-
(announcer) The Human Language series
-
was funded by the National Endowment
for the Humanities,
-
the National Science Foundation,
-
the Annenberg Foundation,
-
the Arthur Vining Davis Foundations,
-
the Corporation for Public Broadcasting,
-
annual financial support
from viewers like you,
-
the National Institute of Mental Health,
-
the Geraldine R. Dodge Foundation,
-
and these additional funders.
-
[dinging]
-
(George Miller) What is language?
-
Everybody knows, thank heavens.
-
[people talking]
-
(female) Is language simple
or is language complex?
-
(George Carlin) The girl that
that's the house of's father.
-
(narrator) This is Part 1 of a series
on the human language.
-
(male) The question is: how can
you learn so much from so little?
-
(Roy) The question is: how does
a child know what a word is?
-
(Lewis) How is it that we can
so alter our thought patterns
-
to fit into the linear arrangement
of any particular arrangement?
-
[girls speaking]
-
(female) I thought
you had already broke up.
-
(female #1) No, he broke up with me
before I could break up with him.
-
(narrator) What specific biological
endowment could we have
-
that allows us to do this?
-
[man shouting]
-
[children shouting]
-
(female) Is there something true,
-
some core of properties true
of all human languages?
-
[man shouting]
-
[children shouting]
-
[people speaking different languages]
-
(Noam Chomsky) How do people
simply interact with language?
-
How do people do
what I am now doing?
-
Or if you meet somebody,
say at a bus stop,
-
and you start having a conversation,
-
how do you do it?
-
(woman) Take care of yourself.
-
(Mark) People don't think when they speak,
they just speak.
-
(narrator) This is a report
on a study of an enigma.
-
[people talking in background]
-
Language is the most human thing
there is about being human,
-
yet most people don't think much about it.
-
Traditionally, linguists have
studied the history, vocabulary,
-
and grammar of the world's languages,
-
but within our generation,
-
new theories have
revolutionized our thinking.
-
Today, linguists ask questions
about how we use language
-
and how language functions
inside the human mind.
-
For example, why are languages
filled with rules we all follow
-
without knowing why?
-
(female) How would you describe this?
-
(female) Well, I know it's supposed
to be a big red balloon.
-
(female) How about a red big balloon?
-
Could this be a red big balloon?
-
(male) A big red balloon.
-
(female) Okay.
-
Do you have any idea why you feel
more comfortable saying a big --
-
Do you have a sense of why that feels
better than a red big balloon?
-
(male) I have no reason why.
-
(male) Three big red round plastic balls.
-
Not big three round plastic red big balls.
-
(narrator) Linguists claim that
languages is rule governed.
-
What rules?
-
(George Carlin) Stop and go traffic.
-
Should be go and stop traffic.
You can't stop until you've gone.
-
[cars honking]
-
(Mark) You can re-climb
and you can re-institutionalize,
-
but you can't re-fall down.
-
[chuckles]
I don't know why that is.
-
It's peculiar.
-
Why can't you re-fall down?
-
(Howard) And you can say things like
"Don't rouse up the natives,"
-
but you can't say,
"Don't rouse up them."
-
You have to say,
"Don't rouse them up."
-
(Russell) Language is untidy.
-
Language is a mess.
-
(male) "That John left surprised me."
-
What's the "that" doing there?
-
I mean, everybody knows you can't say,
-
"John left surprised me."
-
There's something wrong with that.
-
The rules of English say you
got to have a "that" in front.
-
Well, why?
-
[female speaking French]
-
(female) Could you do it
the other way around?
-
[female speaking French]
-
(female) And is that fine?
-
(female) No, it's not.
-
The first one is fine,
-
the other one you just will never say it.
-
(child) A big red balloon.
-
(Mark) Those are just
the rules of my language
-
that I learned totally
unconsciously as a little child.
-
(female) It's the same old story.
-
(George Carlin) What's going on in here?
-
(female) It's the old same story?
-
(narrator) Who or what tells us
how to say it?
-
(Noam) The language faculty is a
system, a subsystem, of the brain,
-
of the human brain.
-
Its major elements don't appear
to exist in other similar organisms.
-
And that organ of the brain yields
a language under appropriate conditions,
-
which is partially tuned
by the environment
-
but to a large extent
appears to be determined
-
by our biological endowment
-
and is essential invariant
across the species.
-
(Frederick) There have really
been three major revolutions
-
in modern linguistics.
-
The first, about 200 years ago,
-
found change is irregular.
-
That you can reconstruct
ancient languages
-
and talk about how
language has changed
-
from the ancient times to the present.
-
The second was at the turn
of this century
-
where it was discovered that
languages themselves are systematic.
-
That there are rules governing
how languages are put together.
-
The third revolution came
with Noam Chomsky
-
in his book Syntactic Structures in 1957.
-
(Howard Lasnik) In 1957, Chomsky published
this little book, Syntactic Structures.
-
And it almost immediately
revolutionized the field.
-
It had a profound influence on
everyone who read it carefully,
-
and the reason for this is the beauty of
the analyses that Chomsky presented.
-
He was able to show that there were
patterns when it had just looked like
-
there were many separate facts
-
and he was able to come up
with very simple rules
-
for explaining these patterns.
-
(Frederick) Basically what
Chomsky said was
-
that your sights aren't set high enough.
-
You're asking the wrong questions.
-
You're not asking the most
interesting questions.
-
What you're asking is,
-
"Well, we know language is a system
so how should we divide it up?
-
Where should we draw
the line in a word,
-
where should we draw the line
in the sentence,
-
what are the pieces?"
-
Chomsky said,
"Let's ask a different question.
-
Let's ask a much more ambitious question.
-
Let's ask the question, 'What is
a possible human language?' "
-
(Howard) What was so appealing
about this analysis
-
and what was so revolutionary about it
-
is it showed that there's
some process in your head,
-
some rule, some pattern
that you have abstractly.
-
And you use this rule in producing
sentences and in understanding sentences.
-
That is, he moved linguistics away
-
from merely gathering
and investigating sentences
-
that have already been produced
-
and moved it towards the investigation
of human potentiality.
-
What kinds of sentences
can you produce,
-
what kinds of sentences
can you understand?
-
[people talking in background]
-
(Frederick) Once you say,
-
"What is a possible human language?"
-
in a way you're saying,
"What is a possible human being?"
-
How does the human mind work?
-
[woman speaking Xhosa language]
-
(Noam) Well, the most elementary
property of human language
-
that one can imagine
-
is that we can create new expressions
which express new thoughts.
-
So for example, the sentences
that I'm now producing,
-
you may not have heard,
-
I may not have said before.
-
Maybe they're new in the history
of the human race.
-
(George Carlin) Here's something
no one has ever said before:
-
Toss that anchor over here,
-
I have room for two or three
more of them in my hip pocket.
-
(Lila) The point of a language
is to be able to say things
-
you've never said before
and never heard before
-
and to understand things
the first time you hear them
-
that have never been said
before to your knowledge
-
and maybe never been said before at all.
-
But if you're going to be able to say
and understand new things,
-
you must have acquired something
very general, a system.
-
(George Miller) There are, perhaps,
in English, there are about 40 sounds.
-
The sounds get combined to form --
-
well, most people know 40,000 words,
something of that sort.
-
(George Carlin) Swimming.
-
(female) Cacophony
-
(Mark) Antidisestablishmentarianism.
-
(male) Lunch.
-
(George Miller) Words are built out of sounds
-
and there are a limited number of them.
-
Sentences are built out of words
-
and there's no limit to
the number of sentences
-
that you can compose in a language.
-
(Mark) You could say
that words are like atoms.
-
The way atoms are in chemistry,
as the basic building blocks,
-
those are what the words are.
-
They're fairly solid things
and you take these atoms
-
and you put them together
to make up the molecules,
-
or the sentences.
-
(Russell) I remember Senator
Joseph McCarthy at one time saying,
-
"That's the most unheard
of thing I've ever heard of."
-
[laughs]
-
Nobody could have invented
that sentence before McCarthy spoke it.
-
(Mark) It's really a deceptively
simple system.
-
The genius of the system is that
with a small number of words
-
and this system of grammar,
-
you can make up an infinite
number of sentences.
-
You can make up a sentence
to fit any occasion.
-
(child) And mommy just goes
to sleep but Daddy just goes--
-
Daddy just, um--
-
(male narrator) The system starts with words.
-
Deceptively simple.
-
But the question is: what is a word?
-
How does a child know what a word is?
-
(child) --to his friend's house.
-
(Judith) You could ask somebody,
"What is a word?"
-
And most people think they know
what a word is until you ask them.
-
[child laughing]
-
(female) I word is, uh‚ --
-
(George Carlin): A word is --
-
(Judith Klavans) And you're
trying to find it
-
and you can't put your finger on it.
-
You don't know.
-
(Jeff) A word is--
-
[man speaking other language]
-
(child) Pencil.
-
(female) Language.
-
A word is something that someone
thought up a long, long time ago.
-
What is a word?
-
That's actually an extraordinarily
difficult question.
-
(Lewis) I haven't the ghost
of an idea.
-
I can't even make up
a plausible evasion.
-
(Suzette) You could say a word
is the smallest separate piece
-
of a language that, all by itself,
-
will have a meaning.
-
(male narrator) On the other hand.
-
(Howard) There are words that
are composed, themselves,
-
of smaller units of bare meaning,
like "kicked",
-
where "kick" has a meaning and
"ed" has another meaning,
-
like past tense.
-
(Judith) And I didn't do that.
-
"n't" is it a word?
-
(male) One of my favorite words is
antidisestablishmentarianism.
-
It's an interesting word,
of course, because everybody thinks
-
that it's the longest word
in the English language.
-
In fact, there are many words in English
-
that are far longer than antidisestablishmentarianism.
-
Most of them chemical terms.
-
(Roy) You know, the longest
word in Webster's 7th
-
is pneumonoultramicroscopicsilicovolca
nokoniosis,
-
a disease of the lungs.
-
(Mark) Infixes and prefixes and suffixes.
-
(George Carlin) I used to wonder:
-
pre- and suf- are prefixes.
-
Suf- is the prefix of suffix.
-
Is -fix the suffix for prefix and suffix?
-
(Mark) Words seem to
be somehow indivisible,
-
but you look at them closely
-
and they've got all kinds
of little parts to them.
-
(female) What's this?
-
I know there's a word for this.
-
(Alvin) I think, for example,
of the word apodictic.
-
Now, that is not only illegal
phonological structure,
-
I happen to know it's a word
-
because I've seen it in print
-
and I've even looked it up
in the dictionary,
-
but I simply can't remember
what it means.
-
(Howard) I haven't a clue
what apodictic means.
-
(Mujinga) [speaking African language]
-
I'm from Africa. Zaire.
-
Mujinga means a child born with
an umbilical cord all over her body.
-
(Lewis) Most words certainly
are complicated words,
-
like human or like generous,
-
are really small tiny sentences
all wrapped up
-
in one set of phonemes.
-
[Dan speaking Turkish]
-
That's a single Turkish word.
-
One of my favorite words
-
because it comes out as
a whole sentence in English.
-
It means, "Are you one of those that
we haven't been able to Europeanize?"
-
[woman speaking Xhosa language]
-
(Jeff) One example from
the Eskimo language I work with,
-
which is called Aleutic,
-
is the word [Aleutic word]
-
which means, "Don't you want to
go window shopping with me?"
-
(Roy) The easy thing to say,
-
for me since I deal with computers a lot,
-
is that a word is the sequence
of letters between two blanks.
-
(Mark) But what about when we speak?
-
We don't have little spaces
when we speak,
-
we just run everything together.
-
(George Carlin) O-m-m-i-n-a.
-
Ommina.
-
It means, "I am going to."
-
[people singing and laughing]
-
(Suzette) You know,
when you listen to people talk,
-
unless they deliberately pause
-
between the pieces
of what they're saying,
-
it all runs together.
-
(George Carlin) How do we know where
the gaps are in between the words?
-
Because this all sounds
like one whole sound.
-
This is just one long sound to me.
-
But you know that I said,
"This is just one long sound to me."
-
(male) Why are you weary?
-
Why are you weary?
-
(Mark) I walk up to you and say,
-
"Steal it!" or "Teach it!"
-
(male) What'd you want to do now?
-
(female) I don't know,
what do you want to do?
-
(male) Dunno.
-
An apple?
-
(girl) Yeah.
-
(male) Okay, so will you go home now?
-
Okay.
-
(George Carlin) I'm going to go
get the bus and go home.
-
(Mark) This may be the hardest thing
that the brain ever has to do.
-
Is to figure out those
boundaries at the words
-
that people have just
run together in speech.
-
(male) Why are you weary?
-
Why are you weary?
-
(Mark) People have invested
millions of dollars
-
and Lord knows how many hours
in trying to devise machinery
-
that will be able to perform this task
-
of breaking spoken
language up into words
-
so that it can be understood.
-
And yet, a child, by the age of one,
-
is perfectly able to understand speech,
-
natural speech,
-
and just pick it apart into words.
-
[child babbling]
-
(Mark) So what's a word?
-
They're very slippery objects.
-
Difficult to deal with.
-
[child babbling]
-
(Mark) Words stand for things.
-
The word "tree" stands for a tree.
-
But words are more than that.
-
After all, the word "tree"
doesn't just stand for that tree
-
or even any individual tree.
-
It stands for the general notion of a tree.
-
The concept of a tree.
-
That's what Plato would have told us.
-
But then that raises
the question of concept.
-
What's a concept?
-
(male narrator) What do we have
in our minds before we speak?
-
Are there concepts
we do not need to invent
-
because we are born
already knowing them?
-
Plato said so and so does Chomsky.
-
(Noam) Well, suppose I have, say, a box
-
and I have a marble inside the box
and a marble outside the box.
-
And I ask, "Which marble
is near to the box?"
-
Well, the answer is that
the marble outside the box
-
is either near the box
or not near it,
-
but the marble in the box
can't be near it.
-
But again, that's one
of those simple things
-
that we ought to be surprised about.
-
How does a child know that a marble
that's in a box can't be near the box?
-
(female teacher) Suppose I had a box
-
and I put one marble outside the box
-
and I put another marble inside the box.
-
And I ask you,
"Which marble is near the box?"
-
Which one would you say?
-
Charlie?
-
(Charlie) Outside.
-
(female) The one outside?
-
What would you say?
-
(child) The one outside.
-
(female) Why?
-
(child) Because the one outside
is really out.
-
The one inside is just like
it's inside the box.
-
It isn't even near it.
-
It's touching the box.
-
(Noam) Notice, if the box
is a geometrical object,
-
say a sphere in the geometrical sense,
-
then the--
or a cube--
-
the marble can be near the box.
-
The marble is near the box's surface.
-
So the geometrical object is the surface.
-
So something inside it can be near it
-
just as something outside it can be near it.
-
But we don't develop concepts
like that naturally.
-
We have to invent them in
some other faculty of the mind.
-
(child) The one outside
because the one inside isn't near.
-
(child) It isn't actually that
both of them are near.
-
One is near and one is not.
-
(female) Okay, now which one is it?
-
(child) Here.
-
(Noam) The concept of the box
that we actually develop
-
is a very obscure one.
-
It includes the interior
-
but it doesn't matter what's inside it.
-
So if the box is filled up with marbles
or if it's just full of air,
-
it's the same box.
-
Our concept of box includes the interior
-
but it doesn't care
what's inside the interior.
-
Now, that's an extremely abstract concept,
-
far beyond what any evidence
could ever drive.
-
There's no evidence that
the child has available
-
that tells the child that
the box isn't just its surface.
-
And in fact, if we were to invent a science,
-
we would make it just a surface
as we do in geometry and physics.
-
Now, what that means is that
-
the concept with all of
its incredible intricacy
-
must be fixed by the mind
-
and just keyed by some trivial fact
about the environment.
-
(female) There.
There she goes.
-
Oh! Where did it go?
-
Where's the duck?
-
Where's the duck?
-
(male narrator) We just know
that a box has an interior
-
but it doesn't matter what's inside.
-
♪ [music playing]
-
What a concept.
