< Return to Video

Human Language 1 Colorless Green Ideas

  • 0:32 - 0:33
    (announcer) The Human Language series
  • 0:33 - 0:37
    was funded by the National Endowment
    for the Humanities,
  • 0:37 - 0:39
    the National Science Foundation,
  • 0:39 - 0:40
    the Annenberg Foundation,
  • 0:40 - 0:41
    the Arthur Vining Davis Foundations,
  • 0:41 - 0:43
    the Corporation for Public Broadcasting,
  • 0:43 - 0:46
    annual financial support
    from viewers like you,
  • 0:46 - 0:48
    the National Institute of Mental Health,
  • 0:48 - 0:51
    the Geraldine R. Dodge Foundation,
  • 0:51 - 0:52
    and these additional funders.
  • 0:52 - 0:55
    [dinging]
  • 0:58 - 1:00
    (George Miller) What is language?
  • 1:00 - 1:02
    Everybody knows, thank heavens.
  • 1:02 - 1:06
    [people talking]
  • 1:06 - 1:10
    (female) Is language simple
    or is language complex?
  • 1:10 - 1:13
    (George Carlin) The girl that
    that's the house of's father.
  • 1:13 - 1:17
    (narrator) This is Part 1 of a series
    on the human language.
  • 1:21 - 1:24
    (male) The question is: how can
    you learn so much from so little?
  • 1:24 - 1:27
    (Roy) The question is: how does
    a child know what a word is?
  • 1:32 - 1:35
    (Lewis) How is it that we can
    so alter our thought patterns
  • 1:35 - 1:38
    to fit into the linear arrangement
    of any particular arrangement?
  • 1:38 - 1:40
    [girls speaking]
  • 1:40 - 1:42
    (female) I thought
    you had already broke up.
  • 1:42 - 1:45
    (female #1) No, he broke up with me
    before I could break up with him.
  • 1:45 - 1:49
    (narrator) What specific biological
    endowment could we have
  • 1:49 - 1:51
    that allows us to do this?
  • 1:51 - 1:59
    [man shouting]
  • 1:59 - 2:01
    [children shouting]
  • 2:01 - 2:03
    (female) Is there something true,
  • 2:03 - 2:07
    some core of properties true
    of all human languages?
  • 2:07 - 2:12
    [man shouting]
  • 2:12 - 2:15
    [children shouting]
  • 2:15 - 2:28
    [people speaking different languages]
  • 2:33 - 2:36
    (Noam Chomsky) How do people
    simply interact with language?
  • 2:36 - 2:39
    How do people do
    what I am now doing?
  • 2:39 - 2:43
    Or if you meet somebody,
    say at a bus stop,
  • 2:43 - 2:45
    and you start having a conversation,
  • 2:45 - 2:46
    how do you do it?
  • 2:48 - 2:50
    (woman) Take care of yourself.
  • 2:54 - 2:58
    (Mark) People don't think when they speak,
    they just speak.
  • 3:01 - 3:06
    (narrator) This is a report
    on a study of an enigma.
  • 3:06 - 3:08
    [people talking in background]
  • 3:08 - 3:13
    Language is the most human thing
    there is about being human,
  • 3:13 - 3:17
    yet most people don't think much about it.
  • 3:17 - 3:21
    Traditionally, linguists have
    studied the history, vocabulary,
  • 3:21 - 3:25
    and grammar of the world's languages,
  • 3:25 - 3:26
    but within our generation,
  • 3:26 - 3:31
    new theories have
    revolutionized our thinking.
  • 3:31 - 3:35
    Today, linguists ask questions
    about how we use language
  • 3:35 - 3:41
    and how language functions
    inside the human mind.
  • 3:41 - 3:45
    For example, why are languages
    filled with rules we all follow
  • 3:45 - 3:49
    without knowing why?
  • 3:49 - 3:51
    (female) How would you describe this?
  • 3:51 - 3:54
    (female) Well, I know it's supposed
    to be a big red balloon.
  • 3:54 - 3:56
    (female) How about a red big balloon?
  • 3:56 - 3:59
    Could this be a red big balloon?
  • 3:59 - 4:00
    (male) A big red balloon.
  • 4:00 - 4:01
    (female) Okay.
  • 4:01 - 4:04
    Do you have any idea why you feel
    more comfortable saying a big --
  • 4:04 - 4:11
    Do you have a sense of why that feels
    better than a red big balloon?
  • 4:11 - 4:12
    (male) I have no reason why.
  • 4:12 - 4:15
    (male) Three big red round plastic balls.
  • 4:15 - 4:20
    Not big three round plastic red big balls.
  • 4:20 - 4:25
    (narrator) Linguists claim that
    languages is rule governed.
  • 4:25 - 4:26
    What rules?
  • 4:26 - 4:28
    (George Carlin) Stop and go traffic.
  • 4:28 - 4:31
    Should be go and stop traffic.
    You can't stop until you've gone.
  • 4:31 - 4:33
    [cars honking]
  • 4:33 - 4:39
    (Mark) You can re-climb
    and you can re-institutionalize,
  • 4:39 - 4:41
    but you can't re-fall down.
  • 4:41 - 4:42
    [chuckles]
    I don't know why that is.
  • 4:42 - 4:43
    It's peculiar.
  • 4:43 - 4:46
    Why can't you re-fall down?
  • 4:46 - 4:49
    (Howard) And you can say things like
    "Don't rouse up the natives,"
  • 4:49 - 4:50
    but you can't say,
    "Don't rouse up them."
  • 4:50 - 4:52
    You have to say,
    "Don't rouse them up."
  • 4:52 - 4:54
    (Russell) Language is untidy.
  • 4:54 - 4:56
    Language is a mess.
  • 4:56 - 4:58
    (male) "That John left surprised me."
  • 4:58 - 4:59
    What's the "that" doing there?
  • 4:59 - 5:01
    I mean, everybody knows you can't say,
  • 5:01 - 5:02
    "John left surprised me."
  • 5:02 - 5:04
    There's something wrong with that.
  • 5:04 - 5:07
    The rules of English say you
    got to have a "that" in front.
  • 5:07 - 5:08
    Well, why?
  • 5:08 - 5:11
    [female speaking French]
  • 5:11 - 5:13
    (female) Could you do it
    the other way around?
  • 5:13 - 5:15
    [female speaking French]
  • 5:15 - 5:17
    (female) And is that fine?
  • 5:17 - 5:18
    (female) No, it's not.
  • 5:18 - 5:19
    The first one is fine,
  • 5:19 - 5:21
    the other one you just will never say it.
  • 5:21 - 5:25
    (child) A big red balloon.
  • 5:25 - 5:27
    (Mark) Those are just
    the rules of my language
  • 5:27 - 5:33
    that I learned totally
    unconsciously as a little child.
  • 5:33 - 5:36
    (female) It's the same old story.
  • 5:36 - 5:38
    (George Carlin) What's going on in here?
  • 5:38 - 5:41
    (female) It's the old same story?
  • 5:41 - 5:46
    (narrator) Who or what tells us
    how to say it?
  • 5:46 - 5:51
    (Noam) The language faculty is a
    system, a subsystem, of the brain,
  • 5:51 - 5:52
    of the human brain.
  • 5:52 - 5:58
    Its major elements don't appear
    to exist in other similar organisms.
  • 5:58 - 6:04
    And that organ of the brain yields
    a language under appropriate conditions,
  • 6:04 - 6:07
    which is partially tuned
    by the environment
  • 6:07 - 6:10
    but to a large extent
    appears to be determined
  • 6:10 - 6:12
    by our biological endowment
  • 6:12 - 6:15
    and is essential invariant
    across the species.
  • 6:15 - 6:17
    (Frederick) There have really
    been three major revolutions
  • 6:17 - 6:19
    in modern linguistics.
  • 6:19 - 6:21
    The first, about 200 years ago,
  • 6:21 - 6:22
    found change is irregular.
  • 6:22 - 6:25
    That you can reconstruct
    ancient languages
  • 6:25 - 6:27
    and talk about how
    language has changed
  • 6:27 - 6:29
    from the ancient times to the present.
  • 6:29 - 6:32
    The second was at the turn
    of this century
  • 6:32 - 6:35
    where it was discovered that
    languages themselves are systematic.
  • 6:35 - 6:39
    That there are rules governing
    how languages are put together.
  • 6:39 - 6:42
    The third revolution came
    with Noam Chomsky
  • 6:42 - 6:45
    in his book Syntactic Structures in 1957.
  • 6:45 - 6:49
    (Howard Lasnik) In 1957, Chomsky published
    this little book, Syntactic Structures.
  • 6:49 - 6:54
    And it almost immediately
    revolutionized the field.
  • 6:54 - 6:58
    It had a profound influence on
    everyone who read it carefully,
  • 6:58 - 7:02
    and the reason for this is the beauty of
    the analyses that Chomsky presented.
  • 7:02 - 7:06
    He was able to show that there were
    patterns when it had just looked like
  • 7:06 - 7:07
    there were many separate facts
  • 7:07 - 7:10
    and he was able to come up
    with very simple rules
  • 7:10 - 7:12
    for explaining these patterns.
  • 7:12 - 7:14
    (Frederick) Basically what
    Chomsky said was
  • 7:14 - 7:17
    that your sights aren't set high enough.
  • 7:17 - 7:18
    You're asking the wrong questions.
  • 7:18 - 7:21
    You're not asking the most
    interesting questions.
