-
Hi, I am Paul Bloom and I’m a Professor
of Psychology at Yale University.
-
And what I want to do today is talk about the field psychology, the science
of the human mind.
-
Now, I’m admittedly biased, but I think
psychology is the most interesting of all
-
fields.
-
It’s the most interesting because it’s
about us.
-
It’s about the most important and intimate
aspects of our lives.
-
It's about language and
perception. It's about our memory of things, it's about our dreams
-
Love, hate. It's about morality our sense of right and wrong.
-
It's about when things go wrong, as in depression or anxiety,
-
it's about happiness.
-
It's about everything that matters to us.
-
Psychology is a huge field and it breaks up into different sub fields.
-
Some psychologists study the brain, they study neuroscience,.
-
The question of how this physical lump of flesh we have
-
gives rise to our mental life.
-
Others, like me, are Developmental Psychologists.
-
We study how babies turn into children and how children turn into adults.
-
How does a baby or child think differently than an adult.
-
How much of it is hardwired?
-
How much of it do we have to learn?
-
Some psychologists study Social Psychology.
-
Which concerns the relationship of people to other people.
-
As with regard to question like prejudice or persuasion.
-
Others are Cognitive Psychologists.
-
Cognitive psychologist treats the brain like a complicated computer.
-
And asks questions like how do we solve computation problems
-
like understanding language or recognizing faces or remembering facts.
-
Some Psychologists approach this from an evolutionary perspective
-
Looking at the question of how the mind evolved.
-
and how the origin, the evolutionary origins of the mind
-
give us insight into how we think in the here and now.
-
Many people when they think of psychology they think of clinical psychology
-
they think of Dr. Phil and Freud and
-
people who are involved with mental illness.
-
and alot of psychologists do study this.
-
One huge and important field of psychology involves exploring the
-
diagnosis and the causes
-
and particularly the treatments mental illnesses.
-
Illnesses like schizophrenia, depression,
-
obsessive compulsive disorders, phobias.
-
and so many other things.
-
I'm going to talk about 3 separate case studies.
-
I'm going to talk about compassion.
-
racism, and sex.
-
And I'm going to use these case studies as a way
-
to introduce you to psychology.
-
The first case study is compassion.
-
what I mean by compassion
is concern for other people.
-
This is particularly interesting to me.
-
This is my own research program and my own
laboratory at Yale; we look at the emergence
-
of morality in babies and young children.
-
And we particularly focus on the emergence
of compassion.
-
At what point in development do babies care
about others?
-
At what point in development does feelings
of empathy and sympathy, sometimes anger,
-
guilt, other moral emotions.
How do they arise?
-
To what extent are they built in?
-
To what extent do they have to be learned?
-
As a starting point, I have here a picture
of a baby
-
and inside the baby’s head is
the baby’s brain.
-
The baby’s brain is an extraordinary computing
machine.
-
There are by some estimates
-
100 billion neurons in a babies brain.
-
Now neurons are basic cells
-
that process and transmit information.
-
And by one estimate, there’s about 1.8 million
connections between neurons that are created
-
per second.
-
To give you a feeling of the complexity of
the baby’s brain, I use an analogy from
-
Jeff Hawkins.
-
Imagine a football stadium.
-
Fill it up with cooked spaghetti, then shrink
it to the size of a soccer ball.
-
Then make it much, much, much denser.
-
And then you’ll have some understanding
-
of how much is going on inside a brain,
-
inside even a babies brain.
-
Now, that much we know for sure,
-
but where the real debate goes on
-
concerns the nature
-
of that computational structure.
-
There’s one of view that is held by many
-
philosophers and many psychologists which is that the
-
brain starts off as a blank slate, what the
philosopher, John Locke, called “a Tabula
Rasa.”
-
And what goes on in development, the point
of all those connections per second is learning,
-
is sucking up information from the environment.
-
The baby starts off knowing nothing and turns
into an adult, by virtue of absorbing information
-
at a tremendously powerful rate.
-
Many philosophers and many psychologists,
including me and my colleagues are more enamored
-
of another view.
-
We don’t deny that learning takes place,
-
but we would argue that in addition to that,
-
there is an extraordinary early understanding,
early specialization.
-
The brain could better be understood in terms
of what the psychologists, Leda Cosmides and
-
John Tooby, described as a Swiss Army knife,
has many different parts.
-
And each part is specialized for a different
function.
-
Now, so much of the action in psychology has
been a running debate over which view is right.
-
And this concerns morality.
-
Both moral judgements
-
right and wrong
-
but also moral feelings
-
including compassion.
-
Many people would argue that in that regard,
the baby starts off with nothing.
-
The idea is that children start off immoral,
-
monsters or if not monsters,
-
at least they know not from good and evil.
-
This is not the view which I think is supported
by the data.
-
I think there is now more and more data in
support of a different view of compassion.
