< Return to Video

The psychology of inequality and political division

  • 0:01 - 0:03
    You've probably heard by now
  • 0:03 - 0:06
    that economic inequality
    is historically high,
  • 0:06 - 0:09
    that the wealthiest one-tenth
    of one percent in the United States
  • 0:09 - 0:12
    have as much wealth
    as the bottom 90 percent combined,
  • 0:13 - 0:16
    or that the wealthiest
    eight individuals in the world
  • 0:16 - 0:17
    have as much wealth
  • 0:17 - 0:21
    as the poorest 3.5 billion
    inhabitants of the planet.
  • 0:22 - 0:28
    But did you know that economic inequality
    is associated with shorter lifespans,
  • 0:28 - 0:29
    less happiness,
  • 0:29 - 0:30
    more crime
  • 0:30 - 0:32
    and more drug abuse?
  • 0:33 - 0:35
    Those sound like problems of poverty,
  • 0:35 - 0:38
    but among wealthy, developed nations
  • 0:38 - 0:40
    those health and social problems
  • 0:40 - 0:44
    are actually more tightly linked
    to inequality between incomes
  • 0:44 - 0:46
    than to absolute incomes.
  • 0:47 - 0:48
    And because of that,
  • 0:48 - 0:50
    the United States,
  • 0:50 - 0:53
    the wealthiest and the most
    unequal of nations,
  • 0:53 - 0:57
    actually fares worse
    than all other developed countries.
  • 0:59 - 1:02
    Surveys show that
    large majorities of Americans,
  • 1:02 - 1:04
    both Democrats and Republicans,
  • 1:04 - 1:08
    believe inequality is too high
    and want more equal pay.
  • 1:08 - 1:14
    And yet as a society, we don't seem
    to be able to find the common ground,
  • 1:14 - 1:17
    the consensus, the political will
    to do anything about it.
  • 1:18 - 1:22
    Because, as inequality
    has risen in recent decades,
  • 1:22 - 1:25
    political polarization
    has risen along with it.
  • 1:27 - 1:31
    We see those who disagree with us
    as idiots or as immoral.
  • 1:32 - 1:34
    Nearly half of Democrats and Republicans
  • 1:34 - 1:38
    now think that the other side
    is not just mistaken
  • 1:38 - 1:39
    but a threat to the nation.
  • 1:40 - 1:44
    And that animosity prevents us
    from finding the common ground
  • 1:44 - 1:45
    to change things.
  • 1:47 - 1:51
    I'm a social psychology professor
    at the University of North Carolina,
  • 1:51 - 1:55
    and I study the effects of inequality
    on people's thinking and behavior.
  • 1:56 - 2:00
    I'm going to argue that it's not just
    an unfortunate coincidence
  • 2:00 - 2:04
    that inequality and political division
    have risen together.
  • 2:05 - 2:07
    There are good psychological reasons
  • 2:07 - 2:09
    that inequality drives wedges
    in our politics.
  • 2:10 - 2:13
    That means there are good
    psychological paths
  • 2:13 - 2:14
    to improve both at once.
  • 2:16 - 2:19
    To understand why inequality
    is so powerful,
  • 2:19 - 2:23
    you have to first understand
    that we are constantly comparing ourselves
  • 2:23 - 2:24
    to other people,
  • 2:24 - 2:26
    and when we do that,
  • 2:26 - 2:29
    we really like to come out on top,
  • 2:29 - 2:32
    and we find it painful
    to be on the bottom.
  • 2:32 - 2:35
    Psychologists call it
    the "better-than-average effect."
  • 2:36 - 2:39
    Most people believe
    they're better than average
  • 2:39 - 2:41
    at just about anything they care about,
  • 2:43 - 2:47
    which isn't strictly possible,
    because that's just what average means.
  • 2:47 - 2:50
    (Laughter)
  • 2:50 - 2:52
    But that's the way people feel.
  • 2:52 - 2:55
    Most people think
    they're smarter than average,
  • 2:55 - 2:57
    harder working than average
  • 2:57 - 2:59
    and more socially skilled.
  • 3:00 - 3:02
    Most people think they're
    better drivers than average.
