Return to Video

The psychology of inequality and political division

  • 0:01 - 0:03
    You've probably heard by now
  • 0:03 - 0:06
    that economic inequality
    is historically high,
  • 0:06 - 0:09
    that the wealthiest one tenth
    of one percent in the United States
  • 0:09 - 0:12
    have as much wealth
    as the bottom 90 percent combined,
  • 0:12 - 0:16
    or that the wealthiest
    eight individuals in the world
  • 0:16 - 0:17
    have as much wealth
  • 0:17 - 0:22
    as the poorest 3.5 billion
    inhabitants of the planet.
  • 0:22 - 0:28
    Did you know that economic inequality
    is associated with shorter lifespans,
  • 0:28 - 0:30
    less happiness, more crime
  • 0:30 - 0:33
    and more drug abuse?
  • 0:33 - 0:36
    Those sound like problems of poverty,
  • 0:36 - 0:38
    but among wealthy, developed nations
  • 0:38 - 0:40
    those health and social problems
  • 0:40 - 0:44
    are actually more tightly linked
    to inequality between incomes
  • 0:44 - 0:47
    than to absolute incomes.
  • 0:47 - 0:51
    And because of that,
  • 0:51 - 0:52
    the United States,
  • 0:52 - 0:54
    the wealthiest and
    the most unequal of nations,
  • 0:54 - 0:57
    actually fares worse
    than all other developed countries.
  • 0:57 - 1:02
    Surveys show that
    large majorities of Americans,
  • 1:02 - 1:04
    both Democrats and Republicans,
  • 1:04 - 1:08
    believe inequality is too high
    and want more equal pay,
  • 1:08 - 1:14
    and yet as a society we don't seem
    to be able to find the common ground,
  • 1:14 - 1:18
    the consensus, the political will
    to do anything about it.
  • 1:18 - 1:22
    Because, as inequality
    has risen in recent decades,
  • 1:22 - 1:27
    political polarization
    has risen along with it.
  • 1:27 - 1:32
    We see those who disagree with us
    as idiots or as immoral.
  • 1:32 - 1:36
    Nearly half of Democrats and Republicans
    now think that the other side
  • 1:36 - 1:39
    is not just mistaken,
    but a threat to the nation,
  • 1:40 - 1:41
    and that animosity
  • 1:41 - 1:45
    prevents us from finding
    the common ground to change things.
  • 1:46 - 1:51
    I'm a social psychology professor
    at the University of North Carolina,
  • 1:51 - 1:55
    and I study the effects of inequality
    on people's thinking and behavior.
  • 1:56 - 2:01
    I'm going to argue that it's not just
    an unfortunate coincidence
  • 2:01 - 2:04
    that inequality and political division
    have risen together.
  • 2:05 - 2:07
    There are good psychological reasons
  • 2:07 - 2:10
    that inequality drives wedges
    in our politics.
  • 2:11 - 2:13
    That means there are good
    psychological paths
  • 2:13 - 2:16
    to improve both at once.
  • 2:17 - 2:19
    To understand why inequality
    is so powerful,
  • 2:19 - 2:23
    you have to first understand
    that we are constantly comparing ourselves
  • 2:23 - 2:25
    to other people,
  • 2:25 - 2:27
    and when we do that,
  • 2:27 - 2:30
    we really like to come out on top,
  • 2:30 - 2:32
    and we find it painful
    to be on the bottom.
  • 2:33 - 2:36
    Psychologists call it
    the "better than average effect."
  • 2:36 - 2:38
    Most people believe
    they're better than average
  • 2:38 - 2:41
    at just about anything they care about,
  • 2:41 - 2:45
    which isn't strictly possible,
    because that's just what average means.
  • 2:45 - 2:50
    (Laughter)
  • 2:50 - 2:52
    But that's the way people feel.
  • 2:52 - 2:55
    Most people think
    they're smarter than average,
  • 2:55 - 2:56
    harder working than average,
  • 2:56 - 2:59
    and more socially skilled.
  • 2:59 - 3:02
    Most people think they're
    better drivers than average.
