Your brain on improv
-
0:00 - 0:03So I am a surgeon who studies creativity,
-
0:03 - 0:06and I have never had a patient tell me
-
0:06 - 0:09that "I really want you to be creative during surgery,"
-
0:09 - 0:12and so I guess there's a little bit of irony to it.
-
0:12 - 0:15I will say though that, after having done surgery a lot,
-
0:15 - 0:17it's somewhat similar to playing a musical instrument.
-
0:17 - 0:20And for me, this sort of deep and enduring fascination with sound
-
0:20 - 0:22is what led me to both be a surgeon
-
0:22 - 0:24and also to study the science of sound, particularly music.
-
0:24 - 0:26And so I'm going to try to talk to you over the next few minutes
-
0:26 - 0:28about my career
-
0:28 - 0:30in terms of how I'm able to actually try to study music
-
0:30 - 0:32and really try to grapple with all these questions
-
0:32 - 0:35of how the brain is able to be creative.
-
0:35 - 0:37I've done most of this work at Johns Hopkins University,
-
0:37 - 0:39but also at the National Institute of Health where I was previously.
-
0:39 - 0:41I'm going to go over some science experiments
-
0:41 - 0:43and try to cover three musical experiments.
-
0:43 - 0:45I'm going to start off by playing a video for you.
-
0:45 - 0:48And this video is a video of Keith Jarrett, who's a well-known jazz improviser
-
0:48 - 0:51and probably the most well-known, iconic example
-
0:51 - 0:53of someone who takes improvisation to a really higher level.
-
0:53 - 0:55And he'll improvise entire concerts
-
0:55 - 0:57off the top of his head,
-
0:57 - 0:59and he'll never play it exactly the same way again,
-
0:59 - 1:01and so, as a form of intense creativity,
-
1:01 - 1:03I think this is a great example.
-
1:03 - 1:05And so why don't we go and click the video.
-
1:06 - 1:10(Music)
-
2:02 - 2:05It's really a remarkable, awesome thing that happens there.
-
2:05 - 2:07I've always -- just as a listener, as just a fan --
-
2:07 - 2:09I listen to that, and I'm just astounded.
-
2:09 - 2:11I think -- how can this possibly be?
-
2:11 - 2:13How can the brain generate that much information,
-
2:13 - 2:15that much music, spontaneously?
-
2:15 - 2:18And so I set out with this concept, scientifically,
-
2:18 - 2:21that artistic creativity, it's magical, but it's not magic,
-
2:21 - 2:23meaning that it's a product of the brain.
-
2:23 - 2:26There's not too many brain-dead people creating art.
-
2:26 - 2:28And so with this notion that artistic creativity
-
2:28 - 2:30is in fact a neurologic product,
-
2:30 - 2:33I took this thesis that we could study it
-
2:33 - 2:36just like we study any other complex neurologic process.
-
2:36 - 2:38And I think there's some sub-questions there that I put there.
-
2:38 - 2:40Is it truly possible to study creativity scientifically?
-
2:40 - 2:42And I think that's a good question.
-
2:42 - 2:45And I'll tell you that most scientific studies of music,
-
2:45 - 2:47they're very dense,
-
2:47 - 2:50and when you actually go through them, it's very hard to recognize the music in it.
-
2:50 - 2:52In fact, they seem to be very unmusical entirely
-
2:52 - 2:54and to miss the whole point of the music.
-
2:54 - 2:56And so it brings the second question:
-
2:56 - 2:58Why should scientists study creativity?
-
2:58 - 3:00Maybe we're not the right people to do it.
-
3:00 - 3:02Well it may be,
-
3:02 - 3:04but I will say that, from a scientific perspective --
-
3:04 - 3:06we talked a lot about innovation today --
-
3:06 - 3:08the science of innovation,
-
3:08 - 3:10how much we understand about how the brain is able to innovate
-
3:10 - 3:12is in its infancy,
-
3:12 - 3:15and truly, we know very little about how we are able to be creative.
-
3:15 - 3:17And so I think that we're going to see
-
3:17 - 3:19over the next 10, 20, 30 years
-
3:19 - 3:22a real science of creativity that's burgeoning and is going to flourish.