  • 3:02 - 3:04
    (Laughter)
  • 3:04 - 3:07
    That's true even if you do the study
    with a sample of people
  • 3:07 - 3:10
    currently hospitalized
    for a car accident that they caused.
  • 3:10 - 3:12
    (Laughter)
  • 3:13 - 3:16
    So we really want to see ourselves
    as better than average,
  • 3:16 - 3:18
    and if we find out otherwise,
  • 3:18 - 3:21
    it's a painful experience
    that we have to cope with.
  • 3:21 - 3:25
    And we cope with it
    by shifting how we see the world.
  • 3:26 - 3:27
    To understand how this works,
  • 3:27 - 3:29
    my collaborators and I ran an experiment.
  • 3:31 - 3:35
    We asked participants to complete
    a decision-making task to earn some money,
  • 3:35 - 3:38
    and in reality, everyone earned
    the same amount of money.
  • 3:39 - 3:42
    But we randomly divided them
    into two groups,
  • 3:42 - 3:45
    and we told one group
    that they had done better than average,
  • 3:45 - 3:48
    and we told the other group
    they had done worse than average.
  • 3:48 - 3:53
    So now we have one group that feels richer
    and one group that feels poorer,
  • 3:53 - 3:55
    but for no objective reason.
  • 3:56 - 3:58
    And then we asked them some questions.
  • 3:58 - 4:02
    When we asked them,
    "How good are you at making decisions?"
  • 4:02 - 4:05
    the better-than-average group
    said that they were more competent
  • 4:05 - 4:07
    than the below-average group.
  • 4:08 - 4:11
    The better-than-average group
    said that their success
  • 4:11 - 4:14
    was a fair outcome of a meritocracy.
  • 4:15 - 4:18
    The below-average group
    thought the system was rigged,
  • 4:18 - 4:20
    and in this case,
    of course, they were right.
  • 4:20 - 4:23
    (Laughter)
  • 4:24 - 4:27
    Even though the two groups
    had the same amount of money,
  • 4:27 - 4:31
    the group that felt richer
    said we should cut taxes on the wealthy,
  • 4:31 - 4:33
    cut benefits to the poor.
  • 4:33 - 4:36
    Let them work hard and be
    responsible for themselves, they said.
  • 4:39 - 4:44
    These are attitudes that we normally
    assume are rooted in deeply held values
  • 4:44 - 4:47
    and a lifetime of experience,
  • 4:47 - 4:48
    but a 10-minute exercise
  • 4:48 - 4:51
    that made people feel richer or poorer
  • 4:51 - 4:53
    was enough to change those views.
  • 4:55 - 5:01
    This difference between being rich or poor
    and feeling rich or poor is important,
  • 5:01 - 5:03
    because the two don't always
    line up very well.
  • 5:04 - 5:06
    You often hear people say with nostalgia,
  • 5:06 - 5:09
    "We were poor, but we didn't know it."
  • 5:10 - 5:12
    That was the case for me growing up,
  • 5:12 - 5:13
    until one day,
  • 5:13 - 5:16
    in the fourth-grade lunch line,
  • 5:16 - 5:19
    we had a new cashier
    who didn't know the ropes,
  • 5:19 - 5:21
    and she asked me for $1.25.
  • 5:22 - 5:27
    I was taken aback because I had never
    been asked to pay for my lunch before.
  • 5:27 - 5:30
    I didn't know what to say
    because I didn't have any money.
  • 5:31 - 5:34
    And suddenly,
    I realized for the first time
  • 5:34 - 5:37
    that we free lunch kids
    were the poor ones.
  • 5:40 - 5:42
    That awkward moment
    in the school lunch line
  • 5:42 - 5:44
    changed so much for me,
  • 5:44 - 5:47
    because for the first time, I felt poor.
  • 5:48 - 5:51
    We didn't have any less money
    than the day before,
  • 5:51 - 5:53
    but for the first time,
  • 5:53 - 5:55
    I started noticing things differently.
  • 5:56 - 5:58
    It changed the way I saw the world.
