< Return to Video

Intro to Dyslexia and the Dyslexic Advantage

  • 0:00 - 0:02
    [Dr. Fernette Eide] Welcome, everyone!
  • 0:02 - 0:04
    This is the first in what I hope will be
  • 0:04 - 0:09
    a series of webinars that will be helpful
  • 0:09 - 0:11
    to you, in our Dyslexic community at
  • 0:11 - 0:14
    Dyslexicadvantage.com. What I'm going to
  • 0:14 - 0:18
    do is do a brief overview of Dyslexia, and
  • 0:18 - 0:20
    talk very briefly about the Dyslexic
  • 0:20 - 0:22
    Advantage. We can talk more about that in
  • 0:22 - 0:25
    a future meeting, perhaps, and I'll try at
  • 0:25 - 0:27
    the end to address some of the questions
  • 0:27 - 0:30
    that we had in our live webinar.
  • 0:33 - 0:38
    First of all, it's good to recognize that
  • 0:38 - 0:41
    one in five people are Dyslexic--extremely
  • 0:41 - 0:44
    common. What that translates into is
  • 0:44 - 0:47
    8.4 million school age children here in
  • 0:47 - 0:50
    the United States who are Dyslexic. For
  • 0:50 - 0:53
    every one who is identified, there will
  • 0:53 - 0:56
    be 3 who will be missed. It 's really a
  • 0:56 - 0:59
    staggering percentage. There are a lot of
  • 0:59 - 1:02
    reasons for this, but teachers are often
  • 1:02 - 1:05
    not trained in detail about how to
  • 1:05 - 1:09
    recognize Dyslexia, and also, it's not
  • 1:09 - 1:15
    widely understood, either, one of the
  • 1:15 - 1:20
    common signs and features of Dyslexia.
  • 1:20 - 1:22
    So, Dyslexic students have strong big
  • 1:22 - 1:25
    picture skills. They are often known to be
  • 1:25 - 1:27
    very creative, observant, empathetic,
  • 1:27 - 1:30
    and great problem solvers. But they are
  • 1:30 - 1:32
    slower to acquire fine detail, basic
  • 1:32 - 1:34
    skills like reading ,writing , spelling,
  • 1:34 - 1:40
    or mastery of math facts. Dyslexia will
  • 1:40 - 1:42
    look different in different individuals,
  • 1:42 - 1:45
    because it will vary in terms of a
  • 1:45 - 1:47
    students age, verbal abilities, motor
  • 1:47 - 1:51
    abilities, working memory, that short term
  • 1:51 - 1:53
    memory that helps you keep information in
  • 1:53 - 1:56
    mind, and individual students learning
  • 1:56 - 2:01
    strengths; and also, temperament. I'm not
  • 2:01 - 2:04
    talking in detail about testing here, but
  • 2:04 - 2:06
    I wanted to introduce some of the common
  • 2:06 - 2:08
    questions that people have. Testing for
  • 2:08 - 2:11
    Dyslexia is called a clinical diagnosis,
  • 2:11 - 2:15
    so it can't be done definitively with a
  • 2:15 - 2:17
    checklist. It really needs to be done,
  • 2:17 - 2:19
    ideally, with comprehensive testing with
  • 2:19 - 2:24
    a professional who has a great deal of
  • 2:24 - 2:26
    experience with the variations that can
  • 2:26 - 2:28
    occur with Dyslexia. There is some debate
  • 2:28 - 2:30
    that exists in terms of the best method,
  • 2:30 - 2:32
    our preference, as I mentioned, is with
  • 2:32 - 2:36
    comprehensive testing. Usually that
  • 2:36 - 2:39
    entails some screening of vision, hearing,
  • 2:39 - 2:43
    and motor ability, but also, standardized
  • 2:43 - 2:50
    tests we like to use: a combination of IQ
  • 2:50 - 2:53
    tests such as the WISC-IV, or the WIAS-IV
  • 2:53 - 2:56
    for adults. These are standardized tests
  • 2:56 - 2:59
    that involve estimates of verbal and
  • 2:59 - 3:01
    non-verbal reasoning, working memory and
  • 3:01 - 3:04
    processing speed; in addition, there are
  • 3:04 - 3:07
    so-called achievement tests which look at
  • 3:07 - 3:11
    performance, for instance, on phonology,
  • 3:11 - 3:14
    on single-word reading, on passage
  • 3:14 - 3:18
    reading, listening. There are other tests
  • 3:18 - 3:20
    that are mentioned here, Nelson Denny is a
  • 3:20 - 3:22
    particular test that is helpful when
  • 3:22 - 3:23
    students are applying to the College
  • 3:23 - 3:25
    Board for accommodations because those
  • 3:25 - 3:28
    passages in the Nelson Denny reading tests
  • 3:28 - 3:31
    are really better indicators of challenges
  • 3:31 - 3:34
    that a student may have with college or
  • 3:34 - 3:38
    graduate school level reading passages.
  • 3:38 - 3:40
    Other things mentioned here are CTOPP, and
  • 3:40 - 3:43
    some of the sub-tests listed: listening,
  • 3:43 - 3:45
    comprehension, single word reading,
  • 3:45 - 3:47
    reading comprehension, writing, spelling,
  • 3:47 - 3:50
    phonology, math. In addition, we like to
  • 3:50 - 3:52
    make sure that we are not just focusing
  • 3:52 - 3:55
    on a students weaknesses, we like to find
  • 3:55 - 3:58
    out what a student likes to do in their
  • 3:58 - 4:00
    spare time, what they may be particularly
  • 4:00 - 4:03
    talented in doing; we evaluate creative
  • 4:03 - 4:05
    work and out-of-school activities. In
  • 4:05 - 4:10
    general, as much as possible, this should
  • 4:10 - 4:14
    be part of the basic assessment of
  • 4:14 - 4:16
    students with Dyslexia. We shouldn't just
  • 4:16 - 4:18
    focus on the weaknesses because that gives
  • 4:18 - 4:20
    a very lopsided view of a student:
  • 4:20 - 4:23
    where they are right now, as well as their
  • 4:23 - 4:24
    potential in the future.
  • 4:30 - 4:34
    So the hallmark of Dyslexia testing when
  • 4:34 - 4:37
    using these methods of ability-achievement
  • 4:37 - 4:41
    discrepancy is to look at how well a
  • 4:41 - 4:43
    student may perform with thinking skills,
  • 4:43 - 4:45
    reasoning skills that are communicated
  • 4:45 - 4:47
    verbally, or non-verbally with puzzles
  • 4:47 - 4:51
    or complex shapes or patterns that are
  • 4:51 - 4:54
    presented in front of them, compared to
  • 4:54 - 4:56
    for instance, written work or
  • 4:56 - 5:01
    calculations. That ability-achievement
  • 5:01 - 5:03
    discrepancy is the foundation for
  • 5:03 - 5:05
    understanding specific learning
  • 5:05 - 5:10
    disabilities, meaning that intellectual
  • 5:10 - 5:13
    ability may be high, but there are certain
  • 5:13 - 5:16
    patterns of specific weaknesses that are
  • 5:16 - 5:19
    consistent with a specific learning
  • 5:19 - 5:21
    disability.
  • 5:25 - 5:30
    So, if we look at how Dyslexic students
  • 5:30 - 5:33
    perform when they have a battery of
  • 5:33 - 5:38
    tests such as this; here, on the Y axis
  • 5:38 - 5:43
    are the scores that you might see in tests
  • 5:43 - 5:45
    that are standardly done as part
  • 5:45 - 5:48
    of the WISC test: this is an example of
  • 5:48 - 5:52
    a student from our clinic. If average is
  • 5:52 - 5:58
    10 for age, then these scores,
  • 5:58 - 6:01
    similarities --that's a measure of
  • 6:01 - 6:05
    analogical reasoning-- things like
  • 6:05 - 6:07
    comprehension might be extremely strong.
  • 6:07 - 6:09
    Here, this student was very high in block
  • 6:09 - 6:12
    design. So being able to perceive and
  • 6:12 - 6:15
    replicate different spatial patterns,
  • 6:15 - 6:19
    these are all above the average of 10.
  • 6:19 - 6:22
    Here, these other measures, things like
  • 6:22 - 6:24
    how much information can be kept in mind
  • 6:24 - 6:27
    at one time, a digit span, numbering,
  • 6:27 - 6:30
    keeping numbers and letters in order,
  • 6:30 - 6:34
    and coding--being able to copy different
  • 6:34 - 6:37
    symbols in a rapid fashion, are below
  • 6:37 - 6:39
    average. In this student's case,
  • 6:39 - 6:42
    listening comprehension was at the
  • 6:42 - 6:44
    98th %tile, single word reading was at the
  • 6:44 - 6:48
    8th, pseudoword reading was at the
  • 6:48 - 6:50
    3rd %tile--that's a measure of
  • 6:50 - 6:52
    phonological awareness, and alphabet
  • 6:52 - 6:54
    fluency-- how quickly you can actually
  • 6:54 - 6:56
    name letters of the alphabet when presented
  • 6:56 - 7:00
    in front of you, was at the 6th. The thing
  • 7:00 - 7:02
    that I wanted to get across to you here
  • 7:02 - 7:04
    is that the Dyslexic picture is really one
  • 7:04 - 7:08
    of peaks and valleys, it's not a flat line.