-
One way to make a baby cry
-
is to expose it to the cries of other babies.
-
There’s sort of contagiousness to the crying.
-
It’s not just crying.
-
We also know that if a baby sees another human
in silent pain, it will distress the baby.
-
It seems part of our very nature is to suffer
at the suffering of others.
-
We know that young babies,
-
as they become capable of moving voluntarily will share.
-
They will share food, for instance,
-
with their siblings and with kids that are around.
-
They will sooth.
-
If they see somebody else in pain, even the
youngest of toddlers will try to reach out
-
and pat the person.
-
Maybe hand over a toy.
-
There’s some lovely studies finding that
slightly older children are able to help others
-
when they see somebody who is unable to fulfill
a goal, they’ll seek out to come to their
-
aid.
-
So one elegant demonstration of this comes
from a recent set of experiments
-
Where they take a toddler
-
put him or her in a situation
-
where an adult is in some sort of
-
mild distress and see if the toddler
-
will voluntarily help
-
even without any prompting.
-
And they find that toddlers typically do.
-
There seems to be some sort of impulse in
us that’s altruistic, that’s kind, that’s
-
compassionate.
-
In all of these cases; however, the kindness
that we see seems to apply to people who are
-
close to us, who are either physically in
-
our proximity or who are our siblings or our
-
parents or our friends.
-
So the question arises, how broad does this
compassion extend?
-
Now some people would argue that we start
off with a very broad compassion, we would
-
extend it to all individuals, to all people.
-
But there’s evidence support a somewhat
different view, which is, there’s a moral
-
instinct in us, there’s a moral sense in
us, but it’s initially very narrow.
-
It’s only triggered by those close to us.
-
In fact, our natural default feelings towards
a stranger, far from being compassionate,
-
is actually some sort of mixture of fear and
hatred.
-
We see this in all sorts of different ways.
-
So in young children, we see it in what’s
called, “stranger anxiety.”
-
At around nine months of age, babies start
becoming panicked at the presence of strangers.
-
and it sees to capture a universal part
-
of development where the other is thought of as dangerous.
-
This sort of stranger anxiety fades in some
cultures.
-
If you were to find yourself in an airport
-
in a new city, you’re not likely to have
-
a panic attack because you’re surrounded
-
by people you don’t know. But in small scale
-
human societies, it might never go away.
-
In a situation when an individual is raised
with a few hundred other individuals around
-
them, that is their circle of compassion.
-
And their response to others is not positive.
-
This is an observation that’s been made
-
by many anthropologists who study small scale
-
societies.
-
So for instance, anthropologist, Jared Diamond,
talking about small scale societies in papua new guinea
-
writes, “To venture out of one’s
territory to meet other humans, even if they
-
lived only a few miles away, was equivalent
to suicide.
-
Many years before, Margaret Meade was talking
about the lifestyles of what were called at
-
the time, “primitive cultures.”
-
And she is famously a supporter of these lifestyles.
-
She argues that the Western world would be
much better if we were to adopt the customs
-
and thoughts and ideas, particularly in regard
to sexuality of these other societies.
-
But she was very honest and very blunt about
how members of these societies treat strangers.
-
She writes: “Most primitive tribes feel
that if you run across one of those sub humans
-
from a rival group in the forest, the most
appropriate thing to do is bludgeon them to
-
death.”
-
I’ve talked about fear and hatred, but there’s
a third sort of response that we often give
to strangers.
-
This is disgust.
-
Disgust is what Paul Rozin described as the
“body/soul emotion,” is a human universal.
-
Humans everywhere are disgusted by certain
things.
-
We are disgusted by feces, urine, blood, vomit,
rotten flesh, and most meat.
-
Disgust has a characteristic facial response
-
<<>>
-
and its easy part of our natures.
-
Now, if it was limited to food and cockroaches
and that sort of thing, it wouldn’t have
-
anything to do with my talk on compassion.
-
But what’s most interesting is that we’re
often disgusted by other people.
-
Particularly, we’re often disgusted by strange
people.
-
By definition
-
any category of human individuals
-
is something you either
-
belong to, or you don't.
-
It's either what psychologists call
-
an in group or an out group.
-
And we have laboratory research that explores
-
the relationship between feelings of disgust
-
and feelings towards out groups.
-
So we know for instance that people differ
in how easily disgusted they are.
-
You do a survey of people.
-
You ask them questions like, how badly would
this bother you.
-
So one of the questions might be, you have
to pick up a dead cat with your hands.
-
And there’s some people who say, “uh, whatever.”
-
Some people, “Oh my god!
-
I’d rather die”.
-
Or, you sit on a city bus seat
-
and it’s warm from the last person
-
who was on it.
-
And some people crack up, well why would that
bother me?
-
Other people say, “That’s very disturbing.”
-
People differ in how sensitive they are to
disgust.
-
It turns out that where you stand with regard
to disgust correlates with your feelings about
-
out groups.
-
It correlates with your feelings about immigrants;
it correlates with your feelings about sexual
-
minorities, in particular male homosexuals.
-
The more easily disgusted you are, the more
aversion you find to these others.
-
We also know this experimentally.
-
We know that by making people be disgusted,
-
we can make them meaner. I’ll give you an example of this.
-
This is from a study I was involved with,
with David Pizarro at Cornell University as
-
the lead investigator.
-
What we did was we brought people into the
lab… into a lab at Cornell.
-
And we asked him all sorts of questions regarding
their feelings towards different groups and
-
different policies.
-
What do you think of African-Americans?
-
What do you think of gay men?
-
What do you think of welfare?
What do you think of immigration?
-
And so on and so forth.
-
Half the people just filled it out and went
home.
-
The other half of the subjects went into the
room, got the same survey.
-
But the difference was, before they entered
the room
-
we sprayed the room with a fart spray.
-
That’s the first experiment I’ve ever
been involved with that used a fart spray.
-
People would be kind of grossed out.
-
And it would make them meaner.
-
Not towards everything, but it would make
-
them particularly meaner towards out groups,
-
like male homosexuals.
-
And this supports the idea that there’s
a connection in our minds between a visceral
-
emotion of disgust and our feelings towards
others.
-
So what I’ve argued is, we do have a natural compassion,
-
but it’s limited. It does not naturally extend to others.
-
But that raises a puzzle
-
because you and me and everyone else we know
-
can extend our compassion to strangers.
-
To put it in the language that the philosopher Peter Singer has used,
-
“Our moral circle has expanded.”
-
It might be that our ancestors, it might be the people in small scale societies only
-
cared about their family and friends.
-
But we have a broader circle of compassion.
-
We think about we care about people in other countries.
-
We care about people from other races.
-
We care about people we’ve never seen before
and we never will see.
-
When some sort of disaster strikes like a
tsunami or a hurricane, many
-
of us give our resources, even our blood,
to help out people we’ve never met before.
-
And that poses a neat psychological puzzle.
-
What forces take our narrow moral circle,
our narrow scope of compassion and
-
bigger and expand it to care for these others?
-
Now I think that there are a lot of different
answers to that question.
-
Robert Wright, for instance, has argued that
one force in expanding the moral circle has
-
been human interconnections in commerce, in
international travel and so on.
-
The more people you know, the more people
you have contact with, the more we are interconnected
-
in the world, the more you might care about
them in a sort of self-interested altruism
-
where you care about them because they’re
fates are intertwined with yours.
-
And I think that there’s a lot of value
in that insight.
-
But I want to focus on a more
-
psychological more individual based mechanism.
-
for expanding our the moral circle.
-
A mechanism that happens to individuals
-
as they get older
-
part of devleopment.
-
which is, their sympathies expand because of a certain
-
sort of persuasion.
-
I want to suggest that there is psychological
evidence that supports the idea that we can
-
expand our compassion, our moral circle to
-
far away strangers by being made to think
-
of them as if they are individual people.
-
We think of them as if they’re our friends and family.
-
We think of them as if they are right in front
of us.
-
Joseph Stalin famously said, “A single death
is a tragedy, a million deaths is a statistic.”
-
And Mother Theresa presented a similar sentiment
when she said, “If I look at the mass, I
-
will never act.
-
If I look at the one, I will.”
-
Psychologists like Paul Slovic has explored this in the lab.
-
So for instance, they would do a study where
-
they would have an appeal for a charity.
-
And in fact, they would take the money they
got and send it to the charity.
-
And they would, for one group of subjects,
describe the problem in terms of statistics,
-
in terms of numbers, in terms of the millions
of people suffering, a sort of suffering a
-
proportion of the population who is in desperate
need.
-
And they found that people would give say,
roughly a dollar.
-
For the other group, they didn’t bother
with statistics at all.
-
They didn’t bother trying to impress them
with the huge number of people suffering.
-
They told them a story about a single individual.
-
They had a picture of that individual, they
gave her a name.
-
And when you do that, you find that people
are far more generous.
-
It’s a far more powerful effect on their
compassion.
-
They will give, roughly, twice as much.
-
Charities, when they try to appeal for people’s
-
help, won’t throw numbers at you.
-
They typically won’t because they know that
-
doesn’t work.
-
The way to extend people’s compassion, the
way to motivate altruistic action is to appeal
-
to some very natural, very hardwired systems
within us that respond to individual people.
-
I think it’s a tremendously
persuasive way for a charity to work.
-
And I think more generally, as part of the
story for how our compassion can get bigger
-
and bigger.
-
People talk about moral progress.
-
People have argued that through our history,
-
our moral circles have been expanding.
-
We now live in a world where people believe
we have moral obligations to other races,
-
other nationalities that sexism and racism
are immoral.
-
Consider the end of slavery in the United States.
-
There are a lot of different factors that
led to the end of slavery, but many historians
-
would argue that one of the forces that led
many white Americans to believe slavery was
-
wrong was persuasion, in particular, it was
the work of the author Harriett Beecher Stowe
-
in her book, Uncle Tom’s Cabin.
-
In this book, she didn’t make logical arguments;
she didn’t make theological points or philosophical
-
proposals.
-
Rather, she got her readers to extend their
sympathies.
-
And this had a profound effect.
-
It had a profound effect persuading them that
-
slavery was wrong
-
so part of morality, part of right and wrong
-
is intimately connected with compassion over our feelings towards others.
-
Some scholars like David Hume,
-
argue that a sense of empathy, a sense of compassion
-
is aptly central to becoming a fully moral being.
-
And certainly notions of right and wrong
-
come up in the domain of race.
-
Cause the very question of forming stereotypes
-
and forming attitudes towards human groups is morally fraught.
-
The second case study I want to talk about
is racism.
-
And I want to begin by making a connection
to a branch of cognitive psychology.
-
In particular the branch of cognitive psychology
that deals with how we make sense of the world.
-
How we naturally form categories of the things
we see and the things we interact with.
-
Cognitive psychologists have pointed out that in order to survive in the world, we have to make generalizations.
-
You probably have never seen those three pictures
I have up here, but you immediately know that
-
one is a dog and one is an apple and one is
a chair.
-
You will also have intuitions about these
things… you’ll make generalizations.
-
You’ll believe the dog can bark, the apple
is something you can eat, a chair is something
-
you can sit on.
-
Now, you probably also realize that there
are exceptions to this.
-
Some dogs are silent, some apples are poisonous,
some chairs will collapse if you sit on them,
-
but still if you couldn’t make those generalizations,
if you didn’t recognize that some properties
-
tend to co-occur with some objects, you would
be helpless in the world.
-
You wouldn’t know what to eat, you wouldn’t
know how anything would react; you wouldn’t
-
survive.
-
Part of being a successful human, in fact,
part of being any successful animal is being
-
able to learn.
-
And a good part of what learning is is to
make statistical generalizations on the basis
-
of limited experience.
-
You eat a thousand apples, they all taste
pretty good, you conclude, I can eat apples,
-
apples taste good.
-
And when you’re hungry, you reach for the apple.
-
This is adaptive, it is rational, it is reasonable.
-
But now there’s a twist.
-
The twist is that some of the categories that
we form are categories of people.
-
We form categories on the basis of sex, of age, of
-
race, profession, religion, sexual orientation,
nationality, and where the person lives.
-
When we form categories of people, we often call these stereotypes.
-
Now, stereotype may sound like a bad word,
but there’s nothing bad about it.
-
For one thing, stereotypes are often accurate.
-
We’re reasonably good statistical learners,
and so we tend to be reasonably accurate.
-
Also, stereotypes are often positive, particularly
of groups that we ourselves belong to.
-
Some of the statistical generalizations may
be correct and may be positive as some groups
-
have reputations for being smart, for being
loyal, for being brave, for all sorts of things
-
that are not at all negative.
-
And so there’s nothing inherently wrong
about stereotypes.
-
On the other hand there are several problems with stereotypes.
-
For one thing, they’re reliable insofar
as they’re based on a sample, an unbiased
-
sample, of the population.
-
But a lot of the information we get about
human groups is through biased sources like
-
how they’re represented in the media.
-
And if these sources don’t give you an accurate
rendition, you’re a stereotype won’t be
-
accurate.
-
For example, many Italian-Americans were upset
at the depiction of Italian-Americans in a
-
television show, “The Sopranos.”
-
This is because, if you are in an area where
the only Italian-Americans you meet are those
-
you see on TV and those you see on “The
Sopranos,” you’re going to think they’re
-
all mobsters.
-
Many Jews historically have been troubled
by Shakespeare’s depiction of Shylock.
-
If the only Jew you know is Shakespeare’s
Shylock, again, it’s going to be a very
-
bad impression.
-
And so one problem with stereotypes is while
we have accurate statistical mechanisms for
-
taking in information and drawing conclusions
from them, often our information isn’t reliable
-
and often this can lead to the formation of
stereotypes that aren’t right.
-
A second problem is that stereotypes regardless
of whether or not they’re accurate can have
-
a negative effect on the people that they apply to.
-
And this is what the psychologist, Claude
Steele, described as stereotype threat.
-
So he has a vivid example of this.
-
Here’s how to make African-Americans do
worse on a math test.
-
You have the test and you put on the test
that they have to identify their race.
-
The very act of acknowledging that their African-American
when given a test ignites in them thoughts
-
of their own stereotype, which isn’t positive,
which is negative regarding academics and
-
that makes them do worse.
-
Want to know how to make a woman do worse
on a math test?
-
Same thing, get her to write down her sex.