-
3:22 - 3:24Because we now have new methods that can enable us
-
3:24 - 3:26to take this process of something like this,
-
3:26 - 3:28complex jazz improvisation, and study it rigorously.
-
3:28 - 3:30And so it gets down to the brain.
-
3:30 - 3:32And so all of us have this remarkable brain,
-
3:32 - 3:35which is poorly understood to say the least.
-
3:35 - 3:37I think that neuroscientists
-
3:37 - 3:39have many more questions than answers,
-
3:39 - 3:41and I myself, I'm not going to give you many answers today,
-
3:41 - 3:43just ask a lot of questions.
-
3:43 - 3:45And fundamentally that's what I do in my lab.
-
3:45 - 3:47I ask questions about what is this brain doing to enable us to do this.
-
3:47 - 3:50This is the main method that I use. This is called functional MRI.
-
3:50 - 3:53If you've been in an MRI scanner, it's very much the same,
-
3:53 - 3:55but this one is outfitted in a special way
-
3:55 - 3:57to not just take pictures of your brain,
-
3:57 - 4:00but to also take pictures of active areas of the brain.
-
4:00 - 4:02Now the way that's done is by the following.
-
4:02 - 4:04There's something called BOLD imaging,
-
4:04 - 4:06which is Blood Oxygen Level Dependent imaging.
-
4:06 - 4:08Now when you're in an fMRI scanner,
-
4:08 - 4:10you're in a big magnet
-
4:10 - 4:12that's aligning your molecules in certain areas.
-
4:12 - 4:15When an area of the brain is active, meaning a neural area is active,
-
4:15 - 4:18it gets blood flow shunted to that area.
-
4:18 - 4:20That blood flow causes an increase
-
4:20 - 4:22in local blood to that area
-
4:22 - 4:25with a deoxyhemoglobin change in concentration.
-
4:25 - 4:27Deoxyhemoglobin can be detected by MRI,
-
4:27 - 4:29whereas oxyhemoglobin can't.
-
4:29 - 4:31So through this method of inference --
-
4:31 - 4:33and we're measuring blood flow, not neural activity --
-
4:33 - 4:35we say that an area of the brain that's getting more blood
-
4:35 - 4:37was active during a particular task,
-
4:37 - 4:39and that's the crux of how fMRI works.
-
4:39 - 4:41And it's been used since the '90s
-
4:41 - 4:44to study really complex processes.
-
4:44 - 4:46Now I'm going to review a study that I did,
-
4:46 - 4:48which was jazz in an fMRI scanner.
-
4:48 - 4:50And this was done with a colleague of mine, Alan Braun, at the NIH.
-
4:50 - 4:53This is a short video of how we did this project.
-
4:53 - 4:55(Video) Charles Limb: This is a plastic MIDI piano keyboard
-
4:55 - 4:57that we use for the jazz experiments.
-
4:57 - 4:59And it's a 35-key keyboard
-
4:59 - 5:01that is designed to fit both inside the scanner,
-
5:01 - 5:03be magnetically safe,
-
5:03 - 5:05have minimal interference
-
5:05 - 5:07that would contribute to any artifact
-
5:07 - 5:10and have this cushion so that it can rest on the players' legs
-
5:10 - 5:13while they're lying down in the scanner, playing on their back.
-
5:13 - 5:16And it works like this -- this doesn't actually produce any sound.
-
5:16 - 5:18It sends out what's called a MIDI signal --
-
5:18 - 5:20or a Musical Instrument Digital Interface --
-
5:20 - 5:23through these wires into the box and then the computer,
-
5:23 - 5:26which then trigger high-quality piano samples like this.
-
5:26 - 5:29(Music)
-
5:32 - 5:52(Music)
-
5:54 - 5:56CL: Okay, so it works.
-
5:56 - 5:58And so through this piano keyboard,
-
5:58 - 6:00we now have the means to take a musical process and study it.
-
6:00 - 6:03So what do you do now that you have this cool piano keyboard?
-
6:03 - 6:05You can't just sort of -- "It's great we've got this keyboard."