  • 3:02 - 3:04
    (Laughter)
  • 3:04 - 3:07
    That's true even if you do
    the study with a sample of people
  • 3:07 - 3:10
    currently hospitalized
    for a car accident that they caused.
  • 3:10 - 3:13
    (Laughter)
  • 3:14 - 3:16
    So we really want to see ourselves
    as better than average,
  • 3:16 - 3:18
    and if we find out otherwise
  • 3:18 - 3:21
    it's a painful experience
    that we have to cope with.
  • 3:21 - 3:23
    And we cope with it
  • 3:23 - 3:25
    by shifting how we see the world.
  • 3:25 - 3:27
    To understand how this works,
  • 3:27 - 3:30
    my collaborators and I ran an experiment.
  • 3:30 - 3:35
    We asked participants to complete
    a decision-making task to earn some money,
  • 3:35 - 3:38
    and in reality everyone earned
    the same amount of money.
  • 3:38 - 3:42
    But we randomly divided them
    into two groups,
  • 3:42 - 3:45
    and we told one group
    that they had done better than average,
  • 3:45 - 3:48
    and we told the other group
    they had done worse than average.
  • 3:48 - 3:53
    So now we have one group that feels richer
    and one group that feels poorer,
  • 3:53 - 3:56
    but for no objective reason.
  • 3:56 - 3:58
    And then we asked them some questions.
  • 3:58 - 4:00
    When we asked them,
  • 4:00 - 4:04
    "How good are you at making decisions?"
  • 4:04 - 4:05
    the better than average group
    said that they were more competent
  • 4:05 - 4:09
    than the below average group.
  • 4:09 - 4:12
    The better than average group
    said that their success
  • 4:12 - 4:14
    was a fair outcome of a meritocracy.
  • 4:14 - 4:18
    The below average group
    thought the system was rigged,
  • 4:18 - 4:21
    and in this case,
    of course, they were right.
  • 4:21 - 4:24
    (Laughter)
  • 4:24 - 4:27
    Even though the two groups
    had the same amount of money,
  • 4:27 - 4:29
    the group that felt richer
  • 4:29 - 4:32
    said we should cut taxes on the wealthy,
  • 4:32 - 4:34
    cut benefits to the poor.
  • 4:34 - 4:37
    Let them work hard and be
    responsible for themselves, they said.
  • 4:37 - 4:44
    These are attitudes that we normally
    assume are rooted in deeply held values
  • 4:44 - 4:46
    and a lifetime of experience,
  • 4:46 - 4:49
    but a 10-minute exercise
  • 4:49 - 4:51
    that made people feel richer or poorer
  • 4:51 - 4:54
    was enough to change those views.
  • 4:55 - 4:58
    This difference between being rich or poor
  • 4:58 - 5:00
    and feeling rich or poor is important,
  • 5:00 - 5:04
    because the two don't always
    line up very well.
  • 5:04 - 5:07
    You often hear people say with nostalgia,
  • 5:07 - 5:10
    "We were poor but we didn't know it."
  • 5:10 - 5:12
    That was the case for me growing up,
  • 5:12 - 5:14
    until one day,
  • 5:14 - 5:16
    in the fourth grade lunch line,
  • 5:16 - 5:20
    we had a new cashier
    who didn't know the ropes
  • 5:20 - 5:22
    and she asked me for $1.25.
  • 5:22 - 5:27
    I was taken aback because I had never
    been asked to pay for my lunch before.
  • 5:28 - 5:29
    I didn't know what to say
    because I didn't have any money.
  • 5:29 - 5:32
    And suddenly I realized for the first time
  • 5:32 - 5:38
    that we free lunch kids
    were the poorer ones.
  • 5:39 - 5:43
    That awkward moment
    in the school lunch line
  • 5:43 - 5:45
    changed so much for me,
  • 5:45 - 5:48
    because for the first time I felt poor.
  • 5:49 - 5:52
    We didn't have any less money
    than the day before,
  • 5:52 - 5:53
    but for the first time,
  • 5:53 - 5:56
    I started noticing things differently.
  • 5:56 - 5:59
    It changed the way I saw the world.