  • 7:21 - 7:23
    What you're asking is,
  • 7:23 - 7:27
    "Well, we know language is a system
    so how should we divide it up?
  • 7:27 - 7:29
    Where should we draw
    the line in a word,
  • 7:29 - 7:31
    where should we draw the line
    in the sentence,
  • 7:31 - 7:32
    what are the pieces?"
  • 7:32 - 7:35
    Chomsky said,
    "Let's ask a different question.
  • 7:35 - 7:38
    Let's ask a much more ambitious question.
  • 7:38 - 7:42
    Let's ask the question, 'What is
    a possible human language?' "
  • 7:42 - 7:45
    (Howard) What was so appealing
    about this analysis
  • 7:45 - 7:47
    and what was so revolutionary about it
  • 7:47 - 7:51
    is it showed that there's
    some process in your head,
  • 7:51 - 7:54
    some rule, some pattern
    that you have abstractly.
  • 7:54 - 7:59
    And you use this rule in producing
    sentences and in understanding sentences.
  • 7:59 - 8:01
    That is, he moved linguistics away
  • 8:01 - 8:05
    from merely gathering
    and investigating sentences
  • 8:05 - 8:06
    that have already been produced
  • 8:06 - 8:11
    and moved it towards the investigation
    of human potentiality.
  • 8:11 - 8:12
    What kinds of sentences
    can you produce,
  • 8:12 - 8:14
    what kinds of sentences
    can you understand?
  • 8:14 - 8:18
    [people talking in background]
  • 8:18 - 8:19
    (Frederick) Once you say,
  • 8:19 - 8:21
    "What is a possible human language?"
  • 8:21 - 8:24
    in a way you're saying,
    "What is a possible human being?"
  • 8:24 - 8:27
    How does the human mind work?
  • 8:27 - 8:34
    [woman speaking Xhosa language]
  • 8:34 - 8:37
    (Noam) Well, the most elementary
    property of human language
  • 8:37 - 8:38
    that one can imagine
  • 8:38 - 8:44
    is that we can create new expressions
    which express new thoughts.
  • 8:44 - 8:49
    So for example, the sentences
    that I'm now producing,
  • 8:49 - 8:50
    you may not have heard,
  • 8:50 - 8:53
    I may not have said before.
  • 8:53 - 8:55
    Maybe they're new in the history
    of the human race.
  • 8:55 - 9:00
    (George Carlin) Here's something
    no one has ever said before:
  • 9:00 - 9:02
    Toss that anchor over here,
  • 9:02 - 9:06
    I have room for two or three
    more of them in my hip pocket.
  • 9:06 - 9:09
    (Lila) The point of a language
    is to be able to say things
  • 9:09 - 9:12
    you've never said before
    and never heard before
  • 9:12 - 9:17
    and to understand things
    the first time you hear them
  • 9:17 - 9:19
    that have never been said
    before to your knowledge
  • 9:19 - 9:23
    and maybe never been said before at all.
  • 9:23 - 9:26
    But if you're going to be able to say
    and understand new things,
  • 9:26 - 9:30
    you must have acquired something
    very general, a system.
  • 9:30 - 9:33
    (George Miller) There are, perhaps,
    in English, there are about 40 sounds.
  • 9:33 - 9:36
    The sounds get combined to form --
  • 9:36 - 9:41
    well, most people know 40,000 words,
    something of that sort.
  • 9:41 - 9:42
    (George Carlin) Swimming.
  • 9:42 - 9:44
    (female) Cacophony
  • 9:44 - 9:47
    (Mark) Antidisestablishmentarianism.
  • 9:47 - 9:48
    (male) Lunch.
  • 9:48 - 9:50
    (George Miller) Words are built out of sounds
  • 9:50 - 9:52
    and there are a limited number of them.
  • 9:52 - 9:53
    Sentences are built out of words
  • 9:53 - 9:57
    and there's no limit to
    the number of sentences
  • 9:57 - 10:01
    that you can compose in a language.
  • 10:01 - 10:04
    (Mark) You could say
    that words are like atoms.
  • 10:04 - 10:08
    The way atoms are in chemistry,
    as the basic building blocks,
  • 10:08 - 10:10
    those are what the words are.
  • 10:10 - 10:14
    They're fairly solid things
    and you take these atoms
  • 10:14 - 10:18
    and you put them together
    to make up the molecules,
  • 10:18 - 10:20
    or the sentences.
  • 10:20 - 10:24
    (Russell) I remember Senator
    Joseph McCarthy at one time saying,
  • 10:24 - 10:28
    "That's the most unheard
    of thing I've ever heard of."
  • 10:28 - 10:29
    [laughs]
  • 10:29 - 10:33
    Nobody could have invented
    that sentence before McCarthy spoke it.
  • 10:33 - 10:36
    (Mark) It's really a deceptively
    simple system.
  • 10:36 - 10:41
    The genius of the system is that
    with a small number of words
  • 10:41 - 10:43
    and this system of grammar,
  • 10:43 - 10:46
    you can make up an infinite
    number of sentences.
  • 10:46 - 10:49
    You can make up a sentence
    to fit any occasion.
  • 10:49 - 10:54
    (child) And mommy just goes
    to sleep but Daddy just goes--
  • 10:54 - 10:56
    Daddy just, um--
  • 10:56 - 10:58
    (male narrator) The system starts with words.
  • 10:58 - 11:01
    Deceptively simple.
  • 11:01 - 11:05
    But the question is: what is a word?
  • 11:05 - 11:07
    How does a child know what a word is?
  • 11:07 - 11:09
    (child) --to his friend's house.
  • 11:09 - 11:11
    (Judith) You could ask somebody,
    "What is a word?"
  • 11:11 - 11:15
    And most people think they know
    what a word is until you ask them.
  • 11:15 - 11:16
    [child laughing]
  • 11:16 - 11:18
    (female) I word is, uh‚ --
  • 11:18 - 11:21
    (George Carlin): A word is --
  • 11:21 - 11:22
    (Judith Klavans) And you're
    trying to find it
  • 11:22 - 11:24
    and you can't put your finger on it.
  • 11:24 - 11:26
    You don't know.
  • 11:26 - 11:28
    (Jeff) A word is--
  • 11:28 - 11:32
    [man speaking other language]
  • 11:32 - 11:33
    (child) Pencil.
  • 11:33 - 11:34
    (female) Language.
  • 11:34 - 11:39
    A word is something that someone
    thought up a long, long time ago.
  • 11:39 - 11:40
    What is a word?
  • 11:40 - 11:45
    That's actually an extraordinarily
    difficult question.
  • 11:45 - 11:46
    (Lewis) I haven't the ghost
    of an idea.
  • 11:46 - 11:53
    I can't even make up
    a plausible evasion.
  • 11:53 - 11:59
    (Suzette) You could say a word
    is the smallest separate piece
  • 11:59 - 12:03
    of a language that, all by itself,
  • 12:03 - 12:06
    will have a meaning.
  • 12:06 - 12:09
    (male narrator) On the other hand.
  • 12:09 - 12:11
    (Howard) There are words that
    are composed, themselves,
  • 12:11 - 12:16
    of smaller units of bare meaning,
    like "kicked",
  • 12:16 - 12:19
    where "kick" has a meaning and
    "ed" has another meaning,
  • 12:19 - 12:20
    like past tense.
  • 12:23 - 12:25
    (Judith) And I didn't do that.
  • 12:25 - 12:27
    "n't" is it a word?
  • 12:27 - 12:31
    (male) One of my favorite words is
    antidisestablishmentarianism.
  • 12:31 - 12:34
    It's an interesting word,
    of course, because everybody thinks
  • 12:34 - 12:36
    that it's the longest word
    in the English language.
  • 12:36 - 12:38
    In fact, there are many words in English
  • 12:38 - 12:41
    that are far longer than antidisestablishmentarianism.
  • 12:41 - 12:44
    Most of them chemical terms.
  • 12:46 - 12:49
    (Roy) You know, the longest
    word in Webster's 7th
  • 12:49 - 12:55
    is pneumonoultramicroscopicsilicovolca
    nokoniosis,
  • 12:55 - 12:58
    a disease of the lungs.
  • 12:58 - 13:01
    (Mark) Infixes and prefixes and suffixes.
  • 13:01 - 13:03
    (George Carlin) I used to wonder:
  • 13:03 - 13:09
    pre- and suf- are prefixes.
  • 13:09 - 13:11
    Suf- is the prefix of suffix.
  • 13:11 - 13:16
    Is -fix the suffix for prefix and suffix?
  • 13:16 - 13:19
    (Mark) Words seem to
    be somehow indivisible,
  • 13:19 - 13:21
    but you look at them closely
  • 13:21 - 13:27
    and they've got all kinds
    of little parts to them.
  • 13:27 - 13:28
    (female) What's this?
  • 13:28 - 13:29
    I know there's a word for this.
  • 13:29 - 13:33
    (Alvin) I think, for example,
    of the word apodictic.
  • 13:33 - 13:36
    Now, that is not only illegal
    phonological structure,
  • 13:36 - 13:37
    I happen to know it's a word
  • 13:37 - 13:39
    because I've seen it in print
  • 13:39 - 13:41
    and I've even looked it up
    in the dictionary,
  • 13:41 - 13:44
    but I simply can't remember
    what it means.
  • 13:44 - 13:46
    (Howard) I haven't a clue
    what apodictic means.
  • 13:48 - 13:50
    (Mujinga) [speaking African language]
  • 13:50 - 13:53
    I'm from Africa. Zaire.
  • 13:53 - 13:59
    Mujinga means a child born with
    an umbilical cord all over her body.
  • 13:59 - 14:03
    (Lewis) Most words certainly
    are complicated words,
  • 14:03 - 14:07
    like human or like generous,
  • 14:07 - 14:11
    are really small tiny sentences
    all wrapped up
  • 14:11 - 14:14
    in one set of phonemes.
  • 14:14 - 14:18
    [Dan speaking Turkish]
  • 14:18 - 14:19
    That's a single Turkish word.
  • 14:19 - 14:20
    One of my favorite words
  • 14:20 - 14:22
    because it comes out as
    a whole sentence in English.
  • 14:22 - 14:26
    It means, "Are you one of those that
    we haven't been able to Europeanize?"
  • 14:26 - 14:31
    [woman speaking Xhosa language]
  • 14:31 - 14:34
    (Jeff) One example from
    the Eskimo language I work with,
  • 14:34 - 14:36
    which is called Aleutic,
  • 14:36 - 14:40
    is the word [Aleutic word]
  • 14:40 - 14:44
    which means, "Don't you want to
    go window shopping with me?"
  • 14:44 - 14:45
    (Roy) The easy thing to say,
  • 14:45 - 14:47
    for me since I deal with computers a lot,
  • 14:47 - 14:52
    is that a word is the sequence
    of letters between two blanks.
  • 14:52 - 14:54
    (Mark) But what about when we speak?
  • 14:54 - 14:56
    We don't have little spaces
    when we speak,
  • 14:56 - 14:59
    we just run everything together.
  • 14:59 - 15:00
    (George Carlin) O-m-m-i-n-a.
  • 15:00 - 15:01
    Ommina.
  • 15:01 - 15:03
    It means, "I am going to."
  • 15:03 - 15:25
    [people singing and laughing]
  • 15:25 - 15:27
    (Suzette) You know,
    when you listen to people talk,
  • 15:27 - 15:30
    unless they deliberately pause
  • 15:30 - 15:32
    between the pieces
    of what they're saying,
  • 15:32 - 15:34
    it all runs together.
  • 15:34 - 15:36
    (George Carlin) How do we know where
    the gaps are in between the words?
  • 15:36 - 15:38
    Because this all sounds
    like one whole sound.
  • 15:38 - 15:39
    This is just one long sound to me.
  • 15:39 - 15:43
    But you know that I said,
    "This is just one long sound to me."
  • 15:43 - 15:45
    (male) Why are you weary?
  • 15:45 - 15:46
    Why are you weary?
  • 15:46 - 15:48
    (Mark) I walk up to you and say,
  • 15:48 - 15:51
    "Steal it!" or "Teach it!"
  • 15:51 - 15:53
    (male) What'd you want to do now?
  • 15:53 - 15:55
    (female) I don't know,
    what do you want to do?
  • 15:55 - 15:56
    (male) Dunno.
  • 15:56 - 15:57
    An apple?
  • 15:57 - 15:58
    (girl) Yeah.
  • 15:58 - 16:03
    (male) Okay, so will you go home now?
  • 16:03 - 16:04
    Okay.
  • 16:04 - 16:06
    (George Carlin) I'm going to go
    get the bus and go home.
  • 16:06 - 16:10
    (Mark) This may be the hardest thing
    that the brain ever has to do.
  • 16:10 - 16:13
    Is to figure out those
    boundaries at the words
  • 16:13 - 16:16
    that people have just
    run together in speech.
  • 16:16 - 16:18
    (male) Why are you weary?
  • 16:18 - 16:19
    Why are you weary?
  • 16:19 - 16:21
    (Mark) People have invested
    millions of dollars
  • 16:21 - 16:27
    and Lord knows how many hours
    in trying to devise machinery
  • 16:27 - 16:30
    that will be able to perform this task
  • 16:30 - 16:33
    of breaking spoken
    language up into words
  • 16:33 - 16:36
    so that it can be understood.
  • 16:36 - 16:39
    And yet, a child, by the age of one,
  • 16:39 - 16:43
    is perfectly able to understand speech,
  • 16:43 - 16:44
    natural speech,
  • 16:44 - 16:47
    and just pick it apart into words.
  • 16:47 - 16:51
    [child babbling]
  • 16:51 - 16:53
    (Mark) So what's a word?
  • 16:53 - 16:54
    They're very slippery objects.
  • 16:54 - 16:56
    Difficult to deal with.
  • 16:56 - 17:00
    [child babbling]
  • 17:00 - 17:02
    (Mark) Words stand for things.
  • 17:02 - 17:05
    The word "tree" stands for a tree.
  • 17:05 - 17:07
    But words are more than that.
  • 17:07 - 17:10
    After all, the word "tree"
    doesn't just stand for that tree
  • 17:10 - 17:12
    or even any individual tree.
  • 17:12 - 17:15
    It stands for the general notion of a tree.
  • 17:15 - 17:17
    The concept of a tree.
  • 17:17 - 17:20
    That's what Plato would have told us.
  • 17:20 - 17:24
    But then that raises
    the question of concept.
  • 17:24 - 17:25
    What's a concept?
  • 17:25 - 17:29
    (male narrator) What do we have
    in our minds before we speak?
  • 17:29 - 17:32
    Are there concepts
    we do not need to invent
  • 17:32 - 17:36
    because we are born
    already knowing them?
  • 17:36 - 17:39
    Plato said so and so does Chomsky.
  • 17:39 - 17:42
    (Noam) Well, suppose I have, say, a box
  • 17:42 - 17:46
    and I have a marble inside the box
    and a marble outside the box.
  • 17:46 - 17:50
    And I ask, "Which marble
    is near to the box?"
  • 17:50 - 17:52
    Well, the answer is that
    the marble outside the box
  • 17:52 - 17:55
    is either near the box
    or not near it,
  • 17:55 - 17:58
    but the marble in the box
    can't be near it.
  • 17:58 - 18:00
    But again, that's one
    of those simple things
  • 18:00 - 18:02
    that we ought to be surprised about.
  • 18:02 - 18:06
    How does a child know that a marble
    that's in a box can't be near the box?
  • 18:06 - 18:09
    (female teacher) Suppose I had a box
  • 18:09 - 18:14
    and I put one marble outside the box
  • 18:14 - 18:19
    and I put another marble inside the box.
  • 18:19 - 18:25
    And I ask you,
    "Which marble is near the box?"
  • 18:25 - 18:27
    Which one would you say?
  • 18:27 - 18:29
    Charlie?
  • 18:29 - 18:30
    (Charlie) Outside.
  • 18:30 - 18:30
    (female) The one outside?
  • 18:30 - 18:31
    What would you say?
  • 18:31 - 18:32
    (child) The one outside.
  • 18:32 - 18:34
    (female) Why?
  • 18:34 - 18:36
    (child) Because the one outside
    is really out.
  • 18:36 - 18:42
    The one inside is just like
    it's inside the box.
  • 18:42 - 18:44
    It isn't even near it.
  • 18:44 - 18:47
    It's touching the box.
  • 18:47 - 18:49
    (Noam) Notice, if the box
    is a geometrical object,
  • 18:49 - 18:52
    say a sphere in the geometrical sense,
  • 18:52 - 18:53
    then the--
    or a cube--
  • 18:53 - 18:55
    the marble can be near the box.
  • 18:55 - 18:59
    The marble is near the box's surface.
  • 18:59 - 19:01
    So the geometrical object is the surface.
  • 19:01 - 19:03
    So something inside it can be near it
  • 19:03 - 19:05
    just as something outside it can be near it.
  • 19:05 - 19:09
    But we don't develop concepts
    like that naturally.
  • 19:09 - 19:12
    We have to invent them in
    some other faculty of the mind.
  • 19:12 - 19:18
    (child) The one outside
    because the one inside isn't near.
  • 19:18 - 19:21
    (child) It isn't actually that
    both of them are near.
  • 19:21 - 19:25
    One is near and one is not.
  • 19:25 - 19:26
    (female) Okay, now which one is it?
  • 19:26 - 19:28
    (child) Here.
  • 19:28 - 19:31
    (Noam) The concept of the box
    that we actually develop
  • 19:31 - 19:33
    is a very obscure one.
  • 19:33 - 19:36
    It includes the interior
  • 19:36 - 19:38
    but it doesn't matter what's inside it.
  • 19:38 - 19:40
    So if the box is filled up with marbles
    or if it's just full of air,
  • 19:40 - 19:42
    it's the same box.
  • 19:42 - 19:45
    Our concept of box includes the interior
  • 19:45 - 19:47
    but it doesn't care
    what's inside the interior.
  • 19:47 - 19:50
    Now, that's an extremely abstract concept,
  • 19:50 - 19:54
    far beyond what any evidence
    could ever drive.
  • 19:54 - 19:56
    There's no evidence that
    the child has available
  • 19:56 - 20:00
    that tells the child that
    the box isn't just its surface.
  • 20:00 - 20:03
    And in fact, if we were to invent a science,
  • 20:03 - 20:07
    we would make it just a surface
    as we do in geometry and physics.
  • 20:07 - 20:08
    Now, what that means is that
  • 20:08 - 20:12
    the concept with all of
    its incredible intricacy
  • 20:12 - 20:14
    must be fixed by the mind
  • 20:14 - 20:18
    and just keyed by some trivial fact
    about the environment.
  • 20:18 - 20:20
    (female) There.
    There she goes.
  • 20:20 - 20:22
    Oh! Where did it go?
  • 20:22 - 20:24
    Where's the duck?