-
6:05 - 6:07We actually have to come up with a scientific experiment.
-
6:07 - 6:10And so the experiment really rests on the following:
-
6:11 - 6:14What happens in the brain during something that's memorized and over-learned,
-
6:14 - 6:16and what happens in the brain during something
-
6:16 - 6:18that is spontaneously generated, or improvised,
-
6:18 - 6:20in a way that's matched motorically
-
6:20 - 6:23and in terms of lower-level sensory motor features?
-
6:23 - 6:26And so, I have here what we call the "paradigms."
-
6:26 - 6:29There's a scale paradigm, which is just playing a scale up and down, memorized.
-
6:29 - 6:31And then there's improvising on a scale --
-
6:31 - 6:33quarter notes, metronome, right hand --
-
6:33 - 6:35scientifically very safe,
-
6:35 - 6:37but musically really boring.
-
6:37 - 6:39And then there's the bottom one, which is called the jazz paradigm.
-
6:39 - 6:41And so what we did was we brought professional jazz players to the NIH,
-
6:41 - 6:44and we had them memorize this piece of music on the left, the lower-left --
-
6:44 - 6:46which is what you heard me playing --
-
6:46 - 6:49and then we had them improvise to the same exact chord changes.
-
6:49 - 6:51And if you can hit that lower-right sound icon,
-
6:51 - 6:53that's an example of what was recorded in the scanner.
-
6:53 - 6:58(Music)
-
7:21 - 7:23So in the end, it's not the most natural environment,
-
7:23 - 7:25but they're able to play real music.
-
7:25 - 7:27And I've listened to that solo 200 times,
-
7:27 - 7:29and I still like it.
-
7:29 - 7:31And the musicians, they were comfortable in the end.
-
7:31 - 7:33And so we first measured the number of notes.
-
7:33 - 7:35Were they in fact just playing a lot more notes when they were improvising?
-
7:35 - 7:37That was not what was going on.
-
7:37 - 7:39And then we looked at the brain activity.
-
7:39 - 7:41I'm going to try to condense this for you.
-
7:41 - 7:44These are contrast maps that are showing subtractions
-
7:44 - 7:46between what changes when you're improvising
-
7:46 - 7:48versus when you're doing something memorized.
-
7:48 - 7:50In red is an area that active in the prefrontal cortex,
-
7:50 - 7:52the frontal lobe of the brain,
-
7:52 - 7:54and in blue is this area that was deactivated.
-
7:54 - 7:56And so we had this focal area called the medial prefrontal cortex
-
7:56 - 7:58that went way up in activity.
-
7:58 - 8:01We had this broad patch of area called the lateral prefrontal cortex
-
8:01 - 8:04that went way down in activity, and I'll summarize that for you here.
-
8:04 - 8:06Now these are multifunctional areas of the brain.
-
8:06 - 8:09As I like to say, these are not the "jazz areas" of the brain.
-
8:09 - 8:11They do a whole host of things
-
8:11 - 8:13that have to do with self-reflection,
-
8:13 - 8:15introspection, working memory and so forth.
-
8:15 - 8:18Really, consciousness is seated in the frontal lobe.
-
8:18 - 8:20But we have this combination
-
8:20 - 8:23of an area that's thought to be involved in self-monitoring, turning off,
-
8:23 - 8:25and this area that's thought to be autobiographical,
-
8:25 - 8:27or self-expressive, turning on.
-
8:27 - 8:29And we think, at least in this preliminary --
-
8:29 - 8:31it's one study; it's probably wrong,
-
8:31 - 8:33but it's one study --
-
8:33 - 8:36we think that at least a reasonable hypothesis
-
8:36 - 8:38is that, to be creative,
-
8:38 - 8:40you have to have this weird dissociation in your frontal lobe.
-
8:40 - 8:42One area turns on, and a big area shuts off,
-
8:42 - 8:45so that you're not inhibited, so that you're willing to make mistakes,
-
8:45 - 8:47so that you're not constantly shutting down
-
8:47 - 8:50all of these new generative impulses.
-
8:50 - 8:53Now a lot of people know that music is not always a solo activity --
-
8:53 - 8:55sometimes it's done communicatively.
-
8:55 - 8:57And so the next question was:
-
8:57 - 8:59What happens when musicians are trading back and forth,
-
8:59 - 9:01something called "trading fours,"
-
9:01 - 9:03which is something they do normally in a jazz experiment?
-
9:03 - 9:05So this is a twelve-bar blues.
-
9:05 - 9:07And I've broken it down into four-bar groups here,
-
9:07 - 9:09so you would know how you would trade.
-
9:09 - 9:11Now what we did was we brought a musician into the scanner -- same way --
-
9:11 - 9:13had them memorize this melody
-
9:13 - 9:15and then had another musician out in the control room
-
9:15 - 9:18trading back and forth interactively.
-
9:18 - 9:20So this is a musician, Mike Pope,
-
9:20 - 9:23one of the world's best bassists and a fantastic piano player.
-
9:28 - 9:30So he's now playing the piece
-
9:30 - 9:32that we just saw
-
9:32 - 9:34just a little better than I wrote it.
-
9:34 - 9:36(Video) CL: Mike, come on in. Mike Pope: May the force be with you.
-
9:36 - 9:38Nurse: Nothing's in your pockets, right Mike?
-
9:38 - 9:41MP: Nope. Nothing's in my pockets. Nurse: Okay.
-
9:50 - 9:52CL: You have to have the right attitude to agree to it.
-
9:52 - 9:54(Laughter)
-
9:54 - 9:56It's kind of fun actually.
-
9:56 - 9:59And so now we're playing back and forth.
-
9:59 - 10:02He's in there. You can see his legs up there.
-
10:03 - 10:06And then I'm in the control room here, playing back and forth.
-
10:06 - 10:09(Music)
-
10:18 - 10:21(Video) Mike Pope: This is a pretty good representation
-
10:21 - 10:23of what it's like.
-
10:23 - 10:25And it's good that it's not too quick.
-
10:25 - 10:27The fact that we do it over and over again
-
10:27 - 10:30lets you acclimate to your surroundings.
-
10:31 - 10:34So the hardest thing for me was the kinesthetic thing,
-
10:34 - 10:36of looking at my hands
-
10:36 - 10:38through two mirrors,
-
10:38 - 10:40laying on my back
-
10:40 - 10:42and not able to move at all except for my hand.
-
10:42 - 10:44That was challenging.
-
10:44 - 10:46But again,
-
10:46 - 10:49there were moments, for sure,
-
10:49 - 10:51there were moments
-
10:51 - 10:55of real, honest-to-God musical interplay, for sure.
-
10:55 - 10:57CL: At this point, I'll take a few moments.
-
10:57 - 10:59And so what you're seeing here --
-
10:59 - 11:01and I'm doing a cardinal sin in science,
-
11:01 - 11:03which is to show you preliminary data.
-
11:03 - 11:05This is one subject's data.
-
11:05 - 11:07This is, in fact, Mike Pope's data.
-
11:07 - 11:09So what am I showing you here?
-
11:09 - 11:12When he was trading fours with me, improvising versus memorized,
-
11:12 - 11:15his language areas lit up, his Broca's area,
-
11:15 - 11:17which is inferior frontal gyrus on the left.
-
11:17 - 11:19He actually had it also homologous on the right.
-
11:19 - 11:22This is an area thought to be involved in expressive communication.
-
11:22 - 11:24This whole notion that music is a language --
-
11:24 - 11:27well maybe there's a neurologic basis to it in fact after all,
-
11:27 - 11:30and we can see it when two musicians are having a musical conversation.
-
11:30 - 11:32And so we've done this actually on eight subjects now,
-
11:32 - 11:34and we're just getting all the data together,
-
11:34 - 11:36so hopefully we'll have something to say about it meaningfully.
-
11:36 - 11:39Now when I think about improvisation and the language, well what's next?
-
11:39 - 11:41Rap, of course, rap --
-
11:41 - 11:43free-style.
-
11:43 - 11:45And so I've always been fascinated by free-style.
-
11:45 - 11:47And let's go ahead and play this video here.