  • 7:12 - 7:14
    To look at....these are some additional
  • 7:14 - 7:17
    sub-tests here, to look at some of the
  • 7:17 - 7:21
    WIAT achievement scores here, again,
  • 7:21 - 7:24
    look at 100 here representing standard
  • 7:24 - 7:28
    scores, we had in our clinic students of
  • 7:28 - 7:31
    average verbal IQ, Superior, Very
  • 7:31 - 7:35
    Superior, and even above that. Again,
  • 7:35 - 7:37
    what I want to convey to you mainly is
  • 7:37 - 7:40
    the picture--if this is the average range,
  • 7:40 - 7:42
    what you don't see, is a flat line, which
  • 7:42 - 7:44
    is more typically the case for
  • 7:44 - 7:46
    non-Dyslexic students, but instead you
  • 7:46 - 7:48
    have Dyslexic students who really have
  • 7:48 - 7:50
    extreme of abilities, and also, very
  • 7:50 - 7:54
    specific weaknesses. Here, this is Oral
  • 7:54 - 7:56
    Reading Accuracy, that contribute to the
  • 7:56 - 7:59
    general reading picture. If we were to
  • 7:59 - 8:01
    look only at the weaknesses, we are not
  • 8:01 - 8:03
    really seeing the tremendous potential
  • 8:03 - 8:06
    that exists among these students.
  • 8:08 - 8:10
    So now, I am going to briefly go through
  • 8:10 - 8:12
    the different ages. In elementary school,
  • 8:12 - 8:17
    typically, Dyslexic students may present
  • 8:17 - 8:19
    with some of the challenges on the left
  • 8:19 - 8:22
    column here. Some students may be late
  • 8:22 - 8:26
    talking, others, parents often say that
  • 8:26 - 8:28
    they notice they seem to be slower with
  • 8:28 - 8:30
    letter or other types of naming. They may
  • 8:30 - 8:33
    have a low interest in books, mild
  • 8:33 - 8:35
    mispronunciation errors might be seen.
  • 8:35 - 8:38
    Things like "back-back" instead of
  • 8:38 - 8:41
    "backpack" , "pisghetti" instead of
  • 8:41 - 8:45
    "spaghetti", these are mispronunciations
  • 8:45 - 8:48
    that are not just motor, motorically being
  • 8:48 - 8:51
    able to pronounce the words, but more
  • 8:51 - 8:54
    representative of a general issue with
  • 8:54 - 8:58
    auditory processing and phonology. Often,
  • 8:58 - 9:01
    Dyslexic students don't 'get' rhymes.
  • 9:01 - 9:03
    It's because they are not hearing the same
  • 9:03 - 9:05
    things as non-Dyslexic students are
  • 9:05 - 9:07
    hearing. They may be slower in learning
  • 9:07 - 9:10
    to read, have difficulty remembering their
  • 9:10 - 9:13
    math facts, and also have some right-left
  • 9:13 - 9:16
    confusion. This is an example of writing
  • 9:16 - 9:19
    of a student who was told to write lower
  • 9:19 - 9:20
    case letters. They had a lot of
  • 9:20 - 9:23
    intrusions, had difficulty knowing what
  • 9:23 - 9:26
    direction to write the letters. At the
  • 9:26 - 9:28
    same time, in the early elementary
  • 9:28 - 9:30
    years, these students may actually be
  • 9:30 - 9:32
    very socially adept, they may show
  • 9:32 - 9:33
    themselves to be excellent problem
  • 9:33 - 9:37
    solvers, be fantastic, for instance, at
  • 9:37 - 9:41
    real life problem-solving, asking good
  • 9:41 - 9:44
    questions, and, for instance, show
  • 9:44 - 9:46
    themselves in science experiments in
  • 9:46 - 9:49
    class. Some of these students are very
  • 9:49 - 9:51
    expressive non-verbally, they might be
  • 9:51 - 9:54
    wonderful builders, great with Legos,
  • 9:54 - 9:57
    K'nex, things like that, or-- are very
  • 9:57 - 9:59
    artistic. These students are often
  • 9:59 - 10:02
    creative. They have lots of ideas, and
  • 10:02 - 10:05
    they may hide their difficulties.
  • 10:12 - 10:16
    In late elementary school, the
  • 10:16 - 10:19
    difficulties change a little bit. You
  • 10:19 - 10:21
    still have some reading difficulties, of
  • 10:21 - 10:24
    course, but writing also takes on greater
  • 10:24 - 10:26
    importance. Examples here: this is a
  • 10:26 - 10:28
    student who wrote, "I thought I could
  • 10:28 - 10:31
    climb the mountain." This student had a
  • 10:31 - 10:33
    great deal of auditory processing
  • 10:33 - 10:36
    difficulties, and as a result, "could",
  • 10:36 - 10:40
    "croud", was not very clearly heard, and
  • 10:40 - 10:45
    as a result, an astute observer could
  • 10:45 - 10:47
    pick up the writing difficulties were
  • 10:47 - 10:49
    really a reflection of auditory
  • 10:49 - 10:51
    processing difficulties that the student
  • 10:51 - 10:54
    was having. The challenges that late
  • 10:54 - 10:57
    elementary school students face include
  • 10:57 - 11:00
    spelling problems, they have often weaker
  • 11:00 - 11:02
    visual word form, grammatical
  • 11:02 - 11:04
    difficulties; writing in general may be
  • 11:04 - 11:07
    disorganized and slow, it may be hard to
  • 11:07 - 11:10
    get ideas down onto paper. Word finding
  • 11:10 - 11:12
    is another issue. Some of these students
  • 11:12 - 11:16
    have actually vivid sensory perceptions
  • 11:16 - 11:18
    and visual imagery, and it's hard for them
  • 11:18 - 11:23
    to translate these perceptions into words.
  • 11:23 - 11:25
    Rote memory also comes online as late
  • 11:25 - 11:27
    elementary school students are expected
  • 11:27 - 11:29
    to have more facts at their disposal,
  • 11:29 - 11:33
    especially if the memorization involves
  • 11:33 - 11:35
    lists of information that are not linked
  • 11:35 - 11:38
    by a story: for example, things like state
  • 11:38 - 11:40
    capitals. It can seem almost impossible to
  • 11:40 - 11:44
    some students.At the same time, we also
  • 11:44 - 11:47
    start seeing some real wonderful spatial
  • 11:47 - 11:49
    talents in students in the late elementary
  • 11:49 - 11:51
    school years. The science and math
  • 11:51 - 11:55
    students also become prominent, and some
  • 11:55 - 11:57
    students find they really excel in sports
  • 11:57 - 12:02
    and take on leadership positions. These
  • 12:02 - 12:03
    are students who are often socially adept.
  • 12:03 - 12:06
    There is strong empathy. They are great
  • 12:06 - 12:08
    team players. And the arts, as mentioned,
  • 12:08 - 12:11
    also, clusters of strengths that we see
  • 12:11 - 12:12
    in students.
  • 12:15 - 12:18
    In the Middle and High School years`
  • 12:18 - 12:20
    what is particularly a challenge is the
  • 12:20 - 12:23
    quantity of reading and writing. It's at
  • 12:23 - 12:26
    this time where students have to write a
  • 12:26 - 12:30
    great deal more than previously. The
  • 12:30 - 12:33
    misunderstanding of syntax, the structure
  • 12:33 - 12:37
    of sentences, and limitations in
  • 12:37 - 12:41
    vocabulary really can make schooling
  • 12:41 - 12:44
    difficult. Standardized tests may also be
  • 12:44 - 12:46
    difficult. Students may actually follow
  • 12:46 - 12:49
    quite well in the classroom, but then be
  • 12:49 - 12:50
    presented with information that is out of
  • 12:50 - 12:53
    context, like in a standardized exam, and
  • 12:53 - 12:57
    more subtle weaknesses than reading really
  • 12:57 - 13:00
    come to bear. As a result, standardized
  • 13:00 - 13:03
    test performance may be far below what a
  • 13:03 - 13:06
    student is doing in the regular classroom.
  • 13:06 - 13:09
    Other issues that are important for
  • 13:09 - 13:10
    middle and high school and college
  • 13:10 - 13:13
    students, things like the speed of being
  • 13:13 - 13:15
    able to complete work, misreading question
  • 13:15 - 13:18
    prompts. Foreign Language can be a
  • 13:18 - 13:21
    strength or a weakness for Dyslexic
  • 13:21 - 13:23
    students. The most typical pattern is that
  • 13:23 - 13:28
    students may actually fare fairly well by
  • 13:28 - 13:30
    listening and doing the conversational
  • 13:30 - 13:33
    aspects of language, but as Foreign
  • 13:33 - 13:35
    Language progresses in the school system,
  • 13:35 - 13:38
    and more conjugation and rote memory
  • 13:38 - 13:40
    rules of language and grammar are
  • 13:40 - 13:42
    required, then students have difficulty,
  • 13:42 - 13:45
    If that is a significant issue, or
  • 13:45 - 13:47
    students have fairly severe auditory
  • 13:47 - 13:49
    processing difficulties, they really have
  • 13:49 - 13:52
    trouble hearing the sounds, then we
  • 13:52 - 13:54
    usually support and recommend a Foreign
  • 13:54 - 13:58
    Language waiver. Middle, high school,
  • 13:58 - 14:01
    and college are also the time where word
  • 14:01 - 14:03
    heavy subjects like science, social
  • 14:03 - 14:06
    studies can also cause trouble for
  • 14:06 - 14:07
    students. The important thing I want to
  • 14:07 - 14:10
    mention here is that there is often a
  • 14:10 - 14:12
    discrepancy. Dyslexic students are often
  • 14:12 - 14:14
    quite strong with science, and scientific
  • 14:14 - 14:18
    thinking. It's only in that middle stage
  • 14:18 - 14:20
    where a lot of science vocabulary is
  • 14:20 - 14:23
    presented, where a student may suddenly
  • 14:23 - 14:25
    feel that they are not good at science.