  • 20:24 - 20:27
    Where's the duck?
  • 20:27 - 20:31
    (male narrator) We just know
    that a box has an interior
  • 20:31 - 20:33
    but it doesn't matter what's inside.
  • 20:33 - 20:44
    ♪ [music playing]
  • 20:44 - 20:45
    What a concept.
  • 20:50 - 20:52
    (George Miller) In everyday conversation,
  • 20:52 - 20:55
    we are enormously creative.
  • 20:55 - 20:57
    (child) Boy, one more minute
    of those hot lights
  • 20:57 - 21:00
    and I'd be fried shish-kabob.
  • 21:00 - 21:04
    (George Miller) It's almost certain
    that any 20 word sentence you hear
  • 21:04 - 21:07
    will be a brand new, a newly minted,
  • 21:07 - 21:10
    newly created sentence
    that has never been uttered before
  • 21:10 - 21:13
    and probably you've never heard it before.
  • 21:13 - 21:16
    (George Carlin) Here's another one
    no one has ever said before:
  • 21:16 - 21:21
    I'm going over to the softball game
    and beat up Hitler's widow.
  • 21:21 - 21:21
    (Mark) What really--
  • 21:21 - 21:23
    What language is really about
  • 21:23 - 21:28
    is putting the words together
    into these sentences.
  • 21:28 - 21:32
    (George Miller) Suppose that it's
    possible to choose the next word
  • 21:32 - 21:36
    in what you're going to say
    as any one of a hundred words.
  • 21:36 - 21:38
    Now, make it even easier.
  • 21:38 - 21:41
    Say if any one of ten words
    that you can use to continue.
  • 21:41 - 21:45
    So the first word that you pick is one of ten.
  • 21:45 - 21:47
    And now you go on and
    you say a second word
  • 21:47 - 21:49
    and it is also one of ten.
  • 21:49 - 21:53
    So the total number of possible
    combinations of two words
  • 21:53 - 21:57
    will be ten times ten, or a hundred.
  • 21:57 - 21:59
    Now you go on to say a third word.
  • 21:59 - 22:01
    And they're ten possibilities there,
  • 22:01 - 22:06
    and that means now we have a
    thousand three-word combinations.
  • 22:06 - 22:08
    And if we go on to a fourth word,
  • 22:08 - 22:11
    there will be10,000.
  • 22:11 - 22:16
    So the number of possible messages
    is increasing very rapidly
  • 22:16 - 22:18
    as the length of the message increases.
  • 22:18 - 22:21
    (child) A little little hat,
  • 22:21 - 22:23
    and in that little little hat
  • 22:23 - 22:25
    he had a little little cat,
  • 22:25 - 22:28
    and in that little little cat
    he had a little little rat,
  • 22:28 - 22:33
    and in that little little rat
    he had a little little mouse.
  • 22:33 - 22:36
    (Howard) This is the desk that
    sits in the office that I work in.
  • 22:36 - 22:37
    Now we can take that
    still larger sentence
  • 22:37 - 22:40
    and put it together into
    a larger sentence still:
  • 22:40 - 22:40
    this is the clutter
  • 22:40 - 22:43
    that's piled on the desk
    that sits in the office that I work in.
  • 22:43 - 22:45
    There doesn't seem to be any limit on that.
  • 22:45 - 22:51
    (George Miller) How many sentences
    would we have if we had,
  • 22:51 - 22:53
    say, 20 word sentences?
  • 22:53 - 22:57
    That would be 10 to the 20th power.
  • 22:57 - 23:00
    How big a number is 10 to the 20th power?
  • 23:00 - 23:01
    [scream]
    [piano going down scale]
  • 23:01 - 23:03
    Let's see.
  • 23:03 - 23:05
    To compare with --
  • 23:05 - 23:10
    There's something like three times ten
    to the 9th seconds in a century.
  • 23:10 - 23:14
    (child) He had an eraser
    and in that little little eraser
  • 23:14 - 23:15
    there was a clock,
  • 23:15 - 23:19
    and in that little little clock
    there was a hand.
  • 23:19 - 23:20
    (female teacher) Period.
  • 23:20 - 23:22
    [children laughing]
  • 23:22 - 23:25
    (Howard) One of the basic formal
    properties that allows for this creativity
  • 23:25 - 23:28
    is the process of taking a sentence
  • 23:28 - 23:31
    and putting it inside another sentence
    to create a still larger sentence.
  • 23:31 - 23:34
    (female teacher) That was really
    really really really really long.
  • 23:34 - 23:36
    [bubbly sound]
  • 23:38 - 23:40
    (George Miller) There's no longest sentence
  • 23:40 - 23:48
    and that's why the number of possible
    sentences is countably infinite.
  • 23:48 - 23:51
    (Russell) General Alexander Haig,
    when he was Secretary of State,
  • 23:51 - 23:56
    once told a Senate foreign
    relations committee,
  • 23:56 - 24:02
    "There is a conscious castration of America's
    eyes and ears around the world."
  • 24:02 - 24:04
    Whatever that meant.
    [laughs]
  • 24:04 - 24:08
    But surely nobody could have ever
    spoken that sentence before Haig
  • 24:08 - 24:13
    and nobody would ever speak it again.
  • 24:13 - 24:14
    (male) Have a nice day.
  • 24:14 - 24:14
    (female) Nice talking to you.
  • 24:14 - 24:15
    (male) Same here.
  • 24:15 - 24:18
    (George Miller) But what
    about shorter sentences?
  • 24:18 - 24:21
    10 word sentences, five word sentences?
  • 24:21 - 24:23
    (male) Twenty-five cents.
  • 24:23 - 24:25
    five for a dollar.
  • 24:25 - 24:27
    It takes two seconds.
  • 24:27 - 24:30
    (male) Do you, Lydia, take
    Raphael in holy matrimony
  • 24:30 - 24:31
    for as long as you both shall live?
  • 24:31 - 24:32
    (female) Yes.
  • 24:32 - 24:37
    (George Miller) Of course, some short
    sentences can be used over and over and over.
  • 24:37 - 24:39
    (male) Do you, Carlos, take Raquel
    in holy matrimony
  • 24:39 - 24:41
    for as long as you both shall live?
  • 24:41 - 24:44
    Do you, Chi-Meng, take Chun-Han
    in holy matrimony
  • 24:44 - 24:45
    for as long as you both shall live?
  • 24:45 - 24:49
    Do you, Byron, take Arrisellis
    in holy matrimony
  • 24:49 - 24:51
    for as long as you both shall live?
  • 24:51 - 24:52
    (male) Five for a dollar.
  • 24:52 - 24:55
    It takes two seconds.
  • 24:55 - 24:57
    (male) As you both have
    consented to wedlock--
  • 24:57 - 24:59
    (male) But short sentences
    can be improbable too.
  • 24:59 - 25:01
    (male) I pronounce you
    husband and wife.
  • 25:01 - 25:02
    You may kiss your bride.
  • 25:06 - 25:08
    Kiss your bride, Raphael.
  • 25:10 - 25:12
    Kiss your bride.
  • 25:16 - 25:20
    (George Miller) For example, you've
    probably never heard anyone say before,
  • 25:20 - 25:22
    "Short sentences can be improbable too."
  • 25:22 - 25:25
    ♪ [music]
  • 25:25 - 25:27
    (male) Okay. Laura and Orlando.
  • 25:27 - 25:29
    (Mark) We have a kind of
    tension operating here.
  • 25:29 - 25:35
    We have a system that is
    very strict in certain ways
  • 25:35 - 25:39
    and yet one which allows you
    many degrees of freedom
  • 25:39 - 25:43
    as long as you stay
    within the bounds of the system.
  • 25:43 - 25:45
    (male) Next morning, just before dawn--
  • 25:45 - 25:48
    (Mark) You've got words,
    a finite number of words.
  • 25:48 - 25:51
    You've got a very small number of rules.
  • 25:51 - 25:55
    And between them, you can make up
    an infinite number of sentences.
  • 25:55 - 25:57
    That's the system.
  • 25:57 - 25:59
    (male) Simple.
  • 25:59 - 26:03
    (child) I got a cold.
    Good-bye.
  • 26:03 - 26:05
    (Noam) The beginning,
    the very beginnings of science
  • 26:05 - 26:07
    are always the capacity
  • 26:07 - 26:14
    to be able to be amazed
    by apparently simple things.
  • 26:14 - 26:17
    For example, if you assume that
    an apple falls to the ground
  • 26:17 - 26:20
    because that's its natural place,
  • 26:20 - 26:21
    it's not surprising.
  • 26:21 - 26:22
    You haven't done science.
  • 26:22 - 26:23
    If you begin to ask yourself,
  • 26:23 - 26:27
    "Why does the apple fall to the ground
    and not rise to the sky?"
  • 26:27 - 26:30
    If you allow yourself to be puzzled
    by that simple fact,
  • 26:30 - 26:34
    well, then you find you have a rather
    serious question before you.
  • 26:34 - 26:36
    And science begins.
  • 26:41 - 26:43
    (Frederick) How did sound and
    meaning ever get linked tougher?
  • 26:43 - 26:45
    When you think about it,
  • 26:45 - 26:47
    there's really no two things that
    are more different from each other
  • 26:47 - 26:49
    than sound and meaning.
  • 26:49 - 26:53
    Meaning, the ideas in the brain,
    you know, to put it in simple terms.
  • 26:53 - 26:57
    Sounds, these things that come out
    of our lungs and our vocal cords.