-
One recent study found a sort of clever twist
on this.
-
The study involved testing Asian-American
women.
-
Turns out, when Asian-American women are given
a test and they’re asked to mark down their
-
race, they do better than they would otherwise
do.
-
They’re reminded of the stereotype, but
as a positive stereotype and it bumps them
-
up.
-
You ask them, on the other hand, to mark down
their sex, they do worse because they’re
-
women and that’s a negative stereotype towards
women.
-
That’s an example of how stereotypes have
a potentially damaging effect on people.
-
A third problem with stereotypes is, in some
way, our stereotypes of human groups are like
-
our categories of dogs and apples and chairs.
-
But there’s a way in which they aren’t.
-
We’re not dogs and apples and chairs.
-
But we are members of human groups.
-
And this fact of how you connect with the
category has an effect on how you think of
-
the category.
-
There’s a lot of evidence suggesting that
when you’re a member of the category, you
-
boost it.
-
You give it higher qualities.
-
People in your group are smarter or nicer,
they’re more deserving and so on.
-
On the other hand, if it’s an out group,
if it’s another category, particularly if
-
it’s a category that you’re in some way
competing against, the category gets denigrated.
-
We see some vivid historical example of this.
-
In one study in 1942, Americans were asked
to describe the top two features of Russians.
-
And they described them as brave and hard-working.
-
In 1948, they were asked the same question.
-
They described them as cruel and conceited.
-
The Russians didn’t change, what changed
was our relationship to them over the intervening
-
years; they went from being part of a group
that we were a part of to the out group.
-
The final problem with stereotypes is a moral
one.
-
Even if stereotypes are perfectly accurate,
even if they’re accurate summaries of the
-
statistics of a group, there are many cases
where we believe that it’s morally wrong
-
to judge somebody based on their group membership.
-
We should judge them as individuals.
-
For all of these reasons, and maybe mostly
for the last one,
-
there’s an interesting tension in how we think about other groups.
-
On the one hand, we want to be consciously
egalitarian, consciously non-racist, consciously
-
thinking of individuals as individuals and
not letting stereotypes, particularly ugly
-
stereotypes affect our judgments.
-
And there’s some evidence that we succeed
at this.
-
You look at the statistics, for instance,
what you see in this graph, is there are a
-
portion of Americans who say they would vote
for a qualified African-American to be President.
-
And what you could see is, at a certain point
by the mid-nineties, just about everybody
-
says, that they would.
-
And the election of Barrack Obama shows that
this wasn’t just people lying when asked
-
questions, it really reflects an honest to god consciously egalitarian viewpoint.
-
On the other hand, we also have an unconscious
system.
-
And an unconscious system is more statistics
driven, more biased and less sensitive to
-
moral concerns.
-
So you get a tension between the conscious
egalitarian system and the unconscious system,
-
which is often driven by bias.
-
This conscious, this unconscious system is
data-driven
-
and it is a lot less sensitive to our
moral concerns than the conscious system.
-
One striking example of unconscious biases involves
-
being shown these two faces.
-
And asked who is more American?
-
Well at some level this is a ridiculous question.
-
You would laugh when they hear it.
-
Barack Obama is more American cause he's like American.
-
Tony Blair is British.
-
But unconsciously you can ask the same question.
-
You can see how quickly it takes to associate
-
these faces with words like American or not American.
-
And it turns out based on this sort of implicit unconscious test
-
people are often more willing and quicker to
-
associate the face of Tony Blair as American
-
than the face of Barack Obama.
-
Of course because Tony Blair has a white face
-
and Barack Obama has a dark face.
-
Now, one response to these sorts of studies,
one perfectly legitimate response I think
-
is say, who cares.
-
Consciously, we’re egalitarian, consciously
we’re non-prejudice, we have these weird,
-
quirky unconscious biases that drive our behavior
-
when pressing buttons and responding fast.
-
What difference does it make?
-
But there’s evidence it does make a difference.
-
There’s evidence that these unconscious
biases play a role in things that matter very
-
much in the real world.
-
So consider some studies by Jack Dovidio and
his colleagues.
-
They first did this study in 1989, and what
it involved is, you give people resumes of
-
candidates and these resumes have pictures.
-
And what the subjects and experiment don’t
know is they were always given the same resume,
-
but half of them got it with a white person,
half of them a black person.
-
And then they were asked, how strongly would
you recommend this person for a job?
-
Now, if these candidates had strong qualifications,
they both would be recommended.
-
In fact, perhaps the black is a bit more likely
to be recommended than the white one.
-
But when they had moderate qualifications,
when it’s a judgment call, the white candidate
-
was statistically more like to be recommended
for a job than the black candidate.
-
Not because these people said, I’m a racist,
I’m going to do it this way,
-
but rather they are swayed by this factor that they might
not have been conscious of.
-
As I said, this was done in 1989.
-
But they did the same study in 1999 and got
the same result.
-
And they did the same study in 2005, and got
the same result.
-
So, we’re at war with ourselves.
-
We have on the one hand these conscious beliefs
about how we think we should think, how we
-
think we should behave.
-
On the other hand, we have this unconscious
system that makes all these sorts of decisions
-
and affects us in ways that we might not know
about, we might not be aware of.
-
The good news is we’re also smart.
-
And part of being smart means that we can
structure our world so that we can make it
-
that unconscious biases matter less.
-
To give you an example of this, I’ll turn not to race, but to gender.
-
Not too long ago, women were deeply underrepresented
in symphony orchestras.
-
And the reason for this, it was argued is
because they don’t play as well.
-
In a fair and biased fashion
they’d been judged and they just don’t
-
sound as good.
-
But in part, based on these sorts of discoveries,
symphony orchestras began to hold blind auditions.
-
What they would do is they would have the
person play behind a screen.
-
The judges won’t know if they are listening to a man or to a woman.
-
Once this was put into place, the representation
of women in symphony orchestras shot up.
-
It wasn’t that originally these were just sexist, to say,
-
I don’t like women; I’m going to count against them.
-
Rather, these were perhaps good, non-sexist
people, who couldn’t help hearing the woman
-
differently from the man.
-
And I like this example because it shows how
first, social psychology and psychology in
-
general can shape policy in a good way.
-
But second, it shows how we’re smart enough
to manipulate the world so that our better
-
selves get to make the decisions.
-
The third case study I want to talk about
is sex.
-
And when it comes to sex, considerations of
evolution become incredibly relevant.
-
I think the question of how we evolve and
the question of how our minds are now shaped
-
in response to evolution pressure is something
that pertains to all of psychology.
-
It pertains to, certainly, for how we think
about human groups, certainly for compassion
-
and morality, and all sorts of other topics
I haven’t discussed, like perception and
-
language and memory.
-
But it’s screamingly obvious in the domain
of sex.
-
As soon as you start thinking about
our bodies and our brains, you’re faced
-
with a puzzle.
-
And it’s the sort of a puzzle that can only
be resolved in terms of evolution.
-
And here’s what the puzzle is.
-
What’s the difference between males and
females?
-
Well, there’s a general answer to this that
doesn’t pertain to any particular species
-
that goes across every creature on earth.
-
The males are the ones with the small sex
cells.
-
To be a male is to have a sperm, which contains
genetic material, and that’s basically it.
-
The females have the large sex cells.
-
The female sex cell, the egg, also contains
genetic material but it contains a cover,
-
it contains food, it contains all the apparatus
needed to get an organism growing.
-
So here’s the puzzle.
-
You look around most animals, not all animals,
but most animals.
-
And the male is bigger and more aggressive.
-
So why would the animal with the smaller sex
cell tend to grow up to be the bigger animal.
-
And this has been a mystery for a very long
time until an evolutionary biologist named
-
Robert Trivers, solved it.
-
And he solved it by making reference to the
idea of parental investment.
-
Trivers defines parental investment as any
investment by the parent in an individual
-
offspring that increases the offspring’s
chances of surviving at the cost of the parent’s
-
ability to invest in other offspring.
-
Given the different size in the sex cells
-
it means that males typicaly have less investment than females.
-
Because in most, though not in all species
-
the offspring grows inside the females body.
-
And while that offspring is growing inside the females body
-
the male is free to have other offspring, but the female is not.
-
for humans for instance a man can ejaculate
-
then moments later or hours later
-
or however much later can ejaculate again.
-
And there's virtually no practical limit
-
to how many different offspring a man can have.
-
For a woman when she get's pregnant
-
she can't have more kids during that period.
-
In fact it is difficult for her to have kids even later on when she is breastfeeding.
-
So there is a fairly small number of offspring
-
a female human can have.
-
This makes a difference,
-
it makes a difference. It makes a difference in the
-
sort of economic game theoretic structure.
-
of human sexuality.
-
What follows from this from an economic point of view
-
is that males compete with on another
-
for access to females.
-
Both males and females want offspring, that’s
the genetic imperative, but males can be more
-
into number while females can be more into
quality.
-
This leads to competition between males.
-
And the competition is of two different sorts.
-
There’s competition male against male, which
leads to the evolution of aggressive trades.
-
It even leads to the evolution of some species
with special organs, like the giant horns
-
of some animals that exist for males and not
females because they’ve evolved according
-
to this reproductive logic based on the lower
parental investment.
-
It also leads males to evolve certain traits
to attract the attention of females.
-
Females are the scarce resource here.
-
Females as a rule are more choosey
-
when it comes to short term sexual partners than males.
-
And so males compete with one another to attract
females.
-
The most striking biological example of this
-
is the elaborate, glorious plumage of the peacock.
-
There’s a carton I enjoy here because the
peahens are saying to the peacock,
-
“Cut the crap and show us your willy.”
-
Which I like because it sounds sort of British,
but I also like because it nicely captures
-
the evolutionary logic behind what all of
this is for.
-
Finally, you get to relative choosiness.
-
Females are, as a rule, more choosy when it
comes to short-term sexual partners than males.
-
And this shows up in a couple of ways, it
shows up in prostitution.
-
So, prostitution is a huge industry in the
world.
-
And with very few exceptions, prostitutes
cater to male customers.
-
Then there’s pornography.
-
Now, pornography may appeal to different sexes,
some people have argued that romance novels
-
are sort of the equivalent to pornography
for women.
-
But what appeals to men is often sort of images
of sexually receptive women.
-
This isn’t the same as sort of a one-night
stand, but is a psychologically vicarious
-
one-night stand, where this image is enough
to lead to arousal.
-
The only thing interesting I have to say about
this is a recent study that suggests this
-
is not exclusively a human vice.
-
So recent study involved showing pornography
to Rhesus Macaques, these are a type of monkey.
-
The question was, would these monkeys pay to see porn?
-
And so you didn’t have a financial system
for these monkeys, so they set up a nice apparatus
-
where at a certain point, the
monkey had a choice, he could either stare
-
at a picture or turn and sip sweet orange
juice; monkeys love orange juice.
-
So the question is, what sort of pictures
would they pay, would they give up on this
-
orange juice in order to see?
-
And there were two sorts of pictures that
they would pay to see.
-
They would pay to see the behinds of female
Rhesus monkeys and they would pay to see the
-
faces of high status male Rhesus monkeys.
-
Sort of like the equivalent of a Playboy Magazine
and People Magazine, suggesting that two of
-
the major human vices, pornography and celebrity
worship are in fact not uniquely human.
-
Now, you can go on about the differences between
males and females in terms of sexual interest
-
and sexual hues and so on, but I want to focus
for the rest of this case study on certain
-
things we have in common.
-
And one thing that we have in common is an
attraction to what we would call beauty.
-
Certain things are beautiful, certain things
appeal to us universally.
-
Some studies find in the first tenth of a
second after looking at a face, you have computed
-
how beautiful it is.
-
Cultures differ tremendously, different times,
different places
-
in what counts as beautiful, what counts as sexually attractive.
-
And that is entirely true; there are interesting
and powerful differences.
-
But at the same time, there are also universals.
-
There are certain things that people everywhere
find attractive.
-
And we can use evolutionary theory to makes
sense of the sort of things people find as
-
beautiful.
-
So to some extent, beauty equates to youth.
-
Hues like round eyes, full lips, smooth tight
skin.
-
Most likely it is because they are cues the person is young,
-
is able to have kids, has many years ahead of them.
-
And so on.
-
Beauty also equates to health.
-
We are drawn to features like
-
absence of deformities, clear eyes, unblemished skin
-
intact teeth, and average faces.
-
And you might think, average faces?
-
That’s strange thing to put in beautiful,
as a category of beautiful.
-
But it turns out average faces actually look
really good.
-
So what an average face does is it gets rid
of all of the things that are unusual and
-
people tend to find it quite attractive.
-
Now one accusation that always comes up in
these situations is,
-
who did you get this data from?
-
And in fact, psychologists are often guilty
of collecting data from 24 university freshmen
-
and then saying that these conclusions apply
to all of humanity, but not in this case.
-
In this case, studies of human attractiveness
have been done cross-culturally and you get
-
pretty much the same findings wherever you
go.
-
Again, there’s some interesting differences,
but these universals seem to always be attractive.
-
The work that’s most exciting to me along
these lines is actually done with babies.
-
So adults can rank faces as attractive or
unattractive, but you can also see what babies
-
think about faces.
-
And it turns out, using babies looking time
as a measure for what they like to see.
-
So how long will they look at a face?
-
It turns out that babies preference for attractive
faces match pretty well adults preferences
-
for attractive faces.
-
So for instance, in some wonderful work by
Languar[ph] and her colleagues, she would
-
show different degrees of averageness across
faces, composites of more and more people.
-
You can see this in these faces are not the
faces of real people, these faces are computer
-
composites of multiple people and they are
not bad looking, but you can get better than
-
average for both males and females.
-
For females, many people will judge a face
better than average if it’s feminized.
-
If you take the features that define a face
as female and you exaggerate them, as in the
-
picture on the right, it tends to look a little
bit better than your average female face.
-
For men, you could do the same thing.
-
you can take faces and
-
take your average male face and turn it into
testosterone man, which is the face that you’re
-
looking at on the left.
-
It turns out that women’s responses to testosterone
man differ according to whether or not they’re
-
ovulating.
-
So if a woman is ovulating, she is more likely
to find this manliest of man face attractive,
-
while if not, she tends to go back to just
find average man more attractive.
-
And there's different theories of precisely why this is so.
-
It does suggest that our sexual psychologies
connect with our reproductive preferences
-
in all sorts of interesting ways.
-
But there’s a lot more to looking
good than how you look.
-
It turns out, how attractive you find a face
-
is critically dependent on how much you like the person.
-
The more you like somebody, the better they look to you.
-
This is why spouses and happy marraiges
-
will honestly find their husband or wife
-
far more attractive than anyone else finds them.
-
This is true more generally.
-
In a classic study, David Bus,
-
tested people from 37 different cultures around the world
-
and asked who is your perfect mate?
-
He was largely looking for sex differences in theses studies
-
and he found them, he found all sorts of differences
-
in what men were looking for and in what women were looking for.
-
But he also found one similarity, one thing
-
one way in which women and men were alike.
-
And this is that for both, the number one quality
-
people were looking for in a mate
-
was kindness.
-
All of this in the domain of sex
-
supports a moral from Shakespeare
-
which is "love looks not with the eyes, but with the mind".
-
What I have done is I have very briefly talked about
-
three case studies in the domain of psychology.
-
I talked about compassion,
-
I talked about racism,
-
and I have talked about sex.
-
In the course of this I've tried to illustrate
-
certain themes in the study of psychology, in general.
-
And in fact I started by listing six domains of psychology.
-
Neuroscience, I started by talking about the babies brain
-
and gave that as a starting point
-
for the question of the development of compassion
-
the development of these other traits.
-
but every domain of psychology every
-
aspect of our mental life
-
is caused by our physical brains.
-
And in fact, in all of these domains I’ve
talked about, compassion and moral psychology
-
and more general, race and stereotyping and
thinking about groups, and sex and sexuality,
-
people have used the methods of neuroscience
-
to better understand how the mind works in
these domains.
-
I’ve talked about development and I focused
mostly in development on the first case study
-
of compassion, but of course there’s a huge
amount of very interesting research on the
-
development of our understanding of groups
asking the question, for instance, are young
-
children racist?
-
Do young children have complicit biases?
-
And of course in the development of romance
and sexuality, how does the mind of a child
-
before puberty differ and how much is it like
the mind of an adult after puberty and how
-
did these differs take place?
-
These are extraordinarily interesting developmental
questions.
-
All three domains connect to social psychology and cognitive psychology in clear ways.
-
They are all questions about social psychology,
they are all questions about dealing with
-
other people; how we deal with and make sense
out of other people.
-
And they all connect to questions of cognitive
psychology, like the perception of faces,
-
the formation of categories, the comprehension
of stories; those are all central parts of
-
cognitive psychology and central to understanding
the domains we’ve talked about here.
-
We’ve talked about evolution, particularly
again, in the case of sexuality.
-
But of course, the evolutionary psychology
of morality and compassion is a fascinating
-
issues connecting it with research done with
other primates, our evolutionary relatives
-
like chimpanzees and monkeys.
-
I’ve said the least about clinical psychology.
-
Clinical psychology is extraordinarily interesting
and also connects to each of the domains that
-
I’ve talked about.
-
People are interested in the psychopathology
related to sexuality.
-
They’re particularly interested in the psychopathology
in mental illness in the domain of morality
-
because this connects to one of the most troubling
and one of the most interesting questions
-
in clinical psychology, which concerns the
psychopath.
-
There are some of us, apparently, who don’t
have consciences, who don’t feel compassion,
-
who will destroy other people’s lives out
of malice or self-interest or simple boredom.
-
And the question of where psychopaths come
from, what’s the precise nature of what’s
-
going wrong with them.
-
Most of all, what can be done about them are
issues of extraordinary interest.
-
Psychology is the perfect liberal arts major
-
because it connects across all these interesting disciplines
-
and gives you intellectual tools, tools that we share
-
with philosophers and we share with chemists
-
and we share with people who study english literature.
-
It brings them all together in the project of studying the mind.
-
Every interesting question in psychology is also an
-
interesting question for scholars outside psychology.
-
We've made huge progress over the last many years
-
in understanding mental life.
-
And I think there's no reason to expect this progress to stop.
-
I think that in the end
-
the most important and intimate aspects of ourselves,
-
how we understand people, our emotions, our motivations
-
our desires, our sense of right and wrong.
-
can be understood through the methods of scientific
psychology
-
through constructing and testing hypotheses.
-
Through bringing to bear considerations
-
based on evolution.
-
and computation and neuroscience.
-
Now, some people might find this a scary prospect.
-
I know that there are some people that worry
that a scientific perspective of the mind
-
takes away from us somehow.
-
It diminishes us.
-
It makes us less than what we are.
-
I don’t agree.
-
I have the opposite reaction, which is that
as the more you understand the mind from a
-
serious scientific point of view; the more
you come to appreciate its complexity, its
-
uniqueness and its beauty.
-
Thank you.