  • 14:25 - 14:27
    It's important to look out for that,
  • 14:27 - 14:30
    because these students often just need
  • 14:30 - 14:32
    more encouragement, they might need more
  • 14:32 - 14:36
    repetition, and even some tutoring. But
  • 14:36 - 14:38
    often if they can get through this period
  • 14:38 - 14:40
    of time where the vocabulary and the
  • 14:40 - 14:45
    technical language of science is mastered,
  • 14:45 - 14:47
    and gapped to the point where you actually
  • 14:47 - 14:49
    have to think and critically analyze
  • 14:49 - 14:51
    observations, that's where Dyslexic
  • 14:51 - 14:53
    Students can really excel and do fantastic
  • 14:53 - 15:00
    work, even breakthrough work in research.
  • 15:00 - 15:02
    Organization is also a typical issue at
  • 15:02 - 15:06
    this age. That occurs for a number of
  • 15:06 - 15:09
    reasons. Sometimes the organizational
  • 15:09 - 15:14
    difficulties reflect inter-connected and
  • 15:14 - 15:18
    immersive thinking style, as a result, it
  • 15:18 - 15:22
    is hard to prioritize information and to
  • 15:22 - 15:25
    "chunk" it, categorize what they know,
  • 15:25 - 15:29
    and then put it in a sequence. I think in
  • 15:29 - 15:31
    another webinar what I might do is --
  • 15:31 - 15:34
    we can talk more about the executive
  • 15:34 - 15:36
    function organizational aspects of
  • 15:36 - 15:40
    Dyslexia. At the same time that we talk
  • 15:40 - 15:41
    about the challenges that occur at this
  • 15:41 - 15:44
    age, it's often really exciting to see
  • 15:44 - 15:48
    these students really start breaking out
  • 15:48 - 15:49
    of the pack in terms of their higher
  • 15:49 - 15:53
    order thinking. Often, students will
  • 15:53 - 15:57
    surprise their teachers with really
  • 15:57 - 16:02
    striking insights, different perspectives
  • 16:02 - 16:06
    on themes, or questions that are presented
  • 16:06 - 16:10
    in the classroom. Students can thrive in
  • 16:10 - 16:12
    project based learning and the arts,
  • 16:12 - 16:15
    verbal talents are often also noted at
  • 16:15 - 16:18
    this age. Some Dyslexic students actually
  • 16:18 - 16:22
    have a wonderful span, being able to keep
  • 16:22 - 16:24
    information in, and as a result they can
  • 16:24 - 16:28
    excel in things such as Drama or Debate,
  • 16:28 - 16:31
    because they can keep in long arguments
  • 16:31 - 16:33
    and what people have said, in their memory
  • 16:33 - 16:36
    banks, and then be able to analyze it and,
  • 16:36 - 16:38
    for instance, to counter arguments when
  • 16:38 - 16:44
    presenting in that kind of forum.
  • 16:44 - 16:48
    Leadership, also, is a very common talent
  • 16:48 - 16:50
    cluster among Dyslexic students. Often,
  • 16:50 - 16:55
    if a parents says, "I don't know what kind
  • 16:55 - 16:58
    of talent my student has, maybe there is
  • 16:58 - 17:02
    no specific talent or creative drive. If
  • 17:02 - 17:05
    we ask more questions, what we find out
  • 17:05 - 17:09
    is that these students, their strength may
  • 17:09 - 17:13
    be social. It's not that they prefer to be
  • 17:13 - 17:17
    in the science lab, or prefer to be doing
  • 17:17 - 17:20
    math equations, or physics; these students
  • 17:20 - 17:22
    really may seem to have no particular
  • 17:22 - 17:26
    special talents or gifts, but it's their
  • 17:26 - 17:28
    gift is actually social. What they really
  • 17:28 - 17:32
    are is making friends all over the school
  • 17:32 - 17:34
    with different ages, and that's going to
  • 17:34 - 17:38
    be a great talent in practically any
  • 17:38 - 17:39
    profession.
  • 17:39 - 17:41
    I also want to mention a bit about stealth
  • 17:41 - 17:45
    Dyslexics there. Stealth Dyslexics often
  • 17:45 - 17:48
    fly under the radar of
  • 17:48 - 17:52
    detection. Stealth Dyslexia in younger
  • 17:52 - 17:55
    children is usually seen in a setting of
  • 17:55 - 17:58
    gifted children who may be able to
  • 17:58 - 18:00
    compensate for some of their weaknesses
  • 18:00 - 18:02
    but, as Dyslexia evolves in older
  • 18:02 - 18:06
    students, often they can crack the code
  • 18:06 - 18:08
    of reading for comprehension purposes.
  • 18:08 - 18:10
    They can often read silently with good
  • 18:10 - 18:14
    comprehension, but their challenges come
  • 18:14 - 18:19
    out more with oral reading accuracy, and
  • 18:19 - 18:21
    with speed of processing, and with
  • 18:21 - 18:25
    writing. Organizational issues also
  • 18:25 - 18:27
    contribute to some of the writing
  • 18:27 - 18:29
    difficulties with Stealth Dyslexia. It's
  • 18:29 - 18:31
    another big topic, to talk about Stealth
  • 18:31 - 18:36
    Dyslexia, so maybe we can put that off
  • 18:36 - 18:39
    for another webinar if there is interest.
  • 18:42 - 18:45
    College and the Workplace: this is really
  • 18:45 - 18:48
    a new field, a lot...a new area.
  • 18:48 - 18:52
    There has been a lot of discussion about
  • 18:52 - 18:55
    the early years of Dyslexia, but, I think
  • 18:55 - 19:00
    it is going to be really an explosion of
  • 19:00 - 19:03
    understanding about Dyslexia in the
  • 19:03 - 19:06
    workplace in the coming decade. As much
  • 19:06 - 19:08
    in, for college, it is the quantity of
  • 19:08 - 19:10
    reading and writing that can be
  • 19:10 - 19:12
    particularly tough, we typically see
  • 19:12 - 19:13
    students in the first two years of college
  • 19:13 - 19:15
    have their greatest struggles. Sometimes
  • 19:15 - 19:17
    students will try a little bit of college,
  • 19:17 - 19:19
    really have trouble, and then have to
  • 19:19 - 19:23
    step out, try again, and then over the
  • 19:23 - 19:26
    course of some time, the brain matures
  • 19:26 - 19:28
    a little bit more, and then they can
  • 19:28 - 19:31
    actually balance schooling when they are
  • 19:31 - 19:33
    a little bit older. Another thing that we
  • 19:33 - 19:35
    see is that, although the first 2 years are
  • 19:35 - 19:37
    the toughest because there is a lot of
  • 19:37 - 19:39
    required reading, and a lot more rote
  • 19:39 - 19:42
    information...Dyslexic students, if they
  • 19:42 - 19:46
    can "push their way through" those first 2
  • 19:46 - 19:50
    years, they may really find the upper two
  • 19:50 - 19:54
    years quite a bit easier as they are used
  • 19:54 - 19:58
    to deep thinking, and analysis, and the
  • 19:58 - 20:01
    thinking part of college, where they work,
  • 20:01 - 20:04
    is not hard for most Dyslexic students.
  • 20:04 - 20:07
    It's really just the rote memorization
  • 20:07 - 20:09
    work that is required in the early years,
  • 20:09 - 20:12
    and the sheer quantity of books that need
  • 20:12 - 20:13
    to be read.
  • 20:13 - 20:15
    Time management: a big issue for many
  • 20:15 - 20:17
    students in the college years. Getting
  • 20:17 - 20:19
    through their major requirements, that's
  • 20:19 - 20:21
    often, as I said, the worst part. If you
  • 20:21 - 20:25
    can request reduced courseload, that's
  • 20:25 - 20:28
    very helpful for many students. Many
  • 20:28 - 20:30
    students also find they need to break
  • 20:30 - 20:33
    out their school schedule so that they
  • 20:33 - 20:36
    take some classes over summer.
  • 20:36 - 20:38
    In the workplace, the things that are the
  • 20:38 - 20:41
    most difficult for young Dyslexics are
  • 20:41 - 20:47
    tasks that require repetitive, and say for
  • 20:47 - 20:51
    instance, data entry, clerical tasks,
  • 20:51 - 20:55
    that's kind of like that fine detail that
  • 20:55 - 20:59
    is difficult for many Dyslexics. Filling
  • 20:59 - 21:01
    out forms, because information is not
  • 21:01 - 21:06
    contextual...questions that also rise,
  • 21:06 - 21:09
    should I disclose my Dyslexia in the
  • 21:09 - 21:12
    workplace? It can really be an important
  • 21:12 - 21:15
    issue for many people.
  • 21:15 - 21:16
    So at the same time there are really
  • 21:16 - 21:19
    significant challenges at the college
  • 21:19 - 21:23
    level and in the workplace, as said,
  • 21:23 - 21:25
    in higher education, and in careers, this
  • 21:25 - 21:28
    is often where Dyslexics shine because
  • 21:28 - 21:31
    they are fantastic problem solvers,
  • 21:31 - 21:33
    whether it's people problems, system
  • 21:33 - 21:36
    problems, or technical problems. Dyslexic
  • 21:36 - 21:38
    strengths often involve strategic
  • 21:38 - 21:41
    thinking, many Dyslexics are strong with
  • 21:41 - 21:46
    empathy and seem particularly good at team
  • 21:46 - 21:49
    building. Dynamic Reasoning, which is one
  • 21:49 - 21:51
    of the mind strengths, is something that
  • 21:51 - 21:55
    we also seem to see more often sort of
  • 21:55 - 21:59
    "turning on" in the college graduate
  • 21:59 - 22:01
    school or workplace years. Being able to
  • 22:01 - 22:04
    spot trends and predict what's going to
  • 22:04 - 22:05
    happen, which is important for so many
  • 22:05 - 22:08
    different cutting edge careers.