  • 26:57 - 26:59
    They're totally different.
  • 26:59 - 27:01
    How did they ever get linked.
  • 27:01 - 27:04
    What biological event
    could have created this?
  • 27:04 - 27:08
    Well, the biological event was
    the creation of grammar, syntax.
  • 27:08 - 27:09
    What grammar is
  • 27:09 - 27:13
    is a system that allows these
    mismatched components,
  • 27:13 - 27:17
    sound and meaning,
    to be linked to each other.
  • 27:17 - 27:21
    (male narrator) The first task of grammar
    is to organize words in a line,
  • 27:21 - 27:24
    one after another.
  • 27:24 - 27:28
    An event is about to take place.
  • 27:28 - 27:34
    Will we report it in the order in
    which things actually happened?
  • 27:34 - 27:38
    Is there a natural order of events?
  • 27:38 - 27:42
    A woman fell from the cable car.
  • 27:42 - 27:45
    We say the woman first,
    then the cable car.
  • 27:45 - 27:48
    Because the woman came first?
  • 27:48 - 27:53
    But surely the cable car was
    already there before the woman fell.
  • 27:55 - 27:57
    (Dan) It seems evident,
  • 27:57 - 28:00
    if you compare your thoughts and
    perceptions to the things that you say,
  • 28:00 - 28:02
    that there's a very direct relationship
  • 28:02 - 28:05
    between what you experience
    and how you talk about it.
  • 28:05 - 28:08
    But in fact, there's a great mystery
  • 28:08 - 28:10
    because when we speak,
  • 28:10 - 28:12
    we have to say things
    one word after another.
  • 28:12 - 28:15
    We have no choice.
    That's the way language works.
  • 28:15 - 28:18
    But when we see things
    and experience things,
  • 28:18 - 28:22
    they don't necessarily come
    bit by bit in a linear order.
  • 28:23 - 28:25
    (Howard) The boy kicks the ball.
  • 28:25 - 28:27
    Which comes first in reality?
  • 28:27 - 28:30
    The boy?
  • 28:30 - 28:32
    Then the kick?
  • 28:32 - 28:36
    Then the ball?
  • 28:36 - 28:39
    This is one of the puzzles
    of syntax that
  • 28:39 - 28:43
    the form of a sentence is often
    independent of its meaning.
  • 28:43 - 28:47
    (Dan) Because boy, ball, and kick
    can occur in any order in language.
  • 28:47 - 28:50
    In perception, they occur simultaneously.
  • 28:50 - 28:52
    (Howard) In fact, the order of the words
    doesn't seem to have anything to do
  • 28:52 - 28:54
    with the order of events.
  • 28:54 - 28:57
    (Dan) But we have to arrange it
    in one way or another
  • 28:57 - 28:59
    in order to speak about it.
  • 28:59 - 29:05
    One of the major tasks of using
    language is to take something
  • 29:05 - 29:08
    which is essentially atemporal, nonlinear,
  • 29:08 - 29:10
    and to arrange it in linear patterns
  • 29:10 - 29:12
    in order to make different kinds of words.
  • 29:15 - 29:19
    (male narrator) Does the form of
    a sentence depend on its meaning?
  • 29:21 - 29:23
    (Howard) There's a famous
    example sentence in linguistics
  • 29:23 - 29:26
    that Chomsky's been talking
    about since the ‘50s.
  • 29:26 - 29:30
    Colorless green ideas sleep furiously.
  • 29:30 - 29:35
    (female) Colorless green ideas
    sleep furiously.
  • 29:35 - 29:40
    (Noam) Well, a small industry has been
    spawned by one linguistic example,
  • 29:40 - 29:42
    namely "Colorless green ideas sleep furiously"
  • 29:42 - 29:46
    which has been the source
    of poems and arguments
  • 29:46 - 29:49
    and set to music and so on.
  • 29:49 - 29:51
    (male) Colorless green ideas sleep furiously.
  • 29:51 - 29:53
    (Howard) This is a very interesting sentence
  • 29:53 - 29:57
    because it shows that syntax
    can be separated from semantics.
  • 29:57 - 29:59
    That form can be separated from meaning.
  • 29:59 - 30:03
    "Colorless green ideas sleep furiously"
    doesn't seem to mean anything coherent
  • 30:03 - 30:06
    but it sounds like an English sentence.
  • 30:06 - 30:08
    If you read it back to front,
  • 30:08 - 30:11
    "Furiously sleep ideas green colorless,"
  • 30:11 - 30:14
    that wouldn't sound like English at all.
  • 30:14 - 30:22
    (child) Colorless green ideas
    sleep furiously.
  • 30:22 - 30:23
    (Noam) Well, that tells us that
  • 30:23 - 30:28
    there's more to what determines
    the structure of a sentence
  • 30:28 - 30:30
    than whether it has meaning or not.
  • 30:30 - 30:33
    (Howard) If you tried out
    the sentence on the child,
  • 30:33 - 30:34
    I bet the child would giggle.
  • 30:34 - 30:37
    [child laughing]
  • 30:37 - 30:39
    But I suspect that
    the reaction would be different
  • 30:39 - 30:44
    if you tried out "Furiously sleep
    ideas green colorless."
  • 30:44 - 30:47
    You might get a blank stare instead.
  • 30:47 - 30:51
    For a sentence to be a sentence
    of English in terms of its structure,
  • 30:51 - 30:53
    it doesn't seem to matter
    much what the words mean
  • 30:53 - 30:55
    or whether they go together
    in a meaningful way.
  • 30:55 - 30:58
    Just, they go together in a way
    that obeys the rules of syntax.
  • 31:01 - 31:03
    (Stephen) Syntax.
  • 31:03 - 31:05
    No other animal seems to have it.
  • 31:05 - 31:09
    We have to assume, I think, that
    something so distinct, so complex,
  • 31:09 - 31:15
    so much at the basis of human
    achievement is an evolved capacity.
  • 31:17 - 31:21
    (Howard) One term that
    Chomsky has used a lot
  • 31:21 - 31:23
    is the term "universal grammar."
  • 31:23 - 31:26
    And this term flows
    naturally from the idea
  • 31:26 - 31:32
    that you inherit many of the principles
    and processes involved in the grammar,
  • 31:32 - 31:35
    your knowledge of
    your particular language.
  • 31:35 - 31:36
    (male) There are 5000,
  • 31:36 - 31:39
    maybe more than 5000, languages
    spoken in the world.
  • 31:39 - 31:42
    But in fact, what Chomsky noticed
  • 31:42 - 31:48
    is that these 5000 languages of
    the world are really very, very similar.
  • 31:48 - 31:49
    (Noam) As human beings,
  • 31:49 - 31:52
    we are naturally interested
    in the differences among humans.
  • 31:52 - 31:55
    We take the similarities
    among humans for granted
  • 31:55 - 31:59
    so we're interested in the way
    humans look different from one another
  • 31:59 - 32:00
    and their faces are so different
  • 32:00 - 32:02
    and their sizes are so different
  • 32:02 - 32:05
    and the way they behave
    is so different and so on.
  • 32:05 - 32:07
    From the point of view of,
    say, some Martian,
  • 32:07 - 32:09
    we would all look alike,
  • 32:09 - 32:12
    just as from our point of view,
    all frogs look alike.
  • 32:12 - 32:14
    Now, from the point of view of the frog,
  • 32:14 - 32:16
    they probably--
    Frogs look, I'm sure,
  • 32:16 - 32:17
    very much different from one another
  • 32:17 - 32:20
    because they're interested
    in the differences among frogs.
  • 32:20 - 32:23
    We just notice the overwhelming
    respects in which they're similar.
  • 32:23 - 32:26
    Now, if we can reach the--
  • 32:26 - 32:29
    If we can make the leap of the imagination
  • 32:29 - 32:33
    that enables us to look at ourselves
    the way we look at other organisms,
  • 32:33 - 32:36
    we will quickly discover
    that we're remarkably alike.
  • 32:36 - 32:44
    [people speaking other languages]
  • 32:44 - 32:46
    (female) Mine air is much
    enamored of thy note.
  • 32:46 - 32:49
    [people speaking other languages]
  • 32:49 - 32:52
    (Lila) In fact, the differences,
  • 32:52 - 32:57
    the existing differences
    among human languages
  • 32:57 - 33:01
    has, in this sense, been called
    by Chomsky "trivial."
  • 33:01 - 33:07
    [people speaking other languages]
  • 33:07 - 33:12
    (Lila) Trivial compared
    with the differences
  • 33:12 - 33:15
    between the human
    languages taken together
  • 33:15 - 33:18
    and any other system of communication
  • 33:18 - 33:20
    by other kinds of animals,
  • 33:20 - 33:23
    by intelligent machines,
    and the like.
  • 33:23 - 33:26
    These are all vastly different
  • 33:26 - 33:28
    from the set of human languages
  • 33:28 - 33:34
    which, by comparison, are very,
    very much like each other.
  • 33:34 - 33:46
    [people speaking other languages]
  • 33:46 - 33:48
    (Dan) There are two major ground plans,
  • 33:48 - 33:50
    two great options in building a language:
  • 33:50 - 33:55
    relying on the order of words to convey
    the meaning of your overall thought.
  • 33:58 - 34:00
    (male narrator) English.
  • 34:00 - 34:03
    (Dan) Or changing the endings
    of your words one by one
  • 34:03 - 34:05
    and shuffling them around.
  • 34:08 - 34:09
    (male narrator) Latin.