-
11:47 - 11:49(Video) Mos Def: ♫ ... brown skin I be, standing five-ten I be ♫
-
11:49 - 11:52♫ Rockin' it when I be, in your vicinity ♫
-
11:52 - 11:54♫ Whole-style synergy, recognize symmetry ♫
-
11:54 - 11:57♫ Go and try to injure me, broke 'em down chemically ♫
-
11:57 - 11:59♫ Ain't the number 10 MC, talk about how been I be ♫
-
11:59 - 12:02♫ Styled it like Kennedy, late like a 10 to three ♫
-
12:02 - 12:05♫ When I say when I be, girls say bend that key cut ♫
-
12:05 - 12:07CL: And so there's a lot of analogy
-
12:07 - 12:09between what takes place in free-style rap and jazz.
-
12:09 - 12:11There are, in fact, a lot of correlations between the two forms of music
-
12:11 - 12:13I think in different time periods.
-
12:13 - 12:15In a lot a ways, rap serves the same social function
-
12:15 - 12:17that jazz used to serve.
-
12:17 - 12:19So how do you study rap scientifically?
-
12:19 - 12:21And my colleagues kind of think I'm crazy,
-
12:21 - 12:23but I think it's very viable.
-
12:23 - 12:25And so this is what you do: you have a free-style artist
-
12:25 - 12:27come in and memorize a rap that you write for them,
-
12:27 - 12:29that they've never heard before,
-
12:29 - 12:31and then you have them free-style.
-
12:31 - 12:33So I told my lab members that I would rap for TED,
-
12:33 - 12:35and they said, "No, you won't."
-
12:35 - 12:37And then I thought --
-
12:37 - 12:43(Applause)
-
12:43 - 12:45But here's the thing.
-
12:45 - 12:48With this big screen, you can all rap with me. Okay?
-
12:48 - 12:50So what we had them do
-
12:50 - 12:52was memorize this lower-left sound icon, please.
-
12:52 - 12:55This is the control condition. This is what they memorized.
-
12:55 - 12:57Computer: ♫ Memory, thump. ♫
-
12:57 - 13:00CL: ♫ Thump of the beat in a known repeat ♫
-
13:00 - 13:03♫ Rhythm and rhyme, they make me complete ♫
-
13:03 - 13:05♫ The climb is sublime when I'm on the mic ♫
-
13:05 - 13:08♫ Spittin' rhymes that hit you like a lightning strike ♫
-
13:08 - 13:10♫ I search for the truth in this eternal quest ♫
-
13:10 - 13:13♫ My passion's not fashion, you can see how I'm dressed ♫
-
13:13 - 13:16♫ Psychopathic words in my head appear ♫
-
13:16 - 13:19♫ Whisper these lyrics only I can hear ♫
-
13:19 - 13:21♫ The art of discovering and that which is hovering ♫
-
13:21 - 13:24♫ Inside the mind of those unconfined ♫
-
13:24 - 13:27♫ All of these words keep pouring out like rain ♫
-
13:27 - 13:30♫ I need a mad scientist to check my brain ♫
-
13:30 - 13:39(Applause)
-
13:39 - 13:42I guarantee you that will never happen again.
-
13:42 - 13:44(Laughter)
-
13:44 - 13:46So now, what's great about these free-stylers,
-
13:46 - 13:48they will get cued different words.
-
13:48 - 13:50They don't know what's coming, but they'll hear something off the cuff.
-
13:50 - 13:52Go ahead and hit that right sound icon.
-
13:52 - 13:55They are going to be cued these three square words: "like," "not" and "head."
-
13:55 - 13:57He doesn't know what's coming.
-
13:57 - 13:59Free-styler: ♫ I'm like some kind of [unclear] ♫
-
13:59 - 14:02♫ [unclear] extraterrestrial, celestial scene ♫
-
14:02 - 14:05♫ Back in the days, I used to sit in pyramids and meditate ♫
-
14:05 - 14:08♫ With two microphones hovering over my head ♫
-
14:08 - 14:11♫ See if I could still listen, spittin' off the sound ♫
-
14:11 - 14:13♫ See what you grinning ♫
-
14:13 - 14:15♫ I teach the children in the back of the classroom ♫
-
14:15 - 14:18♫ About the message of apocalyptical ♫
-
14:18 - 14:21♫ Not really though, 'cause I've got to keep it simple ♫
-
14:21 - 14:23♫ [unclear] instrumental ♫
-
14:23 - 14:26♫ Detrimental playing Super Mario ♫
-
14:26 - 14:30♫ [unclear] boxes [unclear] hip hop ♫
-
14:30 - 14:32CL: So again, it's an incredible thing that's taking place.