  • 22:08 - 22:11
    Entrepreneurship, Dyslexics, as many of
  • 22:11 - 22:13
    you many know, really excel as
  • 22:13 - 22:17
    entrepreneurs. They make up 35-40% of the
  • 22:17 - 22:19
    successful entrepreneurs here in the
  • 22:19 - 22:21
    United States. So, really highly
  • 22:21 - 22:24
    over-represented among successful
  • 22:24 - 22:27
    entrepreneurs compared to the general
  • 22:27 - 22:29
    population.
  • 22:31 - 22:33
    And what about older Dyslexic adults?
  • 22:33 - 22:37
    It's important to be aware if either you
  • 22:37 - 22:41
    or a friend or family member is suspected
  • 22:41 - 22:43
    to have Dyslexia,that...if you are over
  • 22:43 - 22:46
    the age of 40, the chances are, you were
  • 22:46 - 22:50
    never formally identified. Part of that
  • 22:50 - 22:53
    reason is because at that time the
  • 22:53 - 22:56
    federal legislation actually hadn't taken
  • 22:56 - 23:00
    place, so there was no pressure to
  • 23:00 - 23:03
    identify Dyslexia, it wasn't identified
  • 23:03 - 23:07
    in the school system yet. Really, the
  • 23:07 - 23:09
    legislation only took effect for people
  • 23:09 - 23:13
    who are now younger than the age of 40.
  • 23:13 - 23:18
    Many older Dyslexic adults got by--
  • 23:18 - 23:20
    succeeded--with personal strengths,
  • 23:20 - 23:22
    with people talents, as mentioned before,
  • 23:22 - 23:25
    strategic thinking and problems solving,
  • 23:25 - 23:26
    and they found that "real life" was easier
  • 23:26 - 23:31
    than school. When we polled our community
  • 23:31 - 23:33
    here at Dyslexic Advantage, many people
  • 23:33 - 23:34
    said that people supports were very
  • 23:34 - 23:37
    important, technology less so, but, that
  • 23:37 - 23:40
    soon will be changing. In answer to the
  • 23:40 - 23:43
    question, "Am I still Dyslexic?"---yes,
  • 23:43 - 23:45
    you are! It's a brain wiring difference.
  • 23:45 - 23:47
    Most Dylsexic adults are able to read and
  • 23:47 - 23:50
    understand, but their reading may be slow,
  • 23:50 - 23:52
    and writing and spelling even more
  • 23:52 - 23:54
    challenging than reading...that's what
  • 23:54 - 23:56
    most people tell us.
  • 23:58 - 24:01
    In answer to the question about the brain
  • 24:01 - 24:03
    science of Dyslexia, "Are Dyslexic brains
  • 24:03 - 24:06
    different?"---Yes, they are. Here is an
  • 24:06 - 24:09
    example from Manuel Casanova, a
  • 24:09 - 24:14
    neuroscientist here represented the
  • 24:14 - 24:17
    different brain volumes in Dyslexics versus
  • 24:17 - 24:22
    non-Dyslexics in terms of color. There
  • 24:22 - 24:24
    were larger right hemispheric brain
  • 24:24 - 24:26
    volumes among Dyslexics compared to
  • 24:26 - 24:32
    non-Dyslexics. Some of that....I think
  • 24:32 - 24:34
    this might be helpful in giving you an
  • 24:34 - 24:37
    idea of what the differences are. If you
  • 24:37 - 24:39
    process more information in the right
  • 24:39 - 24:41
    hemisphere, versus the left hemisphere,
  • 24:41 - 24:46
    this is an example from some really
  • 24:46 - 24:48
    fascinating split brain studies from
  • 24:48 - 24:53
    Sperry and Gazzaniga. These experiments
  • 24:53 - 24:55
    were not done on Dyslexic or non-Dyslexic
  • 24:55 - 24:59
    subjects...they were not done on Dyslexic
  • 24:59 - 25:04
    subjects. Instead,patients who were under
  • 25:04 - 25:07
    going a procedure which actually split the
  • 25:07 - 25:10
    hemispheres. So what it told us was what
  • 25:10 - 25:12
    do the respective hemispheres do when they
  • 25:12 - 25:16
    process language. For example, when one
  • 25:16 - 25:18
    of these split-brained patients were
  • 25:18 - 25:23
    flashed the word "knight" on a screen, if
  • 25:23 - 25:28
    it was flashed on the screen in a way that
  • 25:28 - 25:31
    the left hemisphere saw the word, then the
  • 25:31 - 25:34
    person said, "It says knight." But
  • 25:34 - 25:36
    interestingly, if it was flashed on the
  • 25:36 - 25:39
    screen so that the right hemisphere saw
  • 25:39 - 25:43
    the word, the person said this, " I have a
  • 25:43 - 25:45
    picture in my mind, but I can't say it.
  • 25:45 - 25:48
    Two fighters in a ring, ancient, using
  • 25:48 - 25:50
    uniforms and helmets on horses, trying
  • 25:50 - 25:54
    to knock each other off....knights?" What
  • 25:54 - 25:58
    this reflects is, although language is
  • 25:58 - 26:00
    really represented on both sides of the
  • 26:00 - 26:03
    brain, depending on whether you have more
  • 26:03 - 26:06
    left sided processing or right sided
  • 26:06 - 26:08
    processing, how that information is
  • 26:08 - 26:13
    organized, stored ,and expressed is very
  • 26:13 - 26:16
    different. On the left hemisphere, the
  • 26:16 - 26:17
    language centers tend to focus on
  • 26:17 - 26:21
    specific, exact information. In the right
  • 26:21 - 26:25
    hemisphere, it's more associational. So
  • 26:25 - 26:27
    there are these complex images and stories
  • 26:27 - 26:30
    associated with a word. As a result, when
  • 26:30 - 26:33
    students are say, in their middle or their
  • 26:33 - 26:35
    high school years, and you know they know
  • 26:35 - 26:37
    so much information. You've asked them to
  • 26:37 - 26:41
    write about it. Sometimes what you see is
  • 26:41 - 26:45
    really this kind of "complex perception"
  • 26:45 - 26:48
    that is really hard to put into words.
  • 26:48 - 26:50
    These are students, if you question them,
  • 26:50 - 26:52
    they know a lot about the topic, and yet
  • 26:52 - 26:53
    there are very few words they have written
  • 26:53 - 26:57
    on the page. Again, I think it's just a
  • 26:57 - 27:02
    great example that really can inform more
  • 27:02 - 27:04
    understanding of why writing is so
  • 27:04 - 27:07
    difficult for many students. There are
  • 27:07 - 27:10
    specific ways to actually encourage
  • 27:10 - 27:12
    elaboration. Translate pictures into words,
  • 27:12 - 27:15
    for instance, that particularly can work
  • 27:15 - 27:18
    well with many Dyslexic students.
  • 27:20 - 27:25
    So, I think we'll probably have a special
  • 27:25 - 27:28
    session on some of the brain research
  • 27:28 - 27:30
    studies in Dyslexia, because it's
  • 27:30 - 27:34
    fascinating and I think a lot of you would
  • 27:34 - 27:39
    have great discussion of some of the
  • 27:39 - 27:44
    observations. We really want to encourage
  • 27:44 - 27:48
    more sharing between Dyslexic populations
  • 27:48 - 27:51
    and the researchers who are trying to
  • 27:51 - 27:53
    study Dyslexia. I think it is very
  • 27:53 - 27:56
    important, and will help things all the
  • 27:56 - 28:00
    way around. A theory or a model that is
  • 28:00 - 28:02
    helpful to think about, what the balance
  • 28:02 - 28:06
    is in Dyslexia is this fine detail and big
  • 28:06 - 28:10
    picture aspect of Dyslexia. Dyslexia as
  • 28:10 - 28:15
    a trade-off is a trade-off for more big
  • 28:15 - 28:18
    picture thinking at the expense of fine
  • 28:18 - 28:21
    detail. Here we have a quote form Manuel
  • 28:21 - 28:24
    Casanova, he said, "Dyslexia is a pattern
  • 28:24 - 28:26
    of connectivity that favors longer
  • 28:26 - 28:28
    connections at the expense of shorter
  • 28:28 - 28:31
    ones, as measured by spaces between cell
  • 28:31 - 28:34
    minicolumns. This pattern of connectivity
  • 28:34 - 28:36
    may yield a greater capacity for abstract,
  • 28:36 - 28:39
    'visionary' thinking".
  • 28:39 - 28:42
    What Dr. Casanova has found is that if
  • 28:42 - 28:46
    white matter connections are measured in
  • 28:46 - 28:48
    Dyslexics versus non-Dyslexics, there are
  • 28:48 - 28:54
    clear differences. In fact, the pattern
  • 28:54 - 28:57
    between these connections looks like
  • 28:57 - 29:01
    there is kind of extremes, where, if there
  • 29:01 - 29:03
    is a normal distribution in the middle,
  • 29:03 - 29:06
    at one extreme favoring the fine detail
  • 29:06 - 29:10
    are autistic individuals. And at the other
  • 29:10 - 29:12
    end, favoring big picture, are Dyslexic
  • 29:12 - 29:16
    individuals. The picture over here on the
  • 29:16 - 29:20
    left, shows this trade-off between autism
  • 29:20 - 29:23
    and Dyslexia. Here, Dyslexia patterns
  • 29:23 - 29:26
    with these long projectios and pathways
  • 29:26 - 29:28
    in the frame, favor these top-down
  • 29:28 - 29:30
    cognitive style. What is the big picture?
  • 29:30 - 29:32
    What is the information we need to know.
  • 29:32 - 29:34
    What's the context. It tends to be more
  • 29:34 - 29:38
    holistic and gestault in terms of
  • 29:38 - 29:43
    processing, and good synthesizing that
  • 29:43 - 29:45
    comes as a result of this.
  • 29:47 - 29:49
    If you look at the fine detal/ big picture
  • 29:49 - 29:55
    trade-off, in other practical clinical
  • 29:55 - 29:58
    academic situations, the fine detail
  • 29:58 - 30:00
    picture are things like specific facts
  • 30:00 - 30:02
    where you are trying to master knowledge
  • 30:02 - 30:04
    in isolation. You are trying to learn
  • 30:04 - 30:07
    rules and procedures and information
  • 30:07 - 30:09
    that is supposed to be predictable,
  • 30:09 - 30:14
    orderly, and for instance, rote. At the
  • 30:14 - 30:18
    other end, the Dyslexic style is more big
  • 30:18 - 30:22
    picture. It's "gist". It's conceptual.
  • 30:22 - 30:26
    This kind of pattern works well for best
  • 30:26 - 30:29
    fit; looking at themes , patterns ,and
  • 30:29 - 30:32
    stories; information that is personal and
  • 30:32 - 30:35
    presented in context, and has associations
  • 30:35 - 30:38
    and relationships. You can imagine that
  • 30:38 - 30:40
    this kind of big picture style is so
  • 30:40 - 30:42
    helpful in many disciplines,
  • 30:42 - 30:45
    where you are trying to look at real-life
  • 30:45 - 30:50
    trends or real-life problems. You can also
  • 30:50 - 30:53
    imagine that in early elementary school,
  • 30:53 - 30:57
    where fine detail and rules and procedures
  • 30:57 - 31:00
    are being mastered, that's a particular
  • 31:00 - 31:04
    time that is difficult for many Dyslexics.
  • 31:04 - 31:06
    Going back to big picture: experiences,
  • 31:06 - 31:11
    kinesthetic learning, also contribute to
  • 31:11 - 31:12
    this big picture understanding of
  • 31:12 - 31:16
    information. Big Picture thinking is often
  • 31:16 - 31:18
    non-sequential, it can be messy because
  • 31:18 - 31:20
    you are really looking at the "best fit",
  • 31:20 - 31:22
    it doesn't have to be exact. Surprise and
  • 31:22 - 31:25
    Novelty are often really important for
  • 31:25 - 31:27
    Big Picture because it might cause you to
  • 31:27 - 31:31
    completely re-set your picture and your
  • 31:31 - 31:34
    perspective. This pattern is thought to be
  • 31:34 - 31:38
    more intuitive, rather than explicit.
  • 31:41 - 31:44
    If you look at this trade-off between Fine
  • 31:44 - 31:47
    Detail and Big Picture, the short
  • 31:47 - 31:49
    connection type processes in the brain
  • 31:49 - 31:50
    are things like Phonological
  • 31:50 - 31:52
    discrimination. What is the difference
  • 31:52 - 31:56
    between "ba" and "da"? Was it "bat" or
  • 31:56 - 32:01
    "dat"? Reading has a lot of fine detail
  • 32:01 - 32:04
    associated with it. Is there an "s" on the
  • 32:04 - 32:06
    end of the word or is there not? Again,
  • 32:06 - 32:08
    rote memory, information that could be
  • 32:08 - 32:11
    quickly processed and more automatic.
  • 32:11 - 32:14
    On the other side, the kinds of tasks that
  • 32:14 - 32:16
    are important for long connections in the
  • 32:16 - 32:19
    brain are things like analogical thinking;
  • 32:19 - 32:23
    how is this like that? Spatial reasoning,
  • 32:23 - 32:27
    which requires often different...how
  • 32:27 - 32:30
    would that object be rotated in space?
  • 32:30 - 32:32
    Engineers are very good at things like
  • 32:32 - 32:35
    that. Mental Simulation, again, not
  • 32:35 - 32:38
    exact but actually approximate. Big
  • 32:38 - 32:41
    picture processes are often slower, and
  • 32:41 - 32:46
    as mentioned, less automatic.
  • 32:46 - 32:47
    If you look at the fine detail challenges
  • 32:47 - 32:49
    in younger students, what are the sorts of
  • 32:49 - 32:53
    things that you'd see? -- Is that the
  • 32:53 - 32:58
    letter 'b' or 'd'? Did you say 'wold' or
  • 32:58 - 33:03
    'world'? Was 4 plus 4 eight, or seven, or
  • 33:03 - 33:06
    six? --These pieces of information are
  • 33:06 - 33:10
    very exact and specific short connection
  • 33:10 - 33:15
    type of tasks. Letter recognition, sound
  • 33:15 - 33:17
    discrimination, fact retrievals,
  • 33:17 - 33:22
    sequencing. What are the fine detail
  • 33:22 - 33:24
    challenges that older students face?
  • 33:24 - 33:25
    Things like homonyms-- was that 'hear',
  • 33:25 - 33:30
    or 'here'? Choosed or chose? You'll hear
  • 33:30 - 33:31
    students say, "I know what that means,
  • 33:31 - 33:33
    but I can't explain why, or I can't
  • 33:33 - 33:39
    pronounce it. Was that punctuation rules
  • 33:39 - 33:41
    that's a fine detail? Was it studies with
  • 33:41 - 33:44
    an apostrophe 's' or with an '-ies'?"
  • 33:48 - 33:51
    Examples of fine detail challenges have
  • 33:51 - 33:54
    been studied in brain systems. These are
  • 33:54 - 33:56
    from fMRI studies. A famous study by
  • 33:56 - 34:00
    John Gabrieli at Stanford, and what it
  • 34:00 - 34:02
    shows you is that in typically reading
  • 34:02 - 34:03
    children, when they were listening to a
  • 34:03 - 34:06
    rhyme, there is activation in these two
  • 34:06 - 34:10
    circles. When Dyslexic students were
  • 34:10 - 34:11
    given the same rhymes--this was an
  • 34:11 - 34:13
    interesting study because the students
  • 34:13 - 34:16
    didn't have to make a decision about
  • 34:16 - 34:17
    anything, they were just listening to
  • 34:17 - 34:19
    rhyming words in the scanner. You can
  • 34:19 - 34:22
    see, it is much less of a match betwe4en
  • 34:22 - 34:25
    those. But when students were remediated,
  • 34:25 - 34:27
    with a variety of methods--some was
  • 34:27 - 34:30
    auditory training, another was a
  • 34:30 - 34:34
    phonological method--that, improvement in
  • 34:34 - 34:36
    that sound discrimination could be seen on
  • 34:36 - 34:38
    an fMRI, and that correlated with the
  • 34:38 - 34:41
    tests that they gave the children. It's
  • 34:41 - 34:44
    important to note that these kinds of
  • 34:44 - 34:49
    tests are not useful for diagnosis, at
  • 34:49 - 34:51
    that point there is a lot of individual
  • 34:51 - 34:53
    variations. Mostly what can be gleaned,
  • 34:53 - 34:55
    at least at this point in time, come from
  • 34:55 - 34:59
    groups of students with controls, rather
  • 34:59 - 35:03
    than relying on a single person or so.
  • 35:03 - 35:05
    It's not as if you could go and have a
  • 35:05 - 35:07
    scan and find out that you are Dyslexic.
  • 35:07 - 35:09
    It's still a clinical diagnosis or
  • 35:09 - 35:11
    identification.
  • 35:11 - 35:12
    Another thing I wanted to mention here,
  • 35:12 - 35:14
    because we talked a bit about Stealth
  • 35:14 - 35:16
    Dyslexia. This is some fascinating work
  • 35:16 - 35:20
    from Fumiko Hoeft. What she found when
  • 35:20 - 35:23
    she studied Stealth Dyslexics was that
  • 35:23 - 35:27
    there was more brain grey matter seen in
  • 35:27 - 35:30
    Stealth Dyslexics, as well as more
  • 35:30 - 35:33
    activation in areas associated with
  • 35:33 - 35:35
    contextual learning. So, these are
  • 35:35 - 35:38
    students who had trouble with single word
  • 35:38 - 35:40
    reading, but were actually quite strong in
  • 35:40 - 35:45
    reading words within a story or a context.
  • 35:45 - 35:48
    The interesting part about this is that
  • 35:48 - 35:51
    the extra brain grey matter in these
  • 35:51 - 35:54
    context areas is consistent with the
  • 35:54 - 35:59
    strength that certainly we see among
  • 35:59 - 36:02
    Stealth Dyslexics, about contextual
  • 36:02 - 36:08
    learning, and being able to function well
  • 36:08 - 36:10
    in applied problem solving.
  • 36:14 - 36:18
    In fact, these slides were taken from
  • 36:18 - 36:20
    Dr. Hoefts presentation at our conference
  • 36:20 - 36:23
    on Dyslexia and Talent, she challenged
  • 36:23 - 36:26
    the concept of Dyslexia being deficit,
  • 36:26 - 36:30
    only, as she presented her work. I highly
  • 36:30 - 36:33
    recommend checking out the videos from our
  • 36:33 - 36:35
    conference on our You-tube channel,
  • 36:35 - 36:44
    youtube.com/dyslexicadvantage. Her talk
  • 36:44 - 36:49
    there showed that Stealth Dyslexics, as
  • 36:49 - 36:52
    much, have that contextual top-down
  • 36:52 - 36:56
    processing, being able to use brain
  • 36:56 - 37:02
    pathways in order to do decoding and
  • 37:02 - 37:05
    problem solving. She also presented and
  • 37:05 - 37:08
    showed us some work from Ken Pugh at the
  • 37:08 - 37:11
    Haskins Lab at Yale that showed that
  • 37:11 - 37:13
    there seems to be a trade off between
  • 37:13 - 37:16
    reading and visual spatial processing
  • 37:16 - 37:19
    ability. In fact, from that work, it looks
  • 37:19 - 37:23
    like the weaker the reading ability, the
  • 37:23 - 37:26
    stronger the visual spatial talent. Sort
  • 37:26 - 37:32
    of a fascinating result. It was building
  • 37:32 - 37:34
    on some previous work that showed that
  • 37:34 - 37:37
    Dyslexics outperformed non-Dyslexics in
  • 37:37 - 37:41
    terms of spatial rotation and problem
  • 37:41 - 37:45
    solving types of work. That might account
  • 37:45 - 37:48
    for why Dyslexics are over-represented in
  • 37:48 - 37:50
    fields such as engineering and
  • 37:50 - 37:53
    architecture. So what she put in her slide
  • 37:53 - 37:56
    is that these kinds of data challenge
  • 37:56 - 38:00
    accounts suggesting that Dyslexia is a
  • 38:00 - 38:03
    deficit. It is not, it's a trade off. You
  • 38:03 - 38:06
    can't just look at weaknesses associated
  • 38:06 - 38:09
    with Dyslexia, the whole picture is a
  • 38:09 - 38:13
    trade off. Some abilities are very strong,
  • 38:13 - 38:15
    or even above average, while other things
  • 38:15 - 38:19
    are the more traditional Dyslexia related
  • 38:19 - 38:22
    challenges with reading and writing.
  • 38:22 - 38:26
    I did want to mention some of the Dyslexic
  • 38:26 - 38:30
    strength literature here. Some examples,
  • 38:30 - 38:32
    Dyslexics, as I mentioned, outperform
  • 38:32 - 38:34
    non-Dyslexics on a variety of tasks. Here,
  • 38:34 - 38:40
    some in a previous study, if you do these
  • 38:40 - 38:44
    spatial puzzles here on the left, Dyslexics
  • 38:44 - 38:46
    are better than non-Dyslexics at these
  • 38:46 - 38:49
    things. On creative insight problems, it
  • 38:49 - 38:52
    shows that more problems were correctly
  • 38:52 - 38:54
    solved by the Dyslexic group compared to
  • 38:54 - 38:57
    the non-Dyslexic group. As mentioned,
  • 38:57 - 38:59
    Dylsexics are over-represented in schools
  • 38:59 - 39:00
    of art and engineering in surveys that
  • 39:00 - 39:04
    have been done, as well as surveys of
  • 39:04 - 39:06
    successful entrepreneurs.
  • 39:10 - 39:12
    These are some of the data that we've had
  • 39:12 - 39:15
    from our clinic. These are WISC scores,
  • 39:15 - 39:17
    here are various measurements on the
  • 39:17 - 39:20
    WIAT, for those of you who know about the
  • 39:20 - 39:23
    technical aspects of these tests. So, if
  • 39:23 - 39:26
    100 is the average, I think what I really
  • 39:26 - 39:30
    wanted to show you here is that there are
  • 39:30 - 39:32
    some areas that are below average, below
  • 39:32 - 39:36
    the line at 100 of things like working
  • 39:36 - 39:39
    memory, processing speed, sentence
  • 39:39 - 39:41
    repetition. Here are things like oral
  • 39:41 - 39:45
    reading accuracy, and math facts fluency.
  • 39:45 - 39:48
    At the same time, there's some
  • 39:48 - 39:50
    spectacularly high scores as well of
  • 39:50 - 39:53
    things like verbal reasoning ability,
  • 39:53 - 39:57
    oral word fluency, reading comprehension
  • 39:57 - 40:02
    can be very strong among some Stealth
  • 40:02 - 40:04
    Dyslexics, and things like that. Math
  • 40:04 - 40:07
    problem solving can also be in the average
  • 40:07 - 40:10
    range or very high amongst some clusters
  • 40:10 - 40:13
    of students. The main point getting across
  • 40:13 - 40:17
    here, Dyslexics are not a flat line. They
  • 40:17 - 40:19
    don't have even abilities. They are
  • 40:19 - 40:22
    lop-sided in this way, but it's also a
  • 40:22 - 40:25
    huge mistake to believe that Dyslexia is
  • 40:25 - 40:27
    weakness only. It's also accompanied by
  • 40:27 - 40:31
    some pretty impressive strengths. If we
  • 40:31 - 40:33
    are not looking for them, we need to be.
  • 40:36 - 40:38
    This just reinforces that the up-side
  • 40:38 - 40:41
    conceptual ability typically far exceeds
  • 40:41 - 40:44
    academic achievement. Often, what we've
  • 40:44 - 40:48
    seen when we spend time with these students
  • 40:48 - 40:51
    is that they really have extraordinary
  • 40:51 - 40:53
    conceptual ability, often 1grade, 2 grades,
  • 40:53 - 40:56
    sometimes even more above their grade.
  • 40:56 - 40:58
    It's something to be looked for because it
  • 40:58 - 41:01
    can be very frustrating, not only to have
  • 41:01 - 41:03
    to spend extra time with the basic rote
  • 41:03 - 41:08
    aspects of schooling, but to not be fed
  • 41:08 - 41:10
    for their advanced conceptual ability can
  • 41:10 - 41:14
    be a recipe for problems, because then
  • 41:14 - 41:17
    school really doesn't hold any excitement
  • 41:17 - 41:21
    or joy for them. It's not uncommon, for
  • 41:21 - 41:23
    instance,where we've had students who
  • 41:23 - 41:27
    were placed in gifted classrooms, but with
  • 41:27 - 41:28
    accommodations to support them for
  • 41:28 - 41:32
    reading and writing. That's why we like the
  • 41:32 - 41:33
    comprehensive testing--you have to look
  • 41:33 - 41:35
    for the strengths as well as the
  • 41:35 - 41:37
    challenges if you are going to be doing a
  • 41:37 - 41:40
    good assessment for Dyslexia.
  • 41:40 - 41:43
    So, maybe that's a good segway into the
  • 41:43 - 41:47
    next and final part of this talk, Dyslexic
  • 41:47 - 41:52
    talents in children. There are a wide
  • 41:52 - 41:54
    range of talents that we see among these
  • 41:54 - 41:55
    students, things like spatial problem
  • 41:55 - 41:57
    solving, this is a fantastic maze birthday
  • 41:57 - 42:00
    card that an 8 year old happened to bring
  • 42:00 - 42:02
    with him, and he showed me actually how it
  • 42:02 - 42:05
    could be solved. Often students really
  • 42:05 - 42:06
    excel or are precocious at hands on
  • 42:06 - 42:11
    projects. They may have unusual
  • 42:11 - 42:14
    perspectives. Here is an example of a
  • 42:14 - 42:18
    drawing of a bike from Michael Critchley's
  • 42:18 - 42:20
    work, a 9 year old was spontaneously
  • 42:20 - 42:24
    drawing a bike from an aerial view.
  • 42:24 - 42:25
    Spatial talents can also present in the
  • 42:25 - 42:28
    kinds of things they do. We've had a
  • 42:28 - 42:33
    number of soccer goalies, hockey goalies,
  • 42:33 - 42:40
    who are Dyslexic. Some of that may be just
  • 42:40 - 42:46
    pure athletic ability, but many cases, I
  • 42:46 - 42:50
    think it's spatial, and it's being able to
  • 42:50 - 42:52
    ...a little bit of dynamic reasoning, to
  • 42:52 - 42:54
    see how people are moving, and to know
  • 42:54 - 42:56
    where the ball is going to go next. We've
  • 42:56 - 42:59
    also had a number of accomplished sailors
  • 42:59 - 43:01
    among our student group, we have parents,
  • 43:01 - 43:06
    also, who are very good at navigation,
  • 43:06 - 43:09
    sailing. This is another kind of complex
  • 43:09 - 43:12
    system where Dyslexics can excel at,
  • 43:12 - 43:14
    factoring in things like the weather,
  • 43:14 - 43:17
    wind, the current, and being able to know
  • 43:17 - 43:22
    where you are and where you'll be.
  • 43:25 - 43:27
    At the same time, the talents in Dyslexic
  • 43:27 - 43:30
    children aren't limited to just spatial
  • 43:30 - 43:32
    abilities. Some students are extremely
  • 43:32 - 43:35
    verbally gifted. They can be wonderful
  • 43:35 - 43:38
    storytellers, have advanced conceptual
  • 43:38 - 43:41
    reasoning as I mentioned, great verbal
  • 43:41 - 43:44
    reasoning...walking arguments, in fact.
  • 43:44 - 43:46
    Some of these students, you can see, they
  • 43:46 - 43:48
    are socially perceptive. We also see
  • 43:48 - 43:51
    students who have particularly strong
  • 43:51 - 43:56
    math and science abilities. One of the
  • 43:56 - 43:58
    questions we were asked in the live
  • 43:58 - 43:59
    webinar was a question about dynamic
  • 43:59 - 44:02
    reasoning, and when does that present?
  • 44:02 - 44:03
    Although that's typically an older
  • 44:03 - 44:06
    presenting gift among Dyslexic students,
  • 44:06 - 44:08
    we also can see that among students who
  • 44:08 - 44:12
    really thrive with for instance, video
  • 44:12 - 44:14
    gaming or imagining video gaming. This is
  • 44:14 - 44:16
    a drawing that one student brought in to
  • 44:16 - 44:19
    us about how he would plan a game, and
  • 44:19 - 44:22
    what you can see is it is a very complex
  • 44:22 - 44:24
    diagram. It had a lot of feedback loops,
  • 44:24 - 44:26
    --if this happened, this would go to
  • 44:26 - 44:29
    here--and students really love the
  • 44:29 - 44:32
    complexity of systems like this. Sometimes
  • 44:32 - 44:34
    gaming at this age may be the best way to
  • 44:34 - 44:38
    kind of analyze things like that. Older
  • 44:38 - 44:40
    students can get involved in things like
  • 44:40 - 44:42
    Game Theory, or Economics, but in the
  • 44:42 - 44:47
    early ages -- maybe video gaming or
  • 44:47 - 44:52
    imaginary worlds or fantasy may satisfy
  • 44:52 - 44:56
    that craving for complexity. Dyslexic
  • 44:56 - 44:59
    students we often find, are very
  • 44:59 - 45:01
    metacognitive. There is a study of college
  • 45:01 - 45:03
    students that found Dyslexic students had
  • 45:03 - 45:06
    stronger metacognitive abilities than
  • 45:06 - 45:08
    non-Dyslexic students, maybe because they
  • 45:08 - 45:10
    had a harder time with rote memory. They
  • 45:10 - 45:13
    are often extremely analytical. When we
  • 45:13 - 45:14
    did a survey at Dyslexic Advantage dot
  • 45:14 - 45:17
    com, I think the analytical strength was
  • 45:17 - 45:19
    among the highest that people reported
  • 45:19 - 45:22
    of their talents that they seemed to use
  • 45:22 - 45:24
    commonly in their workplace and their
  • 45:24 - 45:25
    jobs.
  • 45:28 - 45:31
    When we surveyed the talents in our
  • 45:31 - 45:34
    parents that we saw for the students in
  • 45:34 - 45:38
    our clinic, we had probably an unusual
  • 45:38 - 45:42
    cluster of parent careers, because we
  • 45:42 - 45:46
    are near Microsoft and Boeing, but, 43%
  • 45:46 - 45:48
    of the parents from out clinic were in
  • 45:48 - 45:50
    engineering, computers, science, or
  • 45:50 - 45:52
    economics. Twenty-five percent were in
  • 45:52 - 45:54
    business, upper management, or sales.
  • 45:54 - 45:58
    Other occupations included pilot,coaches,
  • 45:58 - 46:01
    counselors, and veterinarians. There were
  • 46:01 - 46:04
    strengths that build on personal
  • 46:04 - 46:07
    communication, empathy, but also things
  • 46:07 - 46:09
    like problem solving and spatial
  • 46:09 - 46:15
    visualization and reasoning. This is just
  • 46:15 - 46:18
    a little pie chart looking at the
  • 46:18 - 46:20
    different kinds of careers that we found
  • 46:20 - 46:24
    when we polled individuals from our
  • 46:24 - 46:26
    community.
  • 46:30 - 46:33
    Okay, this brings me to Dyslexic Mind
  • 46:33 - 46:36
    Strengths and I'll come into a close here.
  • 46:36 - 46:38
    MIND is an acronym that we use that
  • 46:38 - 46:40
    stands for Material Reasoning,
  • 46:40 - 46:42
    Interconnected Reasoning, Narrative
  • 46:42 - 46:44
    Reasoning, and Dynamic Reasoning. These
  • 46:44 - 46:47
    are brain-based mechanisms that contribute
  • 46:47 - 46:49
    to Dyslexic strengths.
  • 46:55 - 46:58
    Material Reasoning is an ability to
  • 46:58 - 47:00
    reason about the physical characteristics
  • 47:00 - 47:02
    of objects in a material universe, largely
  • 47:02 - 47:04
    spatial reasoning ability. Material
  • 47:04 - 47:07
    reasoners are often able to, for instance,
  • 47:07 - 47:11
    figure out how things work, gears,
  • 47:11 - 47:14
    pulleys, for example. This is a drawing
  • 47:14 - 47:17
    from a great little builder in our clinic.
  • 47:17 - 47:19
    At 6 years old, he was already drawing
  • 47:19 - 47:22
    ships in cross-section from an aerial
  • 47:22 - 47:26
    view. This child's understanding of what
  • 47:26 - 47:29
    a boat is certainly better than a standard
  • 47:29 - 47:32
    six year old who doesn't have that strong
  • 47:32 - 47:36
    physical knowledge of what things are.
  • 47:38 - 47:40
    The "I" stands for Interconnected
  • 47:40 - 47:42
    Reasoning, and that's an ability to spot
  • 47:42 - 47:43
    connections or relationships between
  • 47:43 - 47:46
    different objects, concepts, or points
  • 47:46 - 47:49
    of view. An ability to connect diverse
  • 47:49 - 47:51
    perspectives, or see things from different
  • 47:51 - 47:54
    points of view can be used in all kinds of
  • 47:54 - 47:59
    situations, from things like the work
  • 47:59 - 48:00
    place, working in businesses,
  • 48:00 - 48:06
    corporations, to writing novels, or...in
  • 48:06 - 48:07
    the military, being able to see a
  • 48:07 - 48:12
    battlefield from different perspectives.
  • 48:12 - 48:16
    This is really an incredible strength, but
  • 48:16 - 48:19
    it can also be a challenge when students
  • 48:19 - 48:21
    are younger, and they have trouble picking
  • 48:21 - 48:25
    one, or narrowing what they want to say
  • 48:25 - 48:28
    in a paper. Interconnected Reasoning
  • 48:28 - 48:33
    allows individuals to unite information,
  • 48:33 - 48:34
    to assume a global or "big picture"
  • 48:34 - 48:37
    perspective, and to also determine large
  • 48:37 - 48:39
    scale features like gist -- what's the
  • 48:39 - 48:44
    "gist" of the situation, or context.
  • 48:45 - 48:47
    N stands for Narrative Reasoning, and
  • 48:47 - 48:50
    that's an ability to create stories by
  • 48:50 - 48:52
    connecting a series of mental scenes from
  • 48:52 - 48:54
    past personal experience, as well as a
  • 48:54 - 48:56
    tendency to use stories to recall the
  • 48:56 - 48:59
    past, understand the present, and imagine
  • 48:59 - 49:04
    the future. We have a lot of gifted
  • 49:04 - 49:08
    storytellers in our clinic. We've seen
  • 49:08 - 49:12
    that in adults with Dyslexia that
  • 49:12 - 49:15
    storytelling ability can be great for all
  • 49:15 - 49:17
    kinds of occupations, not just writing,
  • 49:17 - 49:20
    but also communicating a vision in a
  • 49:20 - 49:24
    workplace as a business leader or, for
  • 49:24 - 49:28
    instance, in the courtroom with a lawyer,
  • 49:28 - 49:30
    who can communicate and really connect
  • 49:30 - 49:33
    to the jury. So, Narrative Reasoning is
  • 49:33 - 49:37
    really a fascinating strength associated
  • 49:37 - 49:41
    with Dyslexia. Many students are able to
  • 49:41 - 49:44
    use stories to really boost their memory
  • 49:44 - 49:48
    for information in the classroom.
  • 49:48 - 49:50
    Which brings us to the last Mind Strength,
  • 49:50 - 49:53
    Dynamic Reasoning. That's an ability to
  • 49:53 - 49:56
    recombine the elements of the past to
  • 49:56 - 49:58
    predict or simulate future outcomes.
  • 49:58 - 50:00
    Dynamic Reasoning really builds on
  • 50:00 - 50:04
    pattern recognition. It often involves
  • 50:04 - 50:07
    personal rather than abstract memory.
  • 50:07 - 50:10
    It can be great for highly changeable or
  • 50:10 - 50:12
    ambiguous situations, where there is
  • 50:12 - 50:15
    incomplete knowledge. Cutting edge fields,
  • 50:15 - 50:18
    finance...if suddenly you have a crash of
  • 50:18 - 50:20
    a stock market, what is going to happen
  • 50:20 - 50:22
    next? It's not ever happened before like
  • 50:22 - 50:26
    this...A lot of Dyslexic dynamic reasoners
  • 50:26 - 50:30
    really excel in times like this because
  • 50:30 - 50:33
    they are trend spotters, pattern people,
  • 50:33 - 50:36
    they look at best fit, and so, it doesn't
  • 50:36 - 50:38
    matter if it hasn't happened before, they
  • 50:38 - 50:39
    will make a good prediction for the
  • 50:39 - 50:43
    future. As mentioned, Dynamic Reasoning
  • 50:43 - 50:45
    uses these "best fit" cognitive processes
  • 50:45 - 50:47
    rather than rule-based, deductive, or
  • 50:47 - 50:50
    formulaic thinking. Rather , Dynamic
  • 50:50 - 50:53
    Reasoners used cases and examples. They
  • 50:53 - 50:55
    like to connect the dots in order to
  • 50:55 - 50:57
    predict what is going to happen next.
  • 50:57 - 51:00
    Particularly good for areas that are
  • 51:00 - 51:03
    completely new, as mentioned, new
  • 51:03 - 51:05
    domains of knowledge... A very famous
  • 51:05 - 51:07
    highly successful Dyslexic researchers in
  • 51:07 - 51:10
    all kinds of fields, and inventors, also
  • 51:10 - 51:15
    in business and finance, as mentioned.
  • 51:20 - 51:22
    I just want to talk a bit--I'm just about
  • 51:22 - 51:24
    to close--about some of the trade-offs
  • 51:24 - 51:26
    that happen in memory, based on the
  • 51:26 - 51:28
    Dyslexic processing style. As mentioned,
  • 51:28 - 51:31
    rote memory is a relative weakness for
  • 51:31 - 51:35
    many Dyslexics. It's non-contextual, it's
  • 51:35 - 51:37
    generalized (generic), like a dictionary
  • 51:37 - 51:41
    definition. The strength side of memory
  • 51:41 - 51:43
    for many Dyslexics is personal memory,
  • 51:43 - 51:46
    it's like an episode, scene-based. It
  • 51:46 - 51:49
    occurs at a specific time and place, and
  • 51:49 - 51:54
    often uses cases and examples with
  • 51:54 - 51:58
    connection of these, in order to create a
  • 51:58 - 52:00
    big picture.
  • 52:02 - 52:05
    Why Dyslexic Strengths matter? There has
  • 52:05 - 52:07
    been a lot of controversy and discussion
  • 52:07 - 52:11
    about whether it's important to address
  • 52:11 - 52:14
    strengths associated with Dyslexic
  • 52:14 - 52:16
    students. We think it is absolutely
  • 52:16 - 52:20
    essential. This is another student in
  • 52:20 - 52:25
    our clinic. This is percentile, now, and
  • 52:25 - 52:30
    the average range is at the 50 percentile
  • 52:30 - 52:34
    mark. If a student has this kind of oral
  • 52:34 - 52:38
    expression, listening comprehension, but
  • 52:38 - 52:42
    down here visual matching, or math facts,
  • 52:42 - 52:46
    fluency; looking at the whole picture is
  • 52:46 - 52:48
    really essential for understanding the
  • 52:48 - 52:50
    pressure points that a student is feelilng
  • 52:50 - 52:53
    in school. How to optimize and have
  • 52:53 - 52:55
    challenge in the areas where they are
  • 52:55 - 52:59
    actually thriving at, but also support and
  • 52:59 - 53:01
    reasonable expectations for their
  • 53:01 - 53:04
    weaknesses. Understanding, for instance,
  • 53:04 - 53:07
    visual matching is a sub-test which can
  • 53:07 - 53:10
    be associated with these so-called
  • 53:10 - 53:12
    "careless mistakes" that happen with
  • 53:12 - 53:16
    mathematics. There is often a little
  • 53:16 - 53:19
    drift that can occur for many Dyslexics.
  • 53:19 - 53:21
    They'll look at numbers, then they'll look
  • 53:21 - 53:24
    again, and the numbers may switch place.
  • 53:25 - 53:27
    They are called careless mistakes, but
  • 53:27 - 53:29
    they aren't really careless, they are
  • 53:29 - 53:31
    perceptual mistakes. Understanding this
  • 53:31 - 53:34
    extra difficulty, rather than give them
  • 53:34 - 53:39
    more and more rote problems to solve, it's
  • 53:39 - 53:40
    good to really see the whole pictureof
  • 53:40 - 53:43
    things, and be able to assign fewer
  • 53:43 - 53:46
    problems. Also, projects that will appeal
  • 53:46 - 53:48
    to the areas of their strength, and the
  • 53:48 - 53:51
    modes that they learn fast in.
  • 53:51 - 53:56
    Dyslexics often have extreme discrepancies
  • 53:56 - 53:58
    between their different cognitive
  • 53:58 - 54:02
    abilities. Failure to recognize that can
  • 54:02 - 54:03
    really take a toll on their esteem and
  • 54:03 - 54:05
    emotional health. It's very important,
  • 54:05 - 54:07
    what the environment is, as I mentioned,
  • 54:07 - 54:13
    what the expectations should be. On the
  • 54:13 - 54:17
    positive side, recognizing student's
  • 54:17 - 54:20
    strengths, really have powerful effects
  • 54:20 - 54:23
    on these students' futures. We've had so
  • 54:23 - 54:24
    many students come back to us, and tell
  • 54:24 - 54:29
    us, just understanding their situation,
  • 54:29 - 54:32
    seeing what they were good at, getting
  • 54:32 - 54:34
    the whole picture of what they were facing
  • 54:34 - 54:37
    was very empowering, very encouraging.
  • 54:37 - 54:39
    And what we've talked to highly
  • 54:39 - 54:42
    accomplished individuals as adults, they
  • 54:42 - 54:44
    often said, "Having my mom or dad really
  • 54:44 - 54:48
    believe in me, understand me, or a teacher
  • 54:48 - 54:51
    who encouraged me, recognized that my
  • 54:51 - 54:53
    ideas were really good, really helped
  • 54:53 - 54:55
    carry me through a difficult time, and a
  • 54:55 - 54:57
    time I really needed to put in a lot of
  • 54:57 - 55:01
    extra work compared to my peers."
  • 55:02 - 55:07
    I wanted to share this brain study. It is
  • 55:07 - 55:11
    really fascinating. In this test, subjects
  • 55:11 - 55:13
    were asked to silently generate the verb
  • 55:13 - 55:17
    associated with a noun. For example, if
  • 55:17 - 55:21
    they were given the example of a boat,
  • 55:21 - 55:23
    what would you expect the boat to do?
  • 55:23 - 55:25
    It would be sailing, Just thinking of
  • 55:25 - 55:29
    sailing, and not even saying anything, the
  • 55:29 - 55:32
    red brain, these are cross-sections from
  • 55:32 - 55:35
    the base of the brain, up to the top. The
  • 55:35 - 55:37
    red parts are all the different parts of
  • 55:37 - 55:39
    the brain that
  • 55:39 - 55:40
    non-Dyslexics used when they were thinking
  • 55:40 - 55:44
    of that. The blue parts were all the parts
  • 55:44 - 55:46
    where Dyslexics subjects were using, so
  • 55:46 - 55:49
    completely different areas of
  • 55:49 - 55:51
    the brain to process things. Just
  • 55:51 - 55:53
    understanding how different Dyslexic
  • 55:53 - 55:57
    processing is from non-Dyslexics, really
  • 55:57 - 56:01
    can help us understand why there can be
  • 56:01 - 56:04
    a disconnect in the classroom, why many
  • 56:04 - 56:07
    Dyslexics are not well understood, and
  • 56:07 - 56:11
    why we need to work and do more research,
  • 56:11 - 56:16
    and more policies that improve our
  • 56:16 - 56:20
    understanding of how to optimize work
  • 56:20 - 56:22
    places and classrooms for Dyslexic
  • 56:22 - 56:24
    people.
  • 56:26 - 56:28
    What next? Dyslexic [Advantage] is now a
  • 56:28 - 56:32
    501c3 organization. We hope that you will
  • 56:32 - 56:35
    watch and share videos from the conference
  • 56:35 - 56:41
    conference. This is the youtube url--
  • 56:41 - 56:45
    youtube.com/dyslexicadvantage . We hope
  • 56:45 - 56:49
    you really consider making a donation to
  • 56:49 - 56:50
    the Webinar series, we'd like to continue
  • 56:50 - 56:55
    to do these, and be able to create a
  • 56:55 - 56:59
    library that can help more individuals.
  • 57:00 - 57:03
    The focus of our group is to look at
  • 57:03 - 57:06
    reframing Dyslexia. Too often right now,
  • 57:06 - 57:09
    Dyslexia is thought to be a stigma, and
  • 57:09 - 57:11
    many people believe that Dyslexics are
  • 57:11 - 57:15
    low potential people. For that reason, we
  • 57:15 - 57:17
    need to speak out more. A lot of people
  • 57:17 - 57:22
    need to make steps to educate their peers,
  • 57:22 - 57:25
    and friend and family members about the
  • 57:25 - 57:28
    high potential associated with Dyslexia,
  • 57:28 - 57:32
    and the talent side. Our goal is to
  • 57:32 - 57:33
    identify and help more students and
  • 57:33 - 57:37
    adults to share more of the research and
  • 57:37 - 57:40
    applications of strength based focus, so
  • 57:40 - 57:41
    that we can help our students in
  • 57:41 - 57:44
    classrooms, and more employees and
  • 57:44 - 57:49
    workplaces. Education, of course, extends
  • 57:49 - 57:51
    to parents, teachers, and non-Dyslexics,
  • 57:51 - 57:54
    and what we'd like to do, is improve
  • 57:54 - 57:56
    awareness all the way around about the
  • 57:56 - 57:58
    talent side of Dyslexia, and help build
  • 57:58 - 58:01
    our community, so it's better
  • 58:01 - 58:04
    opportunities for everyone. So, thank
  • 58:04 - 58:07
    you very much for joining us, and please
  • 58:07 - 58:10
    get in touch with us so if we can help
  • 58:10 - 58:12
    more, if you've got ideas for future
  • 58:12 - 58:15
    webinars, or if we can answer some
  • 58:15 - 58:19
    questions. Thank you!
Title:
Intro to Dyslexia and the Dyslexic Advantage
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English, British
Duration:
58:19

English subtitles

Revisions