  • 5:59 - 6:02
    I started noticing how the kids
    who paid for their lunch
  • 6:02 - 6:05
    seemed to dress better
    than the free lunch kids.
  • 6:05 - 6:08
    I started noticing the big yellow blocks
    of government cheese
  • 6:08 - 6:10
    that showed up at our door,
  • 6:10 - 6:13
    and the food stamps my mother
    would pull out at the grocery store.
  • 6:14 - 6:16
    I was always a shy kid,
  • 6:16 - 6:19
    but I hardly talked at all
    after that at school.
  • 6:19 - 6:21
    Who was I to speak up?
  • 6:23 - 6:26
    For decades, social scientists
    looked for evidence
  • 6:26 - 6:29
    that feeling deprived
    compared to other people
  • 6:29 - 6:31
    would motivate political action.
  • 6:31 - 6:33
    They thought it would mobilize protests,
  • 6:33 - 6:34
    strikes,
  • 6:34 - 6:36
    maybe even revolutions,
  • 6:36 - 6:39
    but again and again what they found
  • 6:39 - 6:41
    was that it paralyzed people,
  • 6:42 - 6:43
    because the truth is,
  • 6:43 - 6:47
    feeling less than other people
  • 6:47 - 6:48
    brings shame.
  • 6:48 - 6:49
    It makes people turn away,
  • 6:49 - 6:51
    disgusted with the system.
  • 6:51 - 6:55
    Feeling better than other people, though,
    now that is motivating.
  • 6:56 - 6:59
    It motivates us to protect that position,
  • 6:59 - 7:02
    and it has important consequences
    for our politics.
  • 7:03 - 7:07
    To see why, consider another experiment.
  • 7:07 - 7:09
    Again, we asked participants
  • 7:09 - 7:12
    to make decisions to earn some money,
  • 7:12 - 7:15
    and we told one group
    that they had done better than average
  • 7:15 - 7:17
    and the other group that they
    had done worse than average.
  • 7:17 - 7:19
    And again, the better than average group
  • 7:19 - 7:22
    said it's a fair meritocracy,
    cut taxes on the wealthy,
  • 7:22 - 7:26
    cut benefits for the poor.
  • 7:28 - 7:31
    But this time, we also asked them
    what did they think
  • 7:31 - 7:34
    about other participants
    who disagree with them
  • 7:34 - 7:36
    on those issues.
  • 7:36 - 7:38
    Are they smart or incompetent?
  • 7:38 - 7:41
    Are they reasonable or are they biased?
  • 7:41 - 7:46
    The better than average group
    said anybody who disagrees with them
  • 7:46 - 7:49
    must be incompetent, biased,
  • 7:49 - 7:52
    blinded by self-interest.
  • 7:53 - 7:54
    The below average group
  • 7:54 - 7:57
    didn't assume that about their opponents.
  • 7:58 - 8:01
    Now, there are lots of psychology studies
  • 8:01 - 8:03
    showing that when people agree with us,
  • 8:03 - 8:05
    we think they're brilliant,
  • 8:05 - 8:06
    and when people disagree with us,
  • 8:06 - 8:08
    we tend to think they're idiots.
  • 8:08 - 8:10
    (Laughter)
  • 8:10 - 8:14
    But this is new because we found
    it was driven entirely by the group
  • 8:14 - 8:15
    that felt better than average,
  • 8:15 - 8:20
    that felt entitled to dismiss
    those people who disagree with them.
  • 8:22 - 8:26
    So think about what this
    is doing to our politics,
  • 8:26 - 8:30
    as the haves and have-nots
    spread further and further apart.
  • 8:32 - 8:37
    Yes, a lot of us think
    that people on the other side are idiots,
  • 8:37 - 8:42
    but the people politically engaged enough
    to be yelling at each other about politics
  • 8:42 - 8:45
    are actually mostly the well off.
  • 8:46 - 8:50
    In fact, as inequality has grown
    in recent decades,
  • 8:50 - 8:53
    political interest and participation
    among the poor has plummeted.
  • 8:53 - 8:58
    Again, we see that people
    who feel left behind
  • 8:58 - 9:03
    aren't taking to the streets to protest
    or organize voter registration drives.