-
14:32 - 14:34It's doing something that, neurologically, is remarkable.
-
14:34 - 14:36Whether or not you like the music is irrelevant.
-
14:36 - 14:38Creatively speaking, it's just a phenomenal thing.
-
14:38 - 14:41This is a short video of how we actually do this in a scanner.
-
14:41 - 14:44(Laughter)
-
14:44 - 14:46(Video) CL: We're here with Emmanuel.
-
14:46 - 14:48CL: That was recorded in the scanner, by the way.
-
14:48 - 14:50(Video) CL: That's Emmanuel in the scanner.
-
14:51 - 14:54He's just memorized a rhyme for us.
-
14:57 - 15:00Emmanuel: ♫ Top of the beat with no repeat ♫
-
15:00 - 15:03♫ Rhythm and rhyme make me complete ♫
-
15:03 - 15:06♫ Climb is sublime when I'm on the mic ♫
-
15:06 - 15:08♫ Spittin' rhymes that'll hit you like a lightning strike ♫
-
15:08 - 15:11♫ I search for the truth in this eternal quest ♫
-
15:11 - 15:14♫ I'm passing on fashion; you can see how I'm dressed ♫
-
15:14 - 15:17CL: Okay. So I'm going to stop that there. So what do we see in his brain?
-
15:17 - 15:19Well, this is actually four rappers' brains.
-
15:19 - 15:21And what we see, we do see language areas lighting up,
-
15:21 - 15:23but then -- eyes closed --
-
15:23 - 15:26when you are free-styling versus memorizing,
-
15:26 - 15:28you've got major visual areas lighting up.
-
15:28 - 15:31You've got major cerebellar activity, which is involved in motor coordination.
-
15:31 - 15:34You have heightened brain activity when you're doing a comparable task,
-
15:34 - 15:37when that one task is creative and the other task is memorized.
-
15:38 - 15:40It's very preliminary, but I think it's kind of cool.
-
15:40 - 15:43So just to conclude, we've got a lot of questions to ask,
-
15:43 - 15:46and like I said, we'll ask questions here, not answer them.
-
15:46 - 15:49But we want to get at the root of what is creative genius, neurologically,
-
15:49 - 15:52and I think, with these methods, we're getting close to being there.
-
15:52 - 15:54And I think hopefully in the next 10, 20 years
-
15:54 - 15:56you'll actually see real, meaningful studies
-
15:56 - 16:00that say science has to catch up to art,
-
16:00 - 16:02and maybe we're starting now to get there.
-
16:02 - 16:04And so I want to thank you for your time. I appreciate it.
-
16:04 - 16:09(Applause)
- Title:
- Your brain on improv
- Speaker:
- Charles Limb
- Description:
-
Musician and researcher Charles Limb wondered how the brain works during musical improvisation -- so he put jazz musicians and rappers in an fMRI to find out. What he and his team found has deep implications for our understanding of creativity of all kinds.
- Video Language:
- English
- Team:
- closed TED
- Project:
- TEDTalks
- Duration:
- 16:10
Krystian Aparta edited English subtitles for Your brain on improv | ||
Krystian Aparta commented on English subtitles for Your brain on improv | ||
Krystian Aparta edited English subtitles for Your brain on improv | ||
Krystian Aparta edited English subtitles for Your brain on improv | ||
TED edited English subtitles for Your brain on improv | ||
TED added a translation |
Krystian Aparta
The English transcript was updated on 11/22/2016. Two subtitles with on-screen text were added: at 14:24, "[fMRI of Hip-Hop Rap]" and at 14:55, "[Control Condition Memorized Verses]."