-
(George Miller) In everyday conversation,
-
we are enormously creative.
-
(child) Boy, one more minute
of those hot lights
-
and I'd be fried shish-kabob.
-
(George Miller) It's almost certain
that any 20 word sentence you hear
-
will be a brand new, a newly minted,
-
newly created sentence
that has never been uttered before
-
and probably you've never heard it before.
-
(George Carlin) Here's another one
no one has ever said before:
-
I'm going over to the softball game
and beat up Hitler's widow.
-
(Mark) What really--
-
What language is really about
-
is putting the words together
into these sentences.
-
(George Miller) Suppose that it's
possible to choose the next word
-
in what you're going to say
as any one of a hundred words.
-
Now, make it even easier.
-
Say if any one of ten words
that you can use to continue.
-
So the first word that you pick is one of ten.
-
And now you go on and
you say a second word
-
and it is also one of ten.
-
So the total number of possible
combinations of two words
-
will be ten times ten, or a hundred.
-
Now you go on to say a third word.
-
And they're ten possibilities there,
-
and that means now we have a
thousand three-word combinations.
-
And if we go on to a fourth word,
-
there will be10,000.
-
So the number of possible messages
is increasing very rapidly
-
as the length of the message increases.
-
(child) A little little hat,
-
and in that little little hat
-
he had a little little cat,
-
and in that little little cat
he had a little little rat,
-
and in that little little rat
he had a little little mouse.
-
(Howard) This is the desk that
sits in the office that I work in.
-
Now we can take that
still larger sentence
-
and put it together into
a larger sentence still:
-
this is the clutter
-
that's piled on the desk
that sits in the office that I work in.
-
There doesn't seem to be any limit on that.
-
(George Miller) How many sentences
would we have if we had,
-
say, 20 word sentences?
-
That would be 10 to the 20th power.
-
How big a number is 10 to the 20th power?
-
[scream]
[piano going down scale]
-
Let's see.
-
To compare with --
-
There's something like three times ten
to the 9th seconds in a century.
-
(child) He had an eraser
and in that little little eraser
-
there was a clock,
-
and in that little little clock
there was a hand.
-
(female teacher) Period.
-
[children laughing]
-
(Howard) One of the basic formal
properties that allows for this creativity
-
is the process of taking a sentence
-
and putting it inside another sentence
to create a still larger sentence.
-
(female teacher) That was really
really really really really long.
-
[bubbly sound]
-
(George Miller) There's no longest sentence
-
and that's why the number of possible
sentences is countably infinite.
-
(Russell) General Alexander Haig,
when he was Secretary of State,
-
once told a Senate foreign
relations committee,
-
"There is a conscious castration of America's
eyes and ears around the world."
-
Whatever that meant.
[laughs]
-
But surely nobody could have ever
spoken that sentence before Haig
-
and nobody would ever speak it again.
-
(male) Have a nice day.
-
(female) Nice talking to you.
-
(male) Same here.
-
(George Miller) But what
about shorter sentences?
-
10 word sentences, five word sentences?
-
(male) Twenty-five cents.
-
five for a dollar.
-
It takes two seconds.
-
(male) Do you, Lydia, take
Raphael in holy matrimony
-
for as long as you both shall live?
-
(female) Yes.
-
(George Miller) Of course, some short
sentences can be used over and over and over.
-
(male) Do you, Carlos, take Raquel
in holy matrimony
-
for as long as you both shall live?
-
Do you, Chi-Meng, take Chun-Han
in holy matrimony
-
for as long as you both shall live?
-
Do you, Byron, take Arrisellis
in holy matrimony
-
for as long as you both shall live?
-
(male) Five for a dollar.
-
It takes two seconds.
-
(male) As you both have
consented to wedlock--
-
(male) But short sentences
can be improbable too.
-
(male) I pronounce you
husband and wife.
-
You may kiss your bride.
-
Kiss your bride, Raphael.
-
Kiss your bride.
-
(George Miller) For example, you've
probably never heard anyone say before,
-
"Short sentences can be improbable too."
-
♪ [music]
-
(male) Okay. Laura and Orlando.
-
(Mark) We have a kind of
tension operating here.
-
We have a system that is
very strict in certain ways
-
and yet one which allows you
many degrees of freedom
-
as long as you stay
within the bounds of the system.
-
(male) Next morning, just before dawn--
-
(Mark) You've got words,
a finite number of words.
-
You've got a very small number of rules.
-
And between them, you can make up
an infinite number of sentences.
-
That's the system.
-
(male) Simple.
-
(child) I got a cold.
Good-bye.
-
(Noam) The beginning,
the very beginnings of science
-
are always the capacity
-
to be able to be amazed
by apparently simple things.
-
For example, if you assume that
an apple falls to the ground
-
because that's its natural place,
-
it's not surprising.
-
You haven't done science.
-
If you begin to ask yourself,
-
"Why does the apple fall to the ground
and not rise to the sky?"
-
If you allow yourself to be puzzled
by that simple fact,
-
well, then you find you have a rather
serious question before you.
-
And science begins.
-
(Frederick) How did sound and
meaning ever get linked tougher?
-
When you think about it,
-
there's really no two things that
are more different from each other
-
than sound and meaning.
-
Meaning, the ideas in the brain,
you know, to put it in simple terms.
-
Sounds, these things that come out
of our lungs and our vocal cords.
-
They're totally different.
-
How did they ever get linked.
-
What biological event
could have created this?
-
Well, the biological event was
the creation of grammar, syntax.
-
What grammar is
-
is a system that allows these
mismatched components,
-
sound and meaning,
to be linked to each other.
-
(male narrator) The first task of grammar
is to organize words in a line,
-
one after another.
-
An event is about to take place.
-
Will we report it in the order in
which things actually happened?
-
Is there a natural order of events?
-
A woman fell from the cable car.
-
We say the woman first,
then the cable car.
-
Because the woman came first?
-
But surely the cable car was
already there before the woman fell.
-
(Dan) It seems evident,
-
if you compare your thoughts and
perceptions to the things that you say,
-
that there's a very direct relationship
-
between what you experience
and how you talk about it.
-
But in fact, there's a great mystery
-
because when we speak,
-
we have to say things
one word after another.
-
We have no choice.
That's the way language works.
-
But when we see things
and experience things,
-
they don't necessarily come
bit by bit in a linear order.
-
(Howard) The boy kicks the ball.
-
Which comes first in reality?
-
The boy?
-
Then the kick?
-
Then the ball?
-
This is one of the puzzles
of syntax that
-
the form of a sentence is often
independent of its meaning.
-
(Dan) Because boy, ball, and kick
can occur in any order in language.
-
In perception, they occur simultaneously.
-
(Howard) In fact, the order of the words
doesn't seem to have anything to do
-
with the order of events.
-
(Dan) But we have to arrange it
in one way or another
-
in order to speak about it.
-
One of the major tasks of using
language is to take something
-
which is essentially atemporal, nonlinear,
-
and to arrange it in linear patterns
-
in order to make different kinds of words.
-
(male narrator) Does the form of
a sentence depend on its meaning?
-
(Howard) There's a famous
example sentence in linguistics
-
that Chomsky's been talking
about since the ‘50s.
-
Colorless green ideas sleep furiously.
-
(female) Colorless green ideas
sleep furiously.
-
(Noam) Well, a small industry has been
spawned by one linguistic example,
-
namely "Colorless green ideas sleep furiously"
-
which has been the source
of poems and arguments
-
and set to music and so on.
-
(male) Colorless green ideas sleep furiously.
-
(Howard) This is a very interesting sentence
-
because it shows that syntax
can be separated from semantics.
-
That form can be separated from meaning.
-
"Colorless green ideas sleep furiously"
doesn't seem to mean anything coherent
-
but it sounds like an English sentence.
-
If you read it back to front,
-
"Furiously sleep ideas green colorless,"
-
that wouldn't sound like English at all.
-
(child) Colorless green ideas
sleep furiously.
-
(Noam) Well, that tells us that
-
there's more to what determines
the structure of a sentence
-
than whether it has meaning or not.
-
(Howard) If you tried out
the sentence on the child,
-
I bet the child would giggle.
-
[child laughing]
-
But I suspect that
the reaction would be different
-
if you tried out "Furiously sleep
ideas green colorless."
-
You might get a blank stare instead.
-
For a sentence to be a sentence
of English in terms of its structure,
-
it doesn't seem to matter
much what the words mean
-
or whether they go together
in a meaningful way.
-
Just, they go together in a way
that obeys the rules of syntax.
-
(Stephen) Syntax.
-
No other animal seems to have it.
-
We have to assume, I think, that
something so distinct, so complex,
-
so much at the basis of human
achievement is an evolved capacity.
-
(Howard) One term that
Chomsky has used a lot
-
is the term "universal grammar."
-
And this term flows
naturally from the idea
-
that you inherit many of the principles
and processes involved in the grammar,
-
your knowledge of
your particular language.
-
(male) There are 5000,
-
maybe more than 5000, languages
spoken in the world.
-
But in fact, what Chomsky noticed
-
is that these 5000 languages of
the world are really very, very similar.
-
(Noam) As human beings,
-
we are naturally interested
in the differences among humans.
-
We take the similarities
among humans for granted
-
so we're interested in the way
humans look different from one another
-
and their faces are so different
-
and their sizes are so different
-
and the way they behave
is so different and so on.
-
From the point of view of,
say, some Martian,
-
we would all look alike,
-
just as from our point of view,
all frogs look alike.
-
Now, from the point of view of the frog,
-
they probably--
Frogs look, I'm sure,
-
very much different from one another
-
because they're interested
in the differences among frogs.
-
We just notice the overwhelming
respects in which they're similar.
-
Now, if we can reach the--
-
If we can make the leap of the imagination
-
that enables us to look at ourselves
the way we look at other organisms,
-
we will quickly discover
that we're remarkably alike.
-
[people speaking other languages]
-
(female) Mine air is much
enamored of thy note.
-
[people speaking other languages]
-
(Lila) In fact, the differences,
-
the existing differences
among human languages
-
has, in this sense, been called
by Chomsky "trivial."
-
[people speaking other languages]
-
(Lila) Trivial compared
with the differences
-
between the human
languages taken together
-
and any other system of communication
-
by other kinds of animals,
-
by intelligent machines,
and the like.
-
These are all vastly different
-
from the set of human languages
-
which, by comparison, are very,
very much like each other.
-
[people speaking other languages]
-
(Dan) There are two major ground plans,
-
two great options in building a language:
-
relying on the order of words to convey
the meaning of your overall thought.
-
(male narrator) English.
-
(Dan) Or changing the endings
of your words one by one
-
and shuffling them around.
-
(male narrator) Latin.
-
(George Miller) For example, instead of
counting on order as we do in English,
-
we might do it by saying,
"Marya hit Johnum."
-
By which we mean that Mary
is clearly the actor, the "a" word,
-
and John is clearly the one acted
upon, the "um" word.
-
(Dan) This is simple book in English.
-
Well, in Russian it's [Russian word]
if I hold it up and show it to you,
-
but if I'm holding it up
or I'm reading it,
-
then it becomes [Russian word]
rather than [Russian word].
-
We change the ending because
when we're acting on an object,
-
we have to say it differently.
-
These endings are what
grammarians call inflections.
-
(George A. Miller) You might wonder how
many languages use the order ground plan,
-
how many languages use
the inflectional ground plan.
-
And it's very, very hard to
answer a question like that
-
because every language
uses a little of both.
-
In English, for example,
we use word order very much
-
but we still have inflections like
the plural "s" at the end of words.
-
That's an inflection that
signifies the number.
-
Over a period of time
in the history of a language,
-
it may change from being
a highly inflected language
-
to being a language without inflections
-
and then change back again.
-
(Dan) We see this happening
in English now
-
if you think of the ending "'ve" on
could've, should've, would've.
-
That "'ve" isn't a word anymore.
-
It's not "have," it's just 've".
-
It's becoming a grammatical ending.
-
(male narrator) If there are universal
features to human language,
-
wouldn't that apply to
all people everywhere,
-
even ancient people like the Warlpiri?
-
Their language has been closely studied.
-
What ground plan do they follow?
-
(Mary Laughren) I'm studying the language
of the Warlpiri people of central Australia.
-
The Warlpiri people are
the Aboriginal inhabitants
-
of a very vast area of central Australia.
-
(male narrator) Mary Laughren, field linguist,
-
has been living with the Warlpiri
for over a decade.
-
[people speaking Warlpiri language]
-
(Mary) The Warlpiri language
is a very interesting language.
-
Like other Australian,
central Australian languages,
-
it has probably been spoken
in relative isolation
-
from non-Australian languages,
-
possibly up to 60,000 years.
-
So that makes it very interesting
for research for linguists
-
who are interested in finding out about
differences between languages
-
but particularly, more particularly,
-
finding about what is common
to human language.
-
(male narrator) The Warlpiri people
were nomads until a short time ago.
-
Now they cluster in the town of Yuendumu
-
where they have adopted
many European ways.
-
[people speaking Warlpiri language]
-
(male narrator) As nomads, they value
things they can carry in their heads
-
instead of on their backs,
like language.
-
Stories which can be told
in paintings or in words.
-
[people speaking Warlpiri language]
-
(male narrator) Here, the subtle use
of language is highly prized.
-
[people speaking Warlpiri language]
-
(male narrator) Jakomara's painting
is the story of a whirlwind,
-
a legend going back to prehistory
in the time they call The Dreaming.
-
[people speaking Warlpiri language]
-
(male narrator) There is an unusual
sign language version of Warlpiri
-
for use in special situations.
-
(Mary) No. Nobody speaks
a primitive language.
-
All language is extremely sophisticated.
-
All human language constitutes
a very, very sophisticated system.
-
(male narrator) Warlpiri turns out to
be an inflected word ending language
-
like Greek or Latin.
-
(Mary) That was one word
I just typed into the dictionary there.
-
Warlpiri's typically a language
that has very, very long words
-
made up of many, many meaningful parts.
-
Unlike English, Warlpiri
doesn't rely on the order
-
in which words come in a sentence
-
to convey the sort of meaning
order conveys in English.
-
What Warlpiri uses instead
are little suffixes
-
which go onto a given word
-
and which indicates the role of that word.
-
So for example, to say,
"The boy kicked the ball in Warlpiri,"
-
we would say something
like [Warlpiri sentence A].
-
[Warlpiri word A] is the word for ball.
-
[Warlpiri word B] is the word for boy.
-
But to indicate that they boy
is the doer of the action,
-
we have an extra little piece on it,
which is [Warlpiri suffix]
-
so we say [Warlpiri word C].
-
And then we have
the verb [Warlpiri word D]
-
and then we have [Warlpiri word E].
-
In Warlpiri, the exact same message
would come across
-
if I changed the order of the words,
-
for example, and said
[Warlpiri sentence A]
-
or [Warlpiri sentence B].
-
Or I could say [Warlpiri sentence C].
-
However, it doesn't matter
what order the words are in.
-
the exact same message will
come across, that I am saying,
-
"The boy kicked the ball."
-
[children playing]
-
♪ [piano and bells music]
-
(Dan) However different languages
may seem on the surface,
-
underneath they seem to be cut
out of the very same pattern.
-
There are striking similarities
of organization.
-
♪ [music continues]
-
In a way, it's like the human face.
-
The human face is very simple:
two eyes, a nose, and a mouth.
-
You can draw a simple sketch of it.
-
But look at the incredible diversity.
-
Each one of us has
a uniquely different face,
-
yet each face is obviously
a human face.
-
Languages are the same.
-
Each one is obviously
a different language,
-
but they're clearly examples
of the same kind of system.
-
(Jerry) One thing you might think to ask --
-
it's actually quite an interesting
topic to think about--
-
is "Just how good is language
at doing the job
-
which it's designed to do?"
-
(George Miller) Language serves
a great many human purposes,
-
but it can't do everything.
-
(Jerry) I mean, after all, if
it's just a biological adaptation,
-
which is what I'm suggesting,
-
then you'd expect,
like other biological adaptations, it
-
would have--
-
there would be limits on the fit
-
between the job that it does
and the job that it's designed to do.
-
(George Miller) Suppose I wanted
you to meet a friend of mine
-
at the train station and
you didn't know this friend
-
and I didn't have a photograph of him
-
and so I was trying to describe him.
-
And he's about 5 feet 8 inches tall
-
and he has brown hair
and a very ordinary face.
-
And I'm sure he'll be wearing clothes.
-
I don't know how to describe him to you
-
but perhaps you could meet him anyhow.
-
Would you please?
-
♪ [music playing]
-
(female) Something which
turns around and comes down.
-
(male) How can you explain
the concept of a spiral?
-
You almost have to make
use of a gesture like that.
-
(female) Can I use my hands?
-
(female) Sure.
-
(George Miller) I give advice.
If I say, "There's a leafy tree,"
-
well, how leafy is it?
-
(female) Something that twists
around in rotating circles.
-
(male) Circular.
-
Circular motion.
It's kinda like a slinky.
-
(George Miller) How leafy is a tree?
-
If you look at it,
you can tell immediately.
-
But trying to describe it,
-
I can't count the leaves and
tell you how many there are.
-
(Jerry) Can one imagine a system
-
that might communicate thought
better than natural languages?
-
Well, certainly, there's some purpose.
-
(George Carlin) For instance,
giving directions to someone.
-
Language is not as good as handing
them a map and saying,
-
"Look, I live here.
You go right down like that."
-
(male) Go down.
-
To North Harrison Street.
-
Just drawing it now.
-
Harrison Street, to Route 1.
-
(David) Someone could describe
a spiral without using gestures
-
but it would take a rather complex,
-
a series of sentences to do it.
-
(male) A loop which proceeds
in two dimensions.
-
It's got at rotational component
-
and the component going
transverse to that direction.
-
(male) Make a left.
-
Go down, you'll see
the state police barracks
-
right on the left hand side.
-
(George Carlin) Jonathan
Windows used to say,
-
"You go on down about a good
hundred miles and take a right."
-
(male) It's back about 250 yards.
-
Go through that guard booth.
-
(George Carlin) Or is to the left.
-
(Noam) The very nature of language
design leads to the consequence
-
that there are thoughts
which are perfectly fine thoughts
-
that are not expressible directly
through the mechanisms of language.
-
Language is a system with structure,
-
and any system with structure
and design has scope and limits.
-
If it had no intrinsic structure and design,
-
it could do nothing.
-
It would achieve nothing. It
-
would be useless for anything.
-
But the very structure that it has
-
not only provides it with its capacities
-
but also imposes certain limits on it.
-
So, for example, take a tool,
a physical tool, say a hammer.
-
If it was just an amorphous blob,
-
you couldn't do anything with it.
-
If it's a hammer with
a particular structure,
-
you can do certain things with it
-
but you can't do other things with it.
-
If a tool is going to be useful
for any purpose,
-
it's going to have to have structure
-
and that's going to prevent it from
being used for other purposes.
-
(Jerry) On the other hand,
-
there's a lot of expressive power
built into our linguistic capacity
-
that's easy to overlook.
-
I mean, people go around
telling you things like--
-
Not only do maps
-
show you how to get there easier
than you can describe it,
-
but moreover, a single picture
is worth thousands of words
-
and all that sort of stuff.
-
In a sense, it's true.
-
In a sense, it's also not.
-
We can do things of
great subtlety with language
-
that it's very hard to imagine
doing in any other way.
-
(George Miller) Much of the subtlety of
language comes, of course, from the syntax.
-
But there's also subtlety
in the words themselves.
-
Words can be associated
arbitrarily with anything.
-
We think of them being
associated with objects,
-
but they can be associated with ideas,
-
which abstractions,
with anything you like.
-
Words can be associated
with anything you can think of.
-
(Mark) Language is arbitrary.
-
There's no connection between
the thing and what we call the thing.
-
It's abstract.
-
You can talk about things
that really aren't there.
-
Things that have no form.
-
A word like "form," for example.
-
Now what is the meaning
of the word "form"?
-
Or a word like "truth."
-
That terrible word that
philosophers have discussed
-
for so many thousands of years.
-
What is truth?
-
We don't know.
-
We just have a word for it.
-
It's an arbitrary word.
-
(female) Love.
-
What does love mean?
-
What's love?
-
(Mark) So language is completely arbitrary.
-
Words are arbitrary.
-
(male) It can be anything you want.
-
Is that a light?
-
That's a lulu.
-
Turn the lulu on.
-
(Mark) Even little kids know that.
-
So my son, for example,
who is four years old.
-
He comes down for breakfast,
-
he sits down in his chair,
-
and he says, "I want gump for breakfast."
-
And you say, "Peter, what is gump?"
-
And he looks and he says,
"Gump is cereal."
-
(Noam) So, for example,
whether I call a tree
-
a tree or whether I call it a Baum as I would
if I grew up in Germany somewhere,
-
that's obviously variable.
-
(Mark) The thing and what you
call a thing have nothing at all
-
to do with one another.
-
(child) Dinosaur.
-
[man speaking other language]
-
(George Carlin) Pancakes.
-
(female) Infrastructure.
-
(George Miller) Different languages
have totally different vocabularies.
-
Different names for the same things.
-
Different names for different things,
depending on what they use.
-
(male) Words for camel.
-
(Michael) The Arabs have dozens
-
if not scores of words for camel and lion
-
and horse and other elements
of their civilization.
-
(Mary) The vocabulary of
each language reflects
-
the way of life of the people
who speak that language.
-
So people obviously need words
to talk about things
-
they want to talk about.
-
(George Miller) If you live at the North Pole,
-
bamboo is not important to you.
-
(male) [word in other language]
is a skinny camel.
-
[word in other language]
is a young female camel.
-
(Mark) There's got to be
a word for everything.
-
Every little piece in this boat,
you've got a different word for it.
-
You've got all these lines.
-
Each one's got a different name for it.
-
(sailor) This particular one is
the halyard for the spinnaker.
-
And over here, the halyard for
the burgee to fly a yacht club
-
or other identifying flag
from the top of the mast.
-
(male) [word in other language] is
a camel used for transporting loads.
-
[word in other language]
is used for camels in general.
-
(male) That horizontal appendage
-
projecting from the right side of
the dress plate is the lances rest.
-
The shoulders are covered
by the pauldrons.
-
The arms, including the upper and
lower arms and bend of the elbow
-
are covered by the vanbraces.
-
The left and right vanbrace.
-
(sailor) And so we have a topping lift
to hold up the end of the pole.
-
We have the halyard and a
downhaul on the butt of the pole
-
to raise and lower it
on the track on the mast.
-
(male) The shield is known
as the escutcheon.
-
And the steel covered shoe
is known as the sabotin.
-
(female) What do you call these camels?
-
(male) Well, I call them Beat,
Jess, Tom, Joe, Jerry, and Billy.
-
[people speaking other language]
-
(Henry) The Czech word
for ostrich is pstros.
-
(female) Bone.
-
(female) Grody.
-
[woman speaking other language]
-
(female) Elliptical.
-
(male) Lunch.
-
Pumpkin.
-
(male) We just have a word for it
so we can talk about it.
-
♪ [music playing]
-
(Jerry) Roughly speaking,
-
the achievement of language
-
is to provide a form of words
that allows us to say
-
anything we can think of.
-
(Thomas) You can make all kinds
of statements about conditions
-
as you wish them to be,
-
conditions as they might be,
-
conditions as they were once upon a time,
-
or conditions as they
will be in the future.
-
An animal cannot make
this kind of statement
-
because this kind of
statement is possible
-
only by means of language.
-
(Jerry) Consider, for example,
-
negation, right.
-
It's easy to tell somebody
that it's not going to rain.
-
Try drawing them a picture
of "It's not going to rain."
-
Or try drawing them a picture of
-
"If I had let the hammer go,
it would have fallen on my toe."
-
(George Carlin) In language, you can say,
-
"If we drop this piano,
-
it's not going to play a polonaise."
-
(Jerry) Think about a giraffe
standing beside me.
-
(Thomas) Here we are communicating
about a world of our imagination.
-
Imagination, if you like,
is also a human construct.
-
(Frederick) The edge that
language gave us
-
that no other animal has,
-
no other being has,
-
is the ability to think abstractly.
-
That's really what language
can do for us.
-
We seem to have this drive
to think abstractly.
-
We're propelled to do it.
-
It's not even a choice.
-
Because language itself is abstract,
-
we seem to be led to think abstractly.
-
Because we can think abstractly,
-
we don't have to talk about
-
what's in our immediate
physical environment.
-
We can plan for the future.
-
We can create art.
-
We can create complex
social organizations.
-
All of these things are possible
because we can think abstractly.
-
[bells music in background]
-
(female) Since my house burned down,
-
I now have a better view
of the rising moon.
-
♪ [woman singing in Xhosa]
-
(Dan) Language, like any
complex human phenomenon,
-
can be studied from many
different points of view.
-
And contemporary linguists specialize.
-
Some like to study words
and their structures
-
and see how the structures
of words affect sentences.
-
Some say the place to go is to work
on the structure of sentences
-
and worry later about
what the words mean.
-
Some people say unless you
look at the role of language
-
in conversations, in stories
-
and longer stretches of discourse,
-
you'll never really understand
what language is about.
-
Now, in a sense, of course,
they're all right.
-
(Frederick) We can't cut open
the brain and look at structures
-
that are involved in visual
perception or in memory,
-
but we do have language.
-
We do have something almost physical.
-
We have the record of what people say
-
and we can analyze that as data.
-
So linguistics provides us with
the most accessible window
-
into the human mind.
-
(Jerry) We have this remarkable system
-
that allows us to choose a form of words
-
corresponding to the thought
that we want to express.
-
And somehow, it's a system
we manage to
-
learn within a very short period of time,
-
if we're a child in a language community,
-
and that we manage to access,
-
to exploit both from the point
of view of the speaker
-
and from the point of view of the hearer,
-
instantaneously, easily, unconsciously,
-
with no sense of effort.
-
(Lila) So if you wanted to ask,
"What's the answer to the question?
-
Is language simple or
is language complex?"
-
It's simple for those creatures who use it.
-
That is, the human brain
is naturally evolved
-
so that it uses a system of
this apparently complex sort
-
in a very simple way.
-
(male) If a child is in an environment
-
in which language interactions
are proceeding,
-
that language will grow
in the child's mind
-
but very much in the manner of
other aspects of physical growth.
-
(female) I take it as the current
task of linguistics
-
to ask the question about language:
-
how much of it is built in
and in what way?
-
And how much of it is
and must be learned
-
by exposure to the environment
of speaking people?
-
(male) A question for the next in
our series on the human language:
-
how do children acquire language
without seeming to learn it?
-
Part 2: Playing the Language Game.