  • 5:58 - 6:02
    I started noticing how the kids
    who paid for their lunch
  • 6:02 - 6:04
    seemed to dress better
    than the free lunch kids.
  • 6:05 - 6:08
    I started noticing the big yellow blocks
    of government cheese
  • 6:08 - 6:10
    that showed up at our door
  • 6:10 - 6:13
    and the food stamps my mother
    would pull out at the grocery store.
  • 6:14 - 6:15
    I was always a shy kid,
  • 6:15 - 6:18
    but I hardly talked at all
    after that at school.
  • 6:18 - 6:20
    Who was I to speak up?
  • 6:22 - 6:26
    For decades, social scientists
    looked for evidence
  • 6:26 - 6:29
    that feeling deprived
    compared to other people
  • 6:29 - 6:31
    would motivate political action.
  • 6:31 - 6:34
    They thought it would mobilize
    protests, strikes,
  • 6:34 - 6:36
    maybe even revolutions.
  • 6:36 - 6:40
    But again and again what they found
    was that it paralyzed people,
  • 6:42 - 6:43
    because the truth is,
  • 6:43 - 6:45
    feeling less than other people
  • 6:45 - 6:46
    brings shame.
  • 6:46 - 6:49
    It makes people turn away,
  • 6:49 - 6:50
    disgusted with the system.
  • 6:52 - 6:54
    Feeling better than other
    people, though --
  • 6:54 - 6:55
    now that is motivating.
  • 6:56 - 6:59
    It motivates us to protect that position,
  • 6:59 - 7:02
    and it has important consequences
    for our politics.
  • 7:03 - 7:06
    To see why, consider another experiment.
  • 7:07 - 7:11
    Again, we asked participants
    to make decisions to earn some money,
  • 7:12 - 7:14
    and we told one group
    that they had done better than average
  • 7:14 - 7:17
    and the other group that they
    had done worse than average.
  • 7:17 - 7:22
    And again, the better-than-average group
    said it's a fair meritocracy,
  • 7:22 - 7:23
    cut taxes on the wealthy,
  • 7:23 - 7:25
    cut benefits on the poor.
  • 7:28 - 7:30
    But this time, we also asked them
    what did they think
  • 7:30 - 7:33
    about other participants
    who disagree with them
  • 7:33 - 7:35
    on those issues.
  • 7:35 - 7:37
    Are they smart or incompetent?
  • 7:37 - 7:40
    Are they reasonable or are they biased?
  • 7:42 - 7:46
    The better-than-average group
    said anybody who disagrees with them
  • 7:46 - 7:49
    must be incompetent, biased,
  • 7:49 - 7:51
    blinded by self-interest.
  • 7:53 - 7:54
    The below-average group
  • 7:54 - 7:56
    didn't assume that about their opponents.
  • 7:58 - 8:00
    Now, there are lots of psychology studies
  • 8:00 - 8:03
    showing that when people agree with us,
  • 8:03 - 8:04
    we think they're brilliant,
  • 8:04 - 8:06
    and when people disagree with us,
  • 8:06 - 8:07
    we tend to think they're idiots.
  • 8:07 - 8:09
    (Laughter)
  • 8:10 - 8:13
    But this is new because we found
    it was driven entirely by the group
  • 8:13 - 8:15
    that felt better than average,
  • 8:15 - 8:19
    who felt entitled to dismiss
    those people who disagree with them.
  • 8:22 - 8:25
    So think about what
    this is doing to our politics,
  • 8:25 - 8:30
    as the haves and have-nots
    spread further and further apart.
  • 8:32 - 8:36
    Yes, a lot of us think
    that people on the other side are idiots,
  • 8:37 - 8:42
    but the people politically engaged enough
    to be yelling at each other about politics
  • 8:42 - 8:44
    are actually mostly the well-off.
  • 8:45 - 8:50
    In fact, as inequality has grown
    in recent decades,
  • 8:50 - 8:54
    political interest and participation
    among the poor has plummeted.
  • 8:55 - 8:58
    Again, we see that people
    who feel left behind
  • 8:58 - 9:03
    aren't taking to the streets to protest
    or organize voter registration drives.
  • 9:03 - 9:05
    Often, they aren't even voting.