  • 34:15 - 34:18
    (George Miller) For example, instead of
    counting on order as we do in English,
  • 34:18 - 34:24
    we might do it by saying,
    "Marya hit Johnum."
  • 34:24 - 34:29
    By which we mean that Mary
    is clearly the actor, the "a" word,
  • 34:29 - 34:33
    and John is clearly the one acted
    upon, the "um" word.
  • 34:33 - 34:37
    (Dan) This is simple book in English.
  • 34:37 - 34:40
    Well, in Russian it's [Russian word]
    if I hold it up and show it to you,
  • 34:40 - 34:42
    but if I'm holding it up
    or I'm reading it,
  • 34:42 - 34:45
    then it becomes [Russian word]
    rather than [Russian word].
  • 34:45 - 34:50
    We change the ending because
    when we're acting on an object,
  • 34:50 - 34:52
    we have to say it differently.
  • 34:52 - 34:56
    These endings are what
    grammarians call inflections.
  • 34:56 - 35:00
    (George A. Miller) You might wonder how
    many languages use the order ground plan,
  • 35:00 - 35:04
    how many languages use
    the inflectional ground plan.
  • 35:04 - 35:07
    And it's very, very hard to
    answer a question like that
  • 35:07 - 35:10
    because every language
    uses a little of both.
  • 35:10 - 35:14
    In English, for example,
    we use word order very much
  • 35:14 - 35:19
    but we still have inflections like
    the plural "s" at the end of words.
  • 35:19 - 35:22
    That's an inflection that
    signifies the number.
  • 35:22 - 35:26
    Over a period of time
    in the history of a language,
  • 35:26 - 35:29
    it may change from being
    a highly inflected language
  • 35:29 - 35:31
    to being a language without inflections
  • 35:31 - 35:34
    and then change back again.
  • 35:34 - 35:35
    (Dan) We see this happening
    in English now
  • 35:35 - 35:39
    if you think of the ending "'ve" on
    could've, should've, would've.
  • 35:39 - 35:40
    That "'ve" isn't a word anymore.
  • 35:40 - 35:42
    It's not "have," it's just 've".
  • 35:42 - 35:44
    It's becoming a grammatical ending.
  • 35:46 - 35:49
    (male narrator) If there are universal
    features to human language,
  • 35:49 - 35:52
    wouldn't that apply to
    all people everywhere,
  • 35:52 - 35:56
    even ancient people like the Warlpiri?
  • 35:56 - 35:59
    Their language has been closely studied.
  • 36:01 - 36:04
    What ground plan do they follow?
  • 36:08 - 36:13
    (Mary Laughren) I'm studying the language
    of the Warlpiri people of central Australia.
  • 36:13 - 36:16
    The Warlpiri people are
    the Aboriginal inhabitants
  • 36:16 - 36:20
    of a very vast area of central Australia.
  • 36:20 - 36:23
    (male narrator) Mary Laughren, field linguist,
  • 36:23 - 36:26
    has been living with the Warlpiri
    for over a decade.
  • 36:26 - 36:34
    [people speaking Warlpiri language]
  • 36:34 - 36:37
    (Mary) The Warlpiri language
    is a very interesting language.
  • 36:37 - 36:41
    Like other Australian,
    central Australian languages,
  • 36:41 - 36:45
    it has probably been spoken
    in relative isolation
  • 36:45 - 36:49
    from non-Australian languages,
  • 36:49 - 36:53
    possibly up to 60,000 years.
  • 36:53 - 36:58
    So that makes it very interesting
    for research for linguists
  • 36:58 - 37:02
    who are interested in finding out about
    differences between languages
  • 37:02 - 37:05
    but particularly, more particularly,
  • 37:05 - 37:09
    finding about what is common
    to human language.
  • 37:14 - 37:18
    (male narrator) The Warlpiri people
    were nomads until a short time ago.
  • 37:18 - 37:21
    Now they cluster in the town of Yuendumu
  • 37:21 - 37:24
    where they have adopted
    many European ways.
  • 37:24 - 37:33
    [people speaking Warlpiri language]
  • 37:33 - 37:36
    (male narrator) As nomads, they value
    things they can carry in their heads
  • 37:36 - 37:41
    instead of on their backs,
    like language.
  • 37:41 - 37:46
    Stories which can be told
    in paintings or in words.
  • 37:46 - 37:53
    [people speaking Warlpiri language]
  • 37:53 - 37:57
    (male narrator) Here, the subtle use
    of language is highly prized.
  • 37:57 - 38:02
    [people speaking Warlpiri language]
  • 38:02 - 38:06
    (male narrator) Jakomara's painting
    is the story of a whirlwind,
  • 38:06 - 38:10
    a legend going back to prehistory
    in the time they call The Dreaming.
  • 38:10 - 38:16
    [people speaking Warlpiri language]
  • 38:16 - 38:19
    (male narrator) There is an unusual
    sign language version of Warlpiri
  • 38:19 - 38:22
    for use in special situations.
  • 38:26 - 38:28
    (Mary) No. Nobody speaks
    a primitive language.
  • 38:28 - 38:31
    All language is extremely sophisticated.
  • 38:31 - 38:36
    All human language constitutes
    a very, very sophisticated system.
  • 38:36 - 38:40
    (male narrator) Warlpiri turns out to
    be an inflected word ending language
  • 38:40 - 38:43
    like Greek or Latin.
  • 38:43 - 38:48
    (Mary) That was one word
    I just typed into the dictionary there.
  • 38:48 - 38:50
    Warlpiri's typically a language
    that has very, very long words
  • 38:50 - 38:54
    made up of many, many meaningful parts.
  • 38:56 - 38:59
    Unlike English, Warlpiri
    doesn't rely on the order
  • 38:59 - 39:01
    in which words come in a sentence
  • 39:01 - 39:05
    to convey the sort of meaning
    order conveys in English.
  • 39:05 - 39:09
    What Warlpiri uses instead
    are little suffixes
  • 39:09 - 39:11
    which go onto a given word
  • 39:11 - 39:14
    and which indicates the role of that word.
  • 39:14 - 39:18
    So for example, to say,
    "The boy kicked the ball in Warlpiri,"
  • 39:18 - 39:22
    we would say something
    like [Warlpiri sentence A].
  • 39:22 - 39:24
    [Warlpiri word A] is the word for ball.
  • 39:24 - 39:26
    [Warlpiri word B] is the word for boy.
  • 39:26 - 39:29
    But to indicate that they boy
    is the doer of the action,
  • 39:29 - 39:32
    we have an extra little piece on it,
    which is [Warlpiri suffix]
  • 39:32 - 39:34
    so we say [Warlpiri word C].
  • 39:34 - 39:35
    And then we have
    the verb [Warlpiri word D]
  • 39:35 - 39:37
    and then we have [Warlpiri word E].
  • 39:37 - 39:40
    In Warlpiri, the exact same message
    would come across
  • 39:40 - 39:41
    if I changed the order of the words,
  • 39:41 - 39:47
    for example, and said
    [Warlpiri sentence A]
  • 39:47 - 39:51
    or [Warlpiri sentence B].
  • 39:51 - 39:56
    Or I could say [Warlpiri sentence C].
  • 39:56 - 39:58
    However, it doesn't matter
    what order the words are in.
  • 39:58 - 40:02
    the exact same message will
    come across, that I am saying,
  • 40:02 - 40:05
    "The boy kicked the ball."
  • 40:05 - 40:12
    [children playing]
  • 40:12 - 40:26
    ♪ [piano and bells music]
  • 40:26 - 40:29
    (Dan) However different languages
    may seem on the surface,
  • 40:29 - 40:31
    underneath they seem to be cut
    out of the very same pattern.
  • 40:31 - 40:35
    There are striking similarities
    of organization.
  • 40:35 - 40:41
    ♪ [music continues]
  • 40:41 - 40:43
    In a way, it's like the human face.
  • 40:43 - 40:45
    The human face is very simple:
    two eyes, a nose, and a mouth.
  • 40:45 - 40:47
    You can draw a simple sketch of it.
  • 40:47 - 40:49
    But look at the incredible diversity.
  • 40:49 - 40:51
    Each one of us has
    a uniquely different face,
  • 40:51 - 40:57
    yet each face is obviously
    a human face.
  • 40:57 - 40:59
    Languages are the same.
  • 40:59 - 41:01
    Each one is obviously
    a different language,
  • 41:01 - 41:04
    but they're clearly examples
    of the same kind of system.
  • 41:07 - 41:10
    (Jerry) One thing you might think to ask --
  • 41:10 - 41:12
    it's actually quite an interesting
    topic to think about--
  • 41:12 - 41:16
    is "Just how good is language
    at doing the job
  • 41:16 - 41:18
    which it's designed to do?"
  • 41:18 - 41:21
    (George Miller) Language serves
    a great many human purposes,
  • 41:21 - 41:23
    but it can't do everything.
  • 41:23 - 41:26
    (Jerry) I mean, after all, if
    it's just a biological adaptation,
  • 41:26 - 41:28
    which is what I'm suggesting,
  • 41:28 - 41:31
    then you'd expect,
    like other biological adaptations, it
  • 41:31 - 41:32
    would have--
  • 41:32 - 41:36
    there would be limits on the fit
  • 41:36 - 41:41
    between the job that it does
    and the job that it's designed to do.
  • 41:41 - 41:44
    (George Miller) Suppose I wanted
    you to meet a friend of mine
  • 41:44 - 41:47
    at the train station and
    you didn't know this friend
  • 41:47 - 41:49
    and I didn't have a photograph of him
  • 41:49 - 41:51
    and so I was trying to describe him.
  • 41:51 - 41:54
    And he's about 5 feet 8 inches tall
  • 41:54 - 41:59
    and he has brown hair
    and a very ordinary face.
  • 41:59 - 42:02
    And I'm sure he'll be wearing clothes.
  • 42:02 - 42:05
    I don't know how to describe him to you
  • 42:05 - 42:09
    but perhaps you could meet him anyhow.
  • 42:09 - 42:11
    Would you please?
  • 42:11 - 42:13
    ♪ [music playing]
  • 42:13 - 42:16
    (female) Something which
    turns around and comes down.
  • 42:16 - 42:19
    (male) How can you explain
    the concept of a spiral?
  • 42:19 - 42:23
    You almost have to make
    use of a gesture like that.
  • 42:23 - 42:24
    (female) Can I use my hands?
  • 42:24 - 42:25
    (female) Sure.
  • 42:27 - 42:32
    (George Miller) I give advice.
    If I say, "There's a leafy tree,"
  • 42:32 - 42:34
    well, how leafy is it?
  • 42:34 - 42:42
    (female) Something that twists
    around in rotating circles.
  • 42:42 - 42:43
    (male) Circular.
  • 42:43 - 42:45
    Circular motion.
    It's kinda like a slinky.
  • 42:45 - 42:47
    (George Miller) How leafy is a tree?
  • 42:47 - 42:49
    If you look at it,
    you can tell immediately.
  • 42:49 - 42:51
    But trying to describe it,
  • 42:51 - 42:54
    I can't count the leaves and
    tell you how many there are.
  • 42:54 - 42:56
    (Jerry) Can one imagine a system
  • 42:56 - 42:59
    that might communicate thought
    better than natural languages?
  • 42:59 - 43:00
    Well, certainly, there's some purpose.
  • 43:00 - 43:03
    (George Carlin) For instance,
    giving directions to someone.
  • 43:03 - 43:06
    Language is not as good as handing
    them a map and saying,
  • 43:06 - 43:08
    "Look, I live here.
    You go right down like that."
  • 43:08 - 43:09
    (male) Go down.
  • 43:09 - 43:11
    To North Harrison Street.
  • 43:11 - 43:13
    Just drawing it now.
  • 43:13 - 43:15
    Harrison Street, to Route 1.
  • 43:15 - 43:19
    (David) Someone could describe
    a spiral without using gestures
  • 43:19 - 43:23
    but it would take a rather complex,
  • 43:23 - 43:26
    a series of sentences to do it.
  • 43:26 - 43:31
    (male) A loop which proceeds
    in two dimensions.
  • 43:31 - 43:34
    It's got at rotational component
  • 43:34 - 43:39
    and the component going
    transverse to that direction.
  • 43:39 - 43:40
    (male) Make a left.
  • 43:40 - 43:43
    Go down, you'll see
    the state police barracks
  • 43:43 - 43:45
    right on the left hand side.
  • 43:45 - 43:46
    (George Carlin) Jonathan
    Windows used to say,
  • 43:46 - 43:49
    "You go on down about a good
    hundred miles and take a right."
  • 43:49 - 43:52
    (male) It's back about 250 yards.
  • 43:52 - 43:54
    Go through that guard booth.
  • 43:54 - 43:55
    (George Carlin) Or is to the left.
  • 43:55 - 44:00
    (Noam) The very nature of language
    design leads to the consequence
  • 44:00 - 44:03
    that there are thoughts
    which are perfectly fine thoughts
  • 44:03 - 44:08
    that are not expressible directly
    through the mechanisms of language.
  • 44:08 - 44:11
    Language is a system with structure,
  • 44:11 - 44:15
    and any system with structure
    and design has scope and limits.
  • 44:15 - 44:18
    If it had no intrinsic structure and design,
  • 44:18 - 44:19
    it could do nothing.
  • 44:19 - 44:19
    It would achieve nothing. It
  • 44:19 - 44:21
    would be useless for anything.
  • 44:21 - 44:24
    But the very structure that it has
  • 44:24 - 44:26
    not only provides it with its capacities
  • 44:26 - 44:28
    but also imposes certain limits on it.
  • 44:28 - 44:33
    So, for example, take a tool,
    a physical tool, say a hammer.
  • 44:33 - 44:35
    If it was just an amorphous blob,
  • 44:35 - 44:37
    you couldn't do anything with it.
  • 44:37 - 44:39
    If it's a hammer with
    a particular structure,
  • 44:39 - 44:40
    you can do certain things with it
  • 44:40 - 44:42
    but you can't do other things with it.
  • 44:42 - 44:46
    If a tool is going to be useful
    for any purpose,
  • 44:46 - 44:47
    it's going to have to have structure
  • 44:47 - 44:51
    and that's going to prevent it from
    being used for other purposes.
  • 44:51 - 44:52
    (Jerry) On the other hand,
  • 44:52 - 45:00
    there's a lot of expressive power
    built into our linguistic capacity
  • 45:00 - 45:02
    that's easy to overlook.
  • 45:02 - 45:04
    I mean, people go around
    telling you things like--
  • 45:04 - 45:06
    Not only do maps
  • 45:06 - 45:08
    show you how to get there easier
    than you can describe it,
  • 45:08 - 45:11
    but moreover, a single picture
    is worth thousands of words
  • 45:11 - 45:14
    and all that sort of stuff.
  • 45:14 - 45:15
    In a sense, it's true.
  • 45:15 - 45:17
    In a sense, it's also not.
  • 45:17 - 45:20
    We can do things of
    great subtlety with language
  • 45:20 - 45:23
    that it's very hard to imagine
    doing in any other way.
  • 45:25 - 45:30
    (George Miller) Much of the subtlety of
    language comes, of course, from the syntax.
  • 45:30 - 45:33
    But there's also subtlety
    in the words themselves.
  • 45:33 - 45:36
    Words can be associated
    arbitrarily with anything.
  • 45:36 - 45:38
    We think of them being
    associated with objects,
  • 45:38 - 45:40
    but they can be associated with ideas,
  • 45:40 - 45:42
    which abstractions,
    with anything you like.
  • 45:42 - 45:46
    Words can be associated
    with anything you can think of.
  • 45:46 - 45:48
    (Mark) Language is arbitrary.
  • 45:48 - 45:51
    There's no connection between
    the thing and what we call the thing.
  • 45:51 - 45:53
    It's abstract.
  • 45:53 - 45:56
    You can talk about things
    that really aren't there.
  • 45:56 - 45:57
    Things that have no form.
  • 45:57 - 45:59
    A word like "form," for example.
  • 45:59 - 46:02
    Now what is the meaning
    of the word "form"?
  • 46:02 - 46:04
    Or a word like "truth."
  • 46:04 - 46:07
    That terrible word that
    philosophers have discussed
  • 46:07 - 46:09
    for so many thousands of years.
  • 46:09 - 46:10
    What is truth?
  • 46:10 - 46:11
    We don't know.
  • 46:11 - 46:12
    We just have a word for it.
  • 46:12 - 46:14
    It's an arbitrary word.
  • 46:15 - 46:17
    (female) Love.
  • 46:17 - 46:20
    What does love mean?
  • 46:20 - 46:22
    What's love?
  • 46:22 - 46:24
    (Mark) So language is completely arbitrary.
  • 46:24 - 46:27
    Words are arbitrary.
  • 46:27 - 46:28
    (male) It can be anything you want.
  • 46:28 - 46:29
    Is that a light?
  • 46:29 - 46:31
    That's a lulu.
  • 46:31 - 46:32
    Turn the lulu on.
  • 46:32 - 46:34
    (Mark) Even little kids know that.
  • 46:34 - 46:37
    So my son, for example,
    who is four years old.
  • 46:37 - 46:39
    He comes down for breakfast,
  • 46:39 - 46:41
    he sits down in his chair,
  • 46:41 - 46:44
    and he says, "I want gump for breakfast."
  • 46:44 - 46:47
    And you say, "Peter, what is gump?"
  • 46:47 - 46:50
    And he looks and he says,
    "Gump is cereal."
  • 46:50 - 46:53
    (Noam) So, for example,
    whether I call a tree
  • 46:53 - 46:59
    a tree or whether I call it a Baum as I would
    if I grew up in Germany somewhere,
  • 46:59 - 47:01
    that's obviously variable.
  • 47:01 - 47:03
    (Mark) The thing and what you
    call a thing have nothing at all
  • 47:03 - 47:05
    to do with one another.
  • 47:05 - 47:07
    (child) Dinosaur.
  • 47:07 - 47:10
    [man speaking other language]
  • 47:10 - 47:11
    (George Carlin) Pancakes.
  • 47:11 - 47:13
    (female) Infrastructure.
  • 47:13 - 47:16
    (George Miller) Different languages
    have totally different vocabularies.
  • 47:16 - 47:19
    Different names for the same things.
  • 47:19 - 47:22
    Different names for different things,
    depending on what they use.
  • 47:22 - 47:24
    (male) Words for camel.
  • 47:24 - 47:25
    (Michael) The Arabs have dozens
  • 47:25 - 47:28
    if not scores of words for camel and lion
  • 47:28 - 47:31
    and horse and other elements
    of their civilization.
  • 47:31 - 47:34
    (Mary) The vocabulary of
    each language reflects
  • 47:34 - 47:37
    the way of life of the people
    who speak that language.
  • 47:37 - 47:39
    So people obviously need words
    to talk about things
  • 47:39 - 47:41
    they want to talk about.
  • 47:41 - 47:43
    (George Miller) If you live at the North Pole,
  • 47:43 - 47:45
    bamboo is not important to you.
  • 47:45 - 47:48
    (male) [word in other language]
    is a skinny camel.
  • 47:48 - 47:53
    [word in other language]
    is a young female camel.
  • 47:53 - 47:54
    (Mark) There's got to be
    a word for everything.
  • 47:54 - 47:57
    Every little piece in this boat,
    you've got a different word for it.
  • 47:57 - 47:57
    You've got all these lines.
  • 47:57 - 48:00
    Each one's got a different name for it.
  • 48:00 - 48:03
    (sailor) This particular one is
    the halyard for the spinnaker.
  • 48:03 - 48:08
    And over here, the halyard for
    the burgee to fly a yacht club
  • 48:08 - 48:12
    or other identifying flag
    from the top of the mast.
  • 48:13 - 48:20
    (male) [word in other language] is
    a camel used for transporting loads.
  • 48:20 - 48:23
    [word in other language]
    is used for camels in general.
  • 48:23 - 48:25
    (male) That horizontal appendage
  • 48:25 - 48:30
    projecting from the right side of
    the dress plate is the lances rest.
  • 48:30 - 48:33
    The shoulders are covered
    by the pauldrons.
  • 48:33 - 48:37
    The arms, including the upper and
    lower arms and bend of the elbow
  • 48:37 - 48:39
    are covered by the vanbraces.
  • 48:39 - 48:42
    The left and right vanbrace.
  • 48:42 - 48:46
    (sailor) And so we have a topping lift
    to hold up the end of the pole.
  • 48:46 - 48:51
    We have the halyard and a
    downhaul on the butt of the pole
  • 48:51 - 48:54
    to raise and lower it
    on the track on the mast.
  • 48:54 - 48:58
    (male) The shield is known
    as the escutcheon.
  • 49:01 - 49:07
    And the steel covered shoe
    is known as the sabotin.
  • 49:07 - 49:09
    (female) What do you call these camels?
  • 49:09 - 49:13
    (male) Well, I call them Beat,
    Jess, Tom, Joe, Jerry, and Billy.
  • 49:13 - 49:19
    [people speaking other language]
  • 49:19 - 49:24
    (Henry) The Czech word
    for ostrich is pstros.
  • 49:24 - 49:25
    (female) Bone.
  • 49:25 - 49:26
    (female) Grody.
  • 49:26 - 49:28
    [woman speaking other language]
  • 49:28 - 49:30
    (female) Elliptical.
  • 49:30 - 49:32
    (male) Lunch.
  • 49:32 - 49:36
    Pumpkin.
  • 49:36 - 49:47
    (male) We just have a word for it
    so we can talk about it.
  • 49:47 - 49:47
    ♪ [music playing]
  • 49:47 - 49:49
    (Jerry) Roughly speaking,
  • 49:49 - 49:51
    the achievement of language
  • 49:51 - 49:54
    is to provide a form of words
    that allows us to say
  • 49:54 - 49:56
    anything we can think of.
  • 49:56 - 50:01
    (Thomas) You can make all kinds
    of statements about conditions
  • 50:01 - 50:02
    as you wish them to be,
  • 50:02 - 50:04
    conditions as they might be,
  • 50:04 - 50:08
    conditions as they were once upon a time,
  • 50:08 - 50:10
    or conditions as they
    will be in the future.
  • 50:10 - 50:14
    An animal cannot make
    this kind of statement
  • 50:14 - 50:16
    because this kind of
    statement is possible
  • 50:16 - 50:19
    only by means of language.
  • 50:19 - 50:21
    (Jerry) Consider, for example,
  • 50:21 - 50:23
    negation, right.
  • 50:23 - 50:27
    It's easy to tell somebody
    that it's not going to rain.
  • 50:27 - 50:31
    Try drawing them a picture
    of "It's not going to rain."
  • 50:31 - 50:33
    Or try drawing them a picture of
  • 50:33 - 50:37
    "If I had let the hammer go,
    it would have fallen on my toe."
  • 50:37 - 50:39
    (George Carlin) In language, you can say,
  • 50:39 - 50:40
    "If we drop this piano,
  • 50:40 - 50:42
    it's not going to play a polonaise."
  • 50:42 - 50:48
    (Jerry) Think about a giraffe
    standing beside me.
  • 50:48 - 50:55
    (Thomas) Here we are communicating
    about a world of our imagination.
  • 50:55 - 50:59
    Imagination, if you like,
    is also a human construct.
  • 50:59 - 51:01
    (Frederick) The edge that
    language gave us
  • 51:01 - 51:03
    that no other animal has,
  • 51:03 - 51:04
    no other being has,
  • 51:04 - 51:06
    is the ability to think abstractly.
  • 51:06 - 51:08
    That's really what language
    can do for us.
  • 51:12 - 51:14
    We seem to have this drive
    to think abstractly.
  • 51:14 - 51:16
    We're propelled to do it.
  • 51:16 - 51:18
    It's not even a choice.
  • 51:20 - 51:22
    Because language itself is abstract,
  • 51:22 - 51:26
    we seem to be led to think abstractly.
  • 51:26 - 51:28
    Because we can think abstractly,
  • 51:28 - 51:29
    we don't have to talk about
  • 51:29 - 51:33
    what's in our immediate
    physical environment.
  • 51:33 - 51:35
    We can plan for the future.
  • 51:38 - 51:42
    We can create art.
  • 51:42 - 51:47
    We can create complex
    social organizations.
  • 51:47 - 51:52
    All of these things are possible
    because we can think abstractly.
  • 51:52 - 52:01
    [bells music in background]
  • 52:01 - 52:09
    (female) Since my house burned down,
  • 52:09 - 52:15
    I now have a better view
    of the rising moon.
  • 52:15 - 52:27
    ♪ [woman singing in Xhosa]
  • 52:27 - 52:29
    (Dan) Language, like any
    complex human phenomenon,
  • 52:29 - 52:32
    can be studied from many
    different points of view.
  • 52:32 - 52:36
    And contemporary linguists specialize.
  • 52:36 - 52:38
    Some like to study words
    and their structures
  • 52:38 - 52:41
    and see how the structures
    of words affect sentences.
  • 52:41 - 52:44
    Some say the place to go is to work
    on the structure of sentences
  • 52:44 - 52:47
    and worry later about
    what the words mean.
  • 52:47 - 52:49
    Some people say unless you
    look at the role of language
  • 52:49 - 52:51
    in conversations, in stories
  • 52:51 - 52:52
    and longer stretches of discourse,
  • 52:52 - 52:56
    you'll never really understand
    what language is about.
  • 52:56 - 52:58
    Now, in a sense, of course,
    they're all right.
  • 52:58 - 53:01
    (Frederick) We can't cut open
    the brain and look at structures
  • 53:01 - 53:04
    that are involved in visual
    perception or in memory,
  • 53:04 - 53:06
    but we do have language.
  • 53:06 - 53:08
    We do have something almost physical.
  • 53:08 - 53:11
    We have the record of what people say
  • 53:11 - 53:13
    and we can analyze that as data.
  • 53:13 - 53:17
    So linguistics provides us with
    the most accessible window
  • 53:17 - 53:18
    into the human mind.
  • 53:18 - 53:21
    (Jerry) We have this remarkable system
  • 53:21 - 53:25
    that allows us to choose a form of words
  • 53:25 - 53:27
    corresponding to the thought
    that we want to express.
  • 53:27 - 53:31
    And somehow, it's a system
    we manage to
  • 53:31 - 53:33
    learn within a very short period of time,
  • 53:33 - 53:35
    if we're a child in a language community,
  • 53:35 - 53:37
    and that we manage to access,
  • 53:37 - 53:39
    to exploit both from the point
    of view of the speaker
  • 53:39 - 53:40
    and from the point of view of the hearer,
  • 53:40 - 53:43
    instantaneously, easily, unconsciously,
  • 53:43 - 53:45
    with no sense of effort.
  • 53:45 - 53:49
    (Lila) So if you wanted to ask,
    "What's the answer to the question?
  • 53:49 - 53:54
    Is language simple or
    is language complex?"
  • 53:54 - 53:58
    It's simple for those creatures who use it.
  • 53:58 - 54:04
    That is, the human brain
    is naturally evolved
  • 54:04 - 54:10
    so that it uses a system of
    this apparently complex sort
  • 54:10 - 54:13
    in a very simple way.
  • 54:19 - 54:21
    (male) If a child is in an environment
  • 54:21 - 54:24
    in which language interactions
    are proceeding,
  • 54:24 - 54:28
    that language will grow
    in the child's mind
  • 54:28 - 54:34
    but very much in the manner of
    other aspects of physical growth.
  • 54:34 - 54:37
    (female) I take it as the current
    task of linguistics
  • 54:37 - 54:40
    to ask the question about language:
  • 54:40 - 54:43
    how much of it is built in
    and in what way?
  • 54:43 - 54:47
    And how much of it is
    and must be learned
  • 54:47 - 54:54
    by exposure to the environment
    of speaking people?
  • 54:54 - 54:58
    (male) A question for the next in
    our series on the human language:
  • 54:58 - 55:03
    how do children acquire language
    without seeming to learn it?
  • 55:03 - 55:06
    Part 2: Playing the Language Game.
Title:
Human Language 1 Colorless Green Ideas
Video Language:
English
Duration:
56:05

English subtitles

Revisions