  • 9:03 - 9:06
    Often, they aren't even voting.
  • 9:06 - 9:08
    Instead, they're turning away
    and dropping out.
  • 9:11 - 9:14
    So if we want to do something
    about extreme inequality,
  • 9:14 - 9:16
    we have to fix our politics,
  • 9:16 - 9:19
    and if we want to fix our politics,
  • 9:19 - 9:21
    we have to do something about inequality.
  • 9:21 - 9:23
    So what do we do?
  • 9:23 - 9:25
    The wonderful thing about spirals
  • 9:25 - 9:29
    is that you can interrupt
    at any point in the cycle.
  • 9:29 - 9:34
    I think our best bet starts
    with those of us
  • 9:34 - 9:39
    who have benefited the most
    from inequality's rise,
  • 9:39 - 9:42
    those of us who have done
    better than average.
  • 9:42 - 9:46
    If you've been successful, it's natural
    to chalk up your success
  • 9:46 - 9:48
    to your own hard work,
  • 9:48 - 9:51
    but, like the studies I showed you,
  • 9:51 - 9:52
    everybody does that,
  • 9:52 - 9:55
    whether or not it really was
    the hard work that mattered most.
  • 9:56 - 10:01
    Every successful person I know
  • 10:01 - 10:06
    can think of times when they worked hard
    and struggled to succeed.
  • 10:06 - 10:10
    They can also think of times
  • 10:10 - 10:12
    when they benefited from good luck
    or a helping hand,
  • 10:12 - 10:14
    but that part is harder.
  • 10:14 - 10:17
    Psychologist ?? and Tom Gilovich
  • 10:17 - 10:20
    call it the headwind-tailwind assymetry.
  • 10:21 - 10:23
    When you're struggling against headwinds,
  • 10:23 - 10:26
    those obstacles are all you can see.
  • 10:26 - 10:28
    It's what you notice and remember.
  • 10:28 - 10:32
    But when the wind's at your back
    and everything's going your way,
  • 10:32 - 10:34
    all you notice is yourself
  • 10:34 - 10:36
    and our own amazing talents.
  • 10:37 - 10:40
    So we have to stop and think for a minute
  • 10:40 - 10:43
    to recognize those tailwinds
    helping us along.
  • 10:45 - 10:49
    It's so easy to see what's wrong
    with people who disagree with you.
  • 10:49 - 10:53
    Some of you decided that I
    was an idiot in the first two minutes
  • 10:53 - 10:55
    because I said inequality was harmful.
  • 10:55 - 10:56
    (Laughter)
  • 10:56 - 10:59
    The hard part is to recognize
  • 10:59 - 11:01
    that if you were in a different position,
  • 11:01 - 11:05
    you might see things differently,
  • 11:05 - 11:07
    just like the subjects in our experiments.
  • 11:07 - 11:10
    So if you're in the
    above average group in life --
  • 11:10 - 11:13
    and if you're watching a TED Talk,
    you most likely are --
  • 11:14 - 11:17
    then I leave you with this challenge.
  • 11:18 - 11:21
    The next time you're tempted
    to dismiss someone
  • 11:21 - 11:23
    who disagrees with you as an idiot,
  • 11:23 - 11:25
    think about the tailwinds
  • 11:25 - 11:28
    that helped you get where you rae.
  • 11:28 - 11:29
    What lucky breaks did you get
  • 11:29 - 11:31
    that might have turned out differently?
  • 11:31 - 11:35
    What helping hands are you grateful for?
  • 11:35 - 11:38
    Recognizing those tailwinds
    gives us the humility we need
  • 11:38 - 11:41
    to see that disagreeing with us
    doesn't make people idiots.
  • 11:41 - 11:45
    The real hard work
    is in finding common ground,
  • 11:45 - 11:48
    because it's the well-off
    who have the power
  • 11:48 - 11:51
    and the responsibility to change things.
  • 11:51 - 11:53
    Thank you.
  • 11:53 - 11:55
    (Applause)
Title:
The psychology of inequality and political division
Speaker:
Keith Payne
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
12:09

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions