Return to Video

How industrialization changed childhood | Dorsa Amir | TEDxCambridge

  • 0:16 - 0:18
    When I was in graduate school,
  • 0:18 - 0:21
    one thing I often heard
    was that the goal of anthropology
  • 0:21 - 0:27
    is to make the strange familiar
    and the familiar strange.
  • 0:29 - 0:33
    I thought I knew what that meant,
    but I didn't really understand it
  • 0:33 - 0:36
    until the first time
    I conducted fieldwork.
  • 0:37 - 0:39
    In my first year of graduate school,
  • 0:39 - 0:42
    I traveled to South America
    to work with the Shuar,
  • 0:42 - 0:46
    an indigenous population
    in the Ecuadorian Amazon.
  • 0:46 - 0:50
    I was studying how children develop
    across different cultures.
  • 0:51 - 0:55
    I had a number of expectations
    for what that trip would be like.
  • 0:56 - 1:00
    I knew I was going to get
    a lot of bug bites - I did.
  • 1:01 - 1:06
    I knew I'd ride a canoe down
    the winding rivers of the jungle - I did.
  • 1:07 - 1:12
    And I knew I'd learn something
    I didn't know before - and I did.
  • 1:12 - 1:16
    I learned what childhood was like
    in a small scale society.
  • 1:17 - 1:22
    I saw independent young kids
    climbing trees to gather papayas
  • 1:22 - 1:24
    when they were hungry.
  • 1:24 - 1:27
    I saw them starting fires,
  • 1:28 - 1:31
    preparing food
    for themselves and their siblings
  • 1:32 - 1:37
    and even using machetes
    quite confidently, I might add.
  • 1:40 - 1:42
    (Laughter)
  • 1:43 - 1:45
    The thing I didn't expect
  • 1:45 - 1:49
    was the culture shock
    of coming back to the United States.
  • 1:50 - 1:54
    The strangeness of the Shuar
    was becoming more familiar to me,
  • 1:55 - 2:00
    but suddenly, the familiarity of home
    started to feel strange.
  • 2:02 - 2:04
    Being a child in a foraging society
  • 2:04 - 2:08
    is very different than being a child
    in a Western society.
  • 2:08 - 2:10
    In societies like the Shuar,
  • 2:10 - 2:15
    children are much more independent
    or as we like to call it "free-range."
  • 2:16 - 2:17
    (Laughter)
  • 2:17 - 2:19
    If you grew up in the era
    before cellphones,
  • 2:19 - 2:22
    that might be what your own
    childhood was like too.
  • 2:23 - 2:27
    But for the newest generation of kids,
    this independance is quickly fading away.
  • 2:28 - 2:33
    And that's because childhood has changed
    very recently and very rapidly.
  • 2:35 - 2:37
    For virtually all of our time
    on this planet,
  • 2:37 - 2:39
    for hundreds of thousands of years,
  • 2:39 - 2:43
    our species lived in small bands
    of hunter-gatherers,
  • 2:43 - 2:48
    more similar to how the Shuar live now
    than how the average American lives.
  • 2:49 - 2:52
    Then just a few thousand years ago,
  • 2:52 - 2:55
    most of our environment
    started to change a lot.
  • 2:55 - 2:58
    In fact, they changed
    so quickly and so drastically
  • 2:59 - 3:01
    that many anthropologists believe
  • 3:01 - 3:05
    we are now in a state
    of evolutionary mismatch.
  • 3:07 - 3:09
    This means that the environment
    has changed too quickly
  • 3:09 - 3:12
    for some of our genes to keep up.
  • 3:13 - 3:17
    Cultural evolution is much faster
    than genetic evolution.
  • 3:18 - 3:20
    And what this means
    is that our minds and bodies
  • 3:20 - 3:25
    might be optimized for a world
    that most of us no longer live in.
  • 3:27 - 3:30
    So what did this
    past environment look like?
  • 3:30 - 3:34
    Well, it's impossible to perfectly
    answer this question:
  • 3:34 - 3:38
    bones fossilize, behavior doesn't.
  • 3:39 - 3:43
    But we can learn a lot by looking
    at current day societies like the Shuar.
  • 3:43 - 3:44
    And I want to clarify here:
  • 3:44 - 3:49
    the Shuar are not a prehistoric people
    or windows into the past;
  • 3:49 - 3:51
    they are modern people.
  • 3:51 - 3:56
    But their way of life may give us a clue
    about what childhood was like in the past.
  • 3:56 - 3:57
    And what we know
  • 3:57 - 4:01
    from looking across a large number
    of these small-scale societies
  • 4:01 - 4:03
    is that there are a lot
    of common patterns.
  • 4:04 - 4:06
    For instance, in virtually
    all of these societies,
  • 4:06 - 4:09
    women give natural birth,
  • 4:09 - 4:13
    mothers breastfeed frequently
    and for long periods of time,
  • 4:14 - 4:17
    parents sleep in the same
    room as their kids
  • 4:18 - 4:22
    and children are constantly
    in physical contact with other people.
  • 4:24 - 4:27
    These patterns have started to change
    in our Western societies:
  • 4:28 - 4:33
    here C-sections are more prevalent,
    as is formula feeding.
  • 4:34 - 4:36
    Kids often sleep
    in their own little rooms,
  • 4:37 - 4:39
    and we don't want people
  • 4:39 - 4:41
    in physical contact with us
    or our children.
  • 4:41 - 4:43
    We value personal space.
  • 4:44 - 4:47
    But let's go back to the idea of mismatch.
  • 4:48 - 4:51
    The practices in yellow have marked
    the human experience
  • 4:51 - 4:54
    for 99% of our time on this planet.
  • 4:55 - 4:59
    The stuff in blue
    is just 1% of human history.
  • 5:00 - 5:03
    These massive changes
    have happened so quickly
  • 5:03 - 5:06
    that they may be out of whack
    with the world we are born expecting.
  • 5:07 - 5:11
    And we think there are consequences
    for these changes, both good and bad.
  • 5:11 - 5:14
    For instance, C-sections save lives,
  • 5:14 - 5:18
    but they also shape the bacteria
    in our gut and our immune system
  • 5:18 - 5:22
    in ways that might
    have negative consequences.
  • 5:23 - 5:26
    But as an anthropoligst
    who studies behavior,
  • 5:26 - 5:30
    one shared feature of these societies
    that I want to focus on
  • 5:30 - 5:33
    and one that I've been able to see
    for myself is this:
  • 5:34 - 5:38
    in small scale societies,
    alongside the adult community,
  • 5:39 - 5:44
    is a complex mini-community of children,
    an alternate society.
  • 5:45 - 5:49
    This child society is made up
    of kids of all ages and genders.
  • 5:50 - 5:54
    It has its own unique culture and leaders.
  • 5:54 - 5:58
    It has drama and a surprising amount
    of political intrigue.
  • 6:01 - 6:04
    To understand why this child society
    is so important for development,
  • 6:04 - 6:07
    it helps to keep in mind
    a curious fact about humans,
  • 6:07 - 6:11
    and that is to become
    a successful adult in any culture,
  • 6:12 - 6:16
    children have to learn an enormous
    number of complicated skills.
  • 6:16 - 6:20
    These include both technical skills,
    like building a fire,
  • 6:20 - 6:24
    and social skills,
    like maintaining friendships.
  • 6:24 - 6:28
    And to perfect these skills,
    which take decades,
  • 6:28 - 6:31
    children also develop foundational skills,
  • 6:31 - 6:36
    like creativity, determination
    and problem solving.
  • 6:37 - 6:41
    Like, one day, when the Shuar adults
    were playing a soccer game,
  • 6:41 - 6:45
    I saw a group of kids set up their own
    soccer game right next to the adults.
  • 6:45 - 6:46
    When they called me over,
  • 6:46 - 6:50
    I got really excited because I thought
    they wanted me to play with them,
  • 6:50 - 6:52
    but they were like,
    actually, you just keep score,
  • 6:52 - 6:54
    so all of us can play.
  • 6:54 - 6:55
    (Laughter)
  • 6:55 - 6:56
    Problem solved.
  • 6:57 - 7:01
    Shuar kids, like kids everywhere,
    spend a lot of time observing adults
  • 7:01 - 7:04
    and incorporating
    their behaviors into their play.
  • 7:04 - 7:09
    In fact, this is one of the reasons
    the play itself has evolved
  • 7:09 - 7:13
    as a way to practice these skills
    in ways that are low-cost.
  • 7:14 - 7:16
    No one has to encourage kids to do this.
  • 7:16 - 7:18
    They do it on their own
  • 7:18 - 7:23
    because through unstructured play,
    children learn how to become adults.
  • 7:24 - 7:27
    Another important benefit
    to this mixed-age society
  • 7:27 - 7:32
    is that kids teach to
    and learn from one another.
  • 7:32 - 7:35
    Younger kids benefit
    by learning from older kids
  • 7:35 - 7:38
    who are only slightly better than them.
  • 7:38 - 7:42
    And through teaching,
    older kids strengthen their own skills,
  • 7:42 - 7:46
    as you may have experienced yourself -
    teaching helps you learn.
  • 7:48 - 7:51
    It's hard to find these patterns
    in our Western societies.
  • 7:52 - 7:54
    Kids here spend the majority
    of their development
  • 7:54 - 7:59
    in a room with other kids
    their own age by design.
  • 8:00 - 8:02
    Adults are totally in charge
  • 8:02 - 8:05
    of the content and structure
    of their time.
  • 8:05 - 8:09
    They determine when kids eat.
    They determine when kids play.
  • 8:09 - 8:11
    They even determine
    when kids can go to the bathroom.
  • 8:12 - 8:15
    And the unstrucutred play time of recess
  • 8:15 - 8:18
    is now quickly becoming
    a structured activity.
  • 8:20 - 8:22
    A strange byproduct
    of all this micro-managing
  • 8:22 - 8:26
    is that we're teaching kids things
    that don't even need to be taught.
  • 8:26 - 8:29
    Kids don't need to be taught how to play.
  • 8:29 - 8:32
    They don't even need
    to be taught how to talk, really.
  • 8:32 - 8:35
    You don't have to point
    to an apple and say, "This is an apple."
  • 8:36 - 8:38
    Kids can also learn through exposure,
  • 8:38 - 8:41
    like hearing you ask your husband
    for an apple and him handing you one.
  • 8:42 - 8:43
    "Apples." - done.
  • 8:45 - 8:47
    And the idea that it is a parent's duty
  • 8:47 - 8:51
    to constantly craft and monitor
    their children's experiences
  • 8:51 - 8:56
    causes a lot of unnecessary anxiety
    and may, in fact, be detrimental.
  • 8:58 - 9:04
    When we take away mixed-aged play groups,
    when we take away unstructured play,
  • 9:04 - 9:08
    we are in fact taking away
    the training wheels to adulthood
  • 9:08 - 9:10
    that children have had for millennia.
  • 9:11 - 9:15
    We are contributing to an increasingly
    mismatch environment.
  • 9:16 - 9:17
    What's more,
  • 9:17 - 9:21
    instead of letting kids develop
    foundational skills like problem solving,
  • 9:21 - 9:24
    we're flipping to the back of the book
    to show them the answers.
  • 9:26 - 9:29
    But that leaves them unprepared for all
    the new problems they're going to face.
  • 9:30 - 9:31
    This might be one of the reasons
  • 9:31 - 9:35
    why the transition to college
    is so difficult for kids here,
  • 9:35 - 9:39
    or why choosing a career path
    can feel so daunting.
  • 9:39 - 9:42
    There's been very little space to explore.
  • 9:44 - 9:48
    The lesson is not that we should
    go back to living like foragers
  • 9:48 - 9:51
    or think whatever is natural
    is what we should do.
  • 9:52 - 9:54
    That's the naturalistic fallacy.
  • 9:55 - 9:58
    But we would benefit
    from taking a broader perspective
  • 9:58 - 10:00
    and understanding
    how our evolutionary history
  • 10:00 - 10:03
    has shaped the way our minds develop.
  • 10:05 - 10:07
    We all want the best for our kids.
  • 10:08 - 10:12
    We want them to be independent,
    confident, problem solvers.
  • 10:12 - 10:13
    We want them to be happy.
  • 10:15 - 10:16
    But paradoxically,
  • 10:16 - 10:20
    our cultural practices in the West
    might be undermining children's abilities
  • 10:20 - 10:21
    to develop these skills.
  • 10:23 - 10:26
    It's hard to study the long-term
    consequences of these changes,
  • 10:26 - 10:28
    especially because
    they're happening so quickly.
  • 10:29 - 10:32
    But there are some tried-and-true
    methods to raising kids
  • 10:32 - 10:33
    that we've been using for millennia.
  • 10:33 - 10:35
    In fact, we've been using them for so long
  • 10:35 - 10:39
    that children minds might actually
    be expecting them.
  • 10:40 - 10:42
    Some of these methods
    you can start using now.
  • 10:42 - 10:45
    Set up more mixed-age
    play dates for your kids.
  • 10:47 - 10:50
    Give them the room
    they need to make mistakes.
  • 10:51 - 10:53
    Give them more unstructured playtime.
  • 10:54 - 10:56
    In fact, this is not just an idea
  • 10:56 - 10:58
    that's relevant for those
    who are raising kids
  • 10:58 - 11:01
    because even as adults,
    we are still developing.
  • 11:02 - 11:04
    These lessons are applicable to you too.
  • 11:05 - 11:07
    Allow yourself to make mistakes.
  • 11:08 - 11:12
    Spend time with older family
    and younger friends.
  • 11:12 - 11:15
    Give yourself some unstructured playtime.
  • 11:17 - 11:21
    And perhaps recognize
    that the familiar culture all around you
  • 11:21 - 11:24
    is, in fact, very strange.
  • 11:25 - 11:27
    Thank you.
  • 11:27 - 11:30
    (Applause)
Title:
How industrialization changed childhood | Dorsa Amir | TEDxCambridge
Description:

You may think your childhood was normal: you had friends your age, attended school to learn from teachers, and maybe even slept in your own bedroom. Evolutionary anthropologist Dorsa Amir shows that these everyday occurrences in Western cultures are actually strange new experiences in human history that may have significant consequences for child development. Learn more at http://www.tedxcambridge.com

Dorsa Amir is an evolutionary anthropologist interested in how differing cultural and ecological environments shape the developing mind. She is a postdoctoral research fellow in the Department of Psychology at Boston College and received her PhD in Anthropology from Yale University. Her research adopts a cross-cultural and developmental perspective to explore the role of the local environment in adaptively shaping behavior and preferences. She is currently investigating cross-cultural variation in the development of risk and time preferences, early life socioeconomic effects on behavior, and the role of scarcity in cognitive development.

This talk was given at a TEDx event using the TED conference format but independently organized by a local community. Learn more at https://www.ted.com/tedx

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDxTalks
Duration:
11:42

English subtitles

Revisions