  • 9:05 - 9:08
    Instead, they're turning away
    and dropping out.
  • 9:10 - 9:14
    So if we want to do something
    about extreme inequality,
  • 9:14 - 9:15
    we have to fix our politics.
  • 9:16 - 9:18
    And if we want to fix our politics,
  • 9:18 - 9:20
    we have to do something about inequality.
  • 9:20 - 9:21
    So what do we do?
  • 9:23 - 9:26
    The wonderful thing about spirals
  • 9:26 - 9:29
    is that you can interrupt
    at any point in the cycle.
  • 9:31 - 9:34
    I think our best bet starts
    with those of us
  • 9:34 - 9:38
    who have benefited the most
    from inequality's rise,
  • 9:38 - 9:40
    those of us who have done
    better than average.
  • 9:42 - 9:43
    If you've been successful,
  • 9:43 - 9:47
    it's natural to chalk up your success
    to your own hard work.
  • 9:48 - 9:49
    But, like the studies I showed you,
  • 9:49 - 9:51
    everybody does that,
  • 9:51 - 9:55
    whether or not it really was
    the hard work that mattered most.
  • 9:59 - 10:01
    Every successful person I know
  • 10:01 - 10:05
    can think of times when they worked hard
    and struggled to succeed.
  • 10:06 - 10:08
    They can also think of times
  • 10:08 - 10:12
    when they benefited from good luck
    or a helping hand,
  • 10:12 - 10:13
    but that part is harder.
  • 10:14 - 10:16
    Psychologists Shai Davidai
    and Tom Gilovich
  • 10:16 - 10:20
    call it the "headwind-tailwind asymmetry."
  • 10:20 - 10:23
    When you're struggling against headwinds,
  • 10:23 - 10:25
    those obstacles are all you can see.
  • 10:25 - 10:28
    It's what you notice and remember.
  • 10:28 - 10:32
    But when the wind's at your back
    and everything's going your way,
  • 10:32 - 10:34
    all you notice is yourself
  • 10:34 - 10:36
    and our own amazing talents.
  • 10:36 - 10:40
    So we have to stop and think for a minute
  • 10:40 - 10:43
    to recognize those tailwinds
    helping us along.
  • 10:45 - 10:47
    It's so easy to see
    what's wrong with people
  • 10:47 - 10:49
    who disagree with you.
  • 10:49 - 10:52
    Some of you decided that I was an idiot
    in the first two minutes
  • 10:52 - 10:54
    because I said inequality was harmful.
  • 10:54 - 10:55
    (Laughter)
  • 10:56 - 10:59
    The hard part is to recognize
  • 10:59 - 11:01
    that if you were in a different position,
  • 11:01 - 11:03
    you might see things differently,
  • 11:03 - 11:05
    just like the subjects in our experiments.
  • 11:07 - 11:10
    So if you're in the above-average
    group in life --
  • 11:10 - 11:13
    and if you're watching a TED talk,
    you most likely are --
  • 11:13 - 11:14
    (Laughter)
  • 11:14 - 11:16
    then I leave you with this challenge:
  • 11:18 - 11:21
    the next time you're tempted to dismiss
    someone who disagrees with you
  • 11:21 - 11:23
    as an idiot,
  • 11:23 - 11:26
    think about the tailwinds
    that helped you get where you are.
  • 11:27 - 11:29
    What lucky breaks did you get
  • 11:29 - 11:31
    that might have turned out differently?
  • 11:31 - 11:34
    What helping hands are you grateful for?
  • 11:35 - 11:38
    Recognizing those tailwinds
    gives us the humility we need
  • 11:38 - 11:42
    to see that disagreeing with us
    doesn't make people idiots.
  • 11:42 - 11:45
    The real hard work
    is in finding common ground,
  • 11:45 - 11:48
    because it's the well-off
    who have the power
  • 11:48 - 11:51
    and the responsibility to change things.
  • 11:51 - 11:52
    Thank you.
  • 11:52 - 11:55
    (Applause)
Title:
The psychology of inequality and political division
Speaker:
Keith Payne
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
12:09

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions