's do a quick review.
-
Taste aversion conditioning differs from
other types of classical conditioning
-
in that associations are formed despite
very long delays between the BLANK
-
and BLANK.
-
Number 2, according to the BLANK effect,
any food that we associate with recovery
-
from an illness can lead us to feel better
at other times.
-
Last one, Sami tried to shape her squirrel
to fetch marbles in the back yard.
-
Things proceeded quite smoothly at first
and then the squirrel started burrying
-
the marbles rather than bringing them
to her.
-
This seems to be an example of-
-
Okay taste aversion conditioning differs
from other types of classical conditioning
-
in that associations are formed despite
very long delays between
-
the conditioned stimulus, the taste of the
food, and the unconditioned stimulus,
-
of being sick, right?
We talked about most of the time
-
in classical conditioning, the
conditioned stimulus
-
and the unconditioned stimulus
-
are presented within
seconds of each other.
-
But with taste aversions and taste
conditioning they can occur
-
in just a single trial and often times we
eat the food but then we're not sick
-
until 30 minutes, several hours later;
and that's different.
-
Okay, the medicine effect is when any food
we associate with recovery from an illness
-
can lead to us feeling
better at other times.
-
And what's going on with Sami
and her squirrel, well this is likely
-
an example of instinctive drift and it
makes sense, right?
-
Squirrels are not in the habit of, uh,
burying marbles but they are
-
in the habit of burying their food and,
um, if Sami is especially using
-
food reinforcers to shape her squirrel
then they're starting to act upon
-
those marbles like they would
with their food.
-
That's an example of instinctive drift.
Okay, I hope you did well.
-
We'll take a break and we'll come back
and talk about adaptive behaviors.