Return to Video

How to change your behavior for the better

  • 0:01 - 0:02
    Hi.
  • 0:02 - 0:05
    You might have noticed
    that I have half a beard.
  • 0:05 - 0:08
    It's not because I lost a bet.
  • 0:08 - 0:11
    Many years ago, I was badly burned.
  • 0:11 - 0:13
    Most of my body is covered with scars,
  • 0:13 - 0:15
    including the right side of my face.
  • 0:15 - 0:18
    I just don't have hair.
    That's just how it happened.
  • 0:18 - 0:20
    It looks symmetrical, but almost.
  • 0:20 - 0:23
    Anyway, now that we discussed facial hair,
  • 0:23 - 0:26
    let's move to social science,
  • 0:26 - 0:32
    and in particularly I want us to think
    about where is the potential for humanity
  • 0:32 - 0:34
    and where we are now.
  • 0:34 - 0:36
    And if you think about it,
    there's a big gap
  • 0:36 - 0:39
    between where we think we could be
    and where we are,
  • 0:39 - 0:41
    and it's in all kinds of areas.
  • 0:41 - 0:43
    So let me ask you:
  • 0:43 - 0:47
    how many of you in the last month
    have eaten more than you think you should?
  • 0:47 - 0:48
    Just kind of general. OK.
  • 0:48 - 0:51
    How many of you in the last month
  • 0:51 - 0:53
    have exercised less than
    you think you should?
  • 0:54 - 0:57
    OK, and for how many of you have
    raising your hands twice
  • 0:57 - 0:59
    been the most exercise you got today?
  • 0:59 - 1:01
    (Laughter)
  • 1:01 - 1:06
    How many of you have ever
    texted while driving?
  • 1:06 - 1:10
    OK, we're getting honest.
    Let's test your honesty.
  • 1:10 - 1:12
    How many people here in the last month
  • 1:12 - 1:15
    have not always washed your hands
    when you left the bathroom?
  • 1:15 - 1:17
    A little less honest.
  • 1:17 - 1:21
    By the way, it's interesting how we're
    willing to admit texting and driving,
  • 1:21 - 1:23
    but not washing our hands,
    that's difficult.
  • 1:23 - 1:25
    (Laughter)
  • 1:26 - 1:27
    We can go on and on,
  • 1:27 - 1:30
    and the problem in the topic is
    that there's lots of things
  • 1:30 - 1:32
    when we know what we could do.
  • 1:32 - 1:35
    We could be very, very different
    by reacting in a very different way.
  • 1:35 - 1:38
    And when we think
    how do we bridge that gap,
  • 1:38 - 1:41
    the usual answer is just tell people.
  • 1:41 - 1:43
    For example, just tell people
    that texting and driving is dangerous.
  • 1:43 - 1:45
    Did you know it's dangerous?
    You should stop doing it.
  • 1:45 - 1:49
    And you tell people something
    is dangerous and they will stop.
  • 1:49 - 1:50
    Texting and driving is one example.
  • 1:50 - 1:54
    Another very sad example
    is that in the US, we spend
  • 1:54 - 1:56
    between seven and eight hundred
    million dollars a year
  • 1:56 - 2:00
    on what's called financial literacy.
  • 2:00 - 2:02
    And what do we get
    as a consequence of that?
  • 2:02 - 2:06
    There was recently a study that looked
    at all the research ever to be conducted
  • 2:06 - 2:09
    on financial literacy,
    what's called a meta-analysis,
  • 2:09 - 2:12
    and what they found is that
    when you tell people,
  • 2:12 - 2:14
    you teach them financial literacy,
  • 2:14 - 2:16
    they learn and they remember.
  • 2:16 - 2:18
    But do people execute? Not so much.
  • 2:18 - 2:21
    The improvement is about
    three or four percent
  • 2:21 - 2:23
    immediately after the course,
  • 2:23 - 2:25
    and then it goes down.
  • 2:25 - 2:27
    And at the end of the day,
  • 2:27 - 2:30
    the improvement is about 0.1 percent,
  • 2:30 - 2:34
    not zero but as humanely close
    to zero as possible.
  • 2:34 - 2:37
    (Laughter)
  • 2:37 - 2:38
    So that's the sad news.
  • 2:38 - 2:41
    The sad news is giving
    information to people
  • 2:41 - 2:43
    is just not a good recipe
    to change behavior.
  • 2:43 - 2:44
    What is?
  • 2:44 - 2:47
    Well, social science
    has made lots of strides,
  • 2:47 - 2:52
    and the basic insight is that
    if we want to change behavior,
  • 2:52 - 2:53
    we have to change the environment.
  • 2:53 - 2:56
    The right way is not to change people,
    it's to change the environment,
  • 2:56 - 3:00
    and I want to present a very simple-minded
    model of how to think about it,
  • 3:00 - 3:02
    and it's to think about behavior change
  • 3:02 - 3:06
    in the same way that we think about
    sending a rocket to space.
  • 3:06 - 3:08
    And when we think about
    sending a rocket to space,
  • 3:08 - 3:10
    we want to do two main things.
  • 3:10 - 3:12
    The first one is to reduce friction.
  • 3:12 - 3:15
    We want to take the rocket
    and have as little friction as possible
  • 3:15 - 3:19
    so it's the most aerodynamic possible.
  • 3:19 - 3:21
    And the second thing is we want
    to load as much fuel as possible,
  • 3:21 - 3:25
    to give it the most amount
    of motivation, energy to do its task.
  • 3:25 - 3:28
    And behavior change is the same thing.
  • 3:28 - 3:31
    So let's first talk about friction.
  • 3:31 - 3:34
    In this particular case study
    I'll tell you about,
  • 3:34 - 3:37
    there's a pharmacy, an online pharmacy.
  • 3:37 - 3:41
    Imagine you go to your doctor,
  • 3:41 - 3:42
    you have a long-term illness,
  • 3:42 - 3:44
    your doctor prescribes
    to you a medication,
  • 3:44 - 3:46
    you sign up for this online pharmacy,
  • 3:46 - 3:49
    and you get your medication
    in the mail every 90 days.
  • 3:50 - 3:52
    Every 90 days, medication,
    medication, medication.
  • 3:52 - 3:54
    And this online pharmacy
  • 3:54 - 3:56
    wants to switch people
  • 3:56 - 3:59
    from branded medication
    to generic medication.
  • 3:59 - 4:04
    So they send people letters and they say
    please, please, please switch to generics.
  • 4:04 - 4:07
    You will save money, we will save money,
    your employer will save money.
  • 4:07 - 4:09
    And what do people do?
  • 4:09 - 4:10
    Nothing.
  • 4:10 - 4:12
    So they try all kinds of things
    and nothing happens.
  • 4:12 - 4:16
    So for one year, they give people
    an amazing offer.
  • 4:16 - 4:19
    They send people a letter and they say,
    if you switch to generics now,
  • 4:19 - 4:22
    it will be free for a a whole year.
    Free for a whole year. Amazing!
  • 4:22 - 4:27
    What percentage of people
    do you think switched?
  • 4:27 - 4:29
    Less than 10 percent.
  • 4:29 - 4:33
    At this point, they show up to my office
    and they come to complain.
  • 4:34 - 4:35
    Why did they pick me?
  • 4:35 - 4:38
    I wrote a couple of papers
    on the allure of "free."
  • 4:38 - 4:41
    In those papers, we showed
    that if you reduce the price of something
  • 4:41 - 4:43
    for let's say 10 cents to one cent,
    nothing much happens.
  • 4:43 - 4:48
    You reduce it from one cent to zero,
    now people get excited.
  • 4:48 - 4:50
    And they said, look,
    we read these papers on free,
  • 4:50 - 4:53
    we gave free, not working as we expected.
  • 4:53 - 4:54
    What's going on?
  • 4:54 - 4:58
    I said, you know, maybe
    it's a question of friction.
  • 4:58 - 4:59
    They said, what do you mean?
  • 4:59 - 5:03
    I said, people are starting with branded.
  • 5:03 - 5:04
    They can do nothing an end with branded.
  • 5:04 - 5:07
    To move to generic, they have to choose
    generic over branded,
  • 5:07 - 5:09
    but they also have to do something.
  • 5:09 - 5:11
    They have to return a letter.
  • 5:11 - 5:14
    So this is what we call
    a confounded design.
  • 5:14 - 5:16
    Two things are happening at the same time.
  • 5:16 - 5:17
    It's branded versus generic,
  • 5:17 - 5:20
    but it's doing nothing
    versus doing something.
  • 5:20 - 5:23
    So I said, why don't we switch it?
  • 5:23 - 5:26
    Why don't we send people a letter
    and say, we're switching you to generics.
  • 5:26 - 5:28
    You don't need to do anything.
  • 5:28 - 5:31
    If you want to stay with branded,
    please return the letter.
  • 5:31 - 5:33
    (Laughter)
  • 5:33 - 5:35
    Right?
  • 5:35 - 5:37
    What do you think happened.
  • 5:37 - 5:39
    Lawyers, lawyers happened.
  • 5:39 - 5:42
    (Laughter)
  • 5:44 - 5:46
    It turns out this is illegal.
  • 5:46 - 5:50
    (Laughter)
  • 5:52 - 5:55
    By the way, for brainstorming
    and creativity,
  • 5:55 - 5:57
    doing things that are illegal
    and immoral, it's fine,
  • 5:57 - 6:02
    as long as it's just
    in a brainstorming phase.
  • 6:02 - 6:04
    (Laughter)
  • 6:04 - 6:05
    But this was the purity of the idea,
    because the initial design
  • 6:05 - 6:08
    was the brand that had
    the no action benefit,
  • 6:08 - 6:10
    in my illegal, immoral design
  • 6:10 - 6:12
    generic had the no -action benefit,
  • 6:12 - 6:15
    but they agreed to give people
    a t-intersection:
  • 6:15 - 6:17
    send people a letter and say,
  • 6:17 - 6:19
    "If you don't return this letter
    we will be forced
  • 6:19 - 6:21
    to stop your medications,
  • 6:21 - 6:25
    but when you return the letter,
    you could choose branded at this price,
  • 6:25 - 6:27
    generic at this price."
  • 6:27 - 6:29
    Now, people had to take an action.
  • 6:29 - 6:31
    They were on even foot. Right?
  • 6:31 - 6:33
    It wasn't that one had
    the no action benefit.
  • 6:33 - 6:37
    What percentage do you think switched?
  • 6:37 - 6:38
    The vast majority switched.
  • 6:38 - 6:40
    So what does it tell us?
  • 6:40 - 6:42
    Do people like generics
    or do we like branded?
  • 6:42 - 6:44
    We hate returning letters.
  • 6:44 - 6:46
    (Laughter)
  • 6:46 - 6:51
    This is the story of friction:
    small things really matter.
  • 6:51 - 6:55
    And friction is about taking
    the desired behavior
  • 6:55 - 6:57
    and saying, where do we have
    too much friction
  • 6:57 - 7:00
    so it's slowing people down
    from acting on it.
  • 7:00 - 7:03
    And every time you see
    that the desired behavior
  • 7:03 - 7:04
    and the easy behavior are not aligned,
  • 7:04 - 7:07
    it means we want to try
    and realign them.
  • 7:07 - 7:08
    That's the first part.
    We talked about friction.
  • 7:08 - 7:11
    Now let's talk about motivation.
  • 7:11 - 7:14
    In this particular study,
    we were trying to get very poor people
  • 7:14 - 7:17
    in a slum called Kibera in Kenya
  • 7:17 - 7:19
    to save a little bit of money
    for a rainy day.
  • 7:20 - 7:22
    You know, if you're very, very poor,
    you have no extra money,
  • 7:22 - 7:24
    you live hand to mouth,
    and from time to time bad things happen,
  • 7:24 - 7:29
    and when something bad happens,
    you have nothing to draw on, you borrow.
  • 7:29 - 7:31
    The Kibera people can draw at sometimes,
  • 7:31 - 7:32
    borrow sometimes at
    up to 10 percent interest a week,
  • 7:32 - 7:39
    and then of course
    really hard to get out of it.
  • 7:39 - 7:42
    You live hand to mouth,
    something bad happens,
  • 7:42 - 7:44
    you borrow, things get worse
    and worse and worse.
  • 7:44 - 7:47
    So we wanted people to keep
    a little bit of money for a rainy day.
  • 7:47 - 7:52
    And we thought about
    what is the motivation,
  • 7:52 - 7:54
    what is the fuel that we need to add?
  • 7:54 - 7:55
    And we tried all kinds of things.
  • 7:55 - 7:58
    Some people we texted them once a week
  • 7:58 - 8:02
    and said, please try to save
    100 shillings, about a dollar, this week.
  • 8:03 - 8:07
    Some people we sent a text message
    as if it came from their kids,
  • 8:07 - 8:11
    so it said, hi Mom, hi Dad,
    this is little Joey --
  • 8:11 - 8:13
    whatever the name of the kid was --
  • 8:13 - 8:17
    try and save a hundred shillings this week
    for the future of our family.
  • 8:17 - 8:19
    Right, I'm Jewish, a little bit
    of guilt always works.
  • 8:19 - 8:21
    (Laughter)
  • 8:21 - 8:24
    Some people got 10 percent.
  • 8:24 - 8:26
    "Save up to a hundred shillings,
    we'll give you 10 percent."
  • 8:26 - 8:28
    Some people got 20 percent.
  • 8:28 - 8:31
    Some people got also
    10 percent and 20 percent,
  • 8:31 - 8:33
    but they got it with loss aversion.
  • 8:33 - 8:35
    What is loss aversion?
  • 8:35 - 8:37
    Loss aversion is the idea
    that we hate losing
  • 8:37 - 8:39
    more than we enjoy gaining?
  • 8:39 - 8:42
    Now think about somebody
    who is in a 10 percent condition
  • 8:42 - 8:44
    and they put 40 shillings in.
  • 8:44 - 8:46
    They put 40 shillings,
    we give them four more,
  • 8:46 - 8:47
    they say thank you very much.
  • 8:47 - 8:49
    That person gave up six.
  • 8:49 - 8:51
    They could have gotten six more
    if they gave a hundred,
  • 8:51 - 8:53
    but they don't see it.
  • 8:53 - 8:56
    So we created what we call pre-match.
  • 8:56 - 8:58
    We put the 10 shillings
    in the beginning of the week.
  • 8:58 - 8:59
    We said it's waiting for you.
  • 8:59 - 9:03
    And then if somebody puts 40 in,
    we say, oh you put 40 in,
  • 9:03 - 9:05
    we're leaving four
    and we're taking six back.
  • 9:05 - 9:08
    So in both cases, pre-match or post-match,
  • 9:08 - 9:11
    people get 10 percent,
  • 9:11 - 9:12
    but in the pre-match,
  • 9:12 - 9:16
    they see the money they did not match
    leaving their account.
  • 9:16 - 9:19
    So we have text, text from kids,
    10 percent, 20 percent,
  • 9:19 - 9:21
    pre-match, post-match.
  • 9:21 - 9:23
    And we had one more condition.
  • 9:23 - 9:25
    It was a coin about this size
  • 9:25 - 9:28
    with 24 numbers written on it,
  • 9:28 - 9:30
    and we asked them to put the point
    somewhere in their hat,
  • 9:30 - 9:35
    and every week we'd take a knife
    and scratch the number for that week,
  • 9:35 - 9:36
    week one, two, three, four,
  • 9:36 - 9:39
    scratch it like a minus
    if they didn't save
  • 9:39 - 9:42
    and scratch it up and down if they saved.
  • 9:42 - 9:43
    Now think to yourself,
  • 9:43 - 9:47
    which one of those methods
    do you think worked the best?
  • 9:47 - 9:49
    Text, text from the kids,
    10 percent, 20 percent,
  • 9:49 - 9:51
    beginning of the week,
    end of the week, and the coin?
  • 9:51 - 9:54
    I'll tell you what
    the average people think.
  • 9:54 - 9:57
    We've done these studies of prediction,
  • 9:57 - 9:58
    both in the US and in Kenya.
  • 9:58 - 10:00
    People think that 20 percent
    will get a lot of action,
  • 10:00 - 10:02
    10 percent less,
  • 10:02 - 10:04
    the rest of it will do nothing.
  • 10:04 - 10:08
    Kids, coin, doesn't matter.
  • 10:08 - 10:11
    People think loss aversion
    will have a small effect.
  • 10:11 - 10:13
    What actually happened:
  • 10:13 - 10:15
    sending a text reminder once a week
  • 10:15 - 10:17
    helps a lot.
  • 10:17 - 10:19
    Good news! This program lasted six months.
  • 10:19 - 10:22
    People forget. Reminding people is great.
  • 10:22 - 10:25
    Ten percent at the end
    of the week helped some more.
  • 10:25 - 10:27
    Financial incentives worked.
  • 10:27 - 10:31
    Twenty percent at the end of the week,
    just like 10 percent, no different.
  • 10:31 - 10:35
    Ten percent in the beginning of the week,
  • 10:35 - 10:35
    helps some more.
  • 10:35 - 10:36
    Loss aversion works.
  • 10:36 - 10:38
    Twenty percent in
    the beginning of the week,
  • 10:38 - 10:41
    just like 10 percent in the beginning
    of the week, no difference.
  • 10:41 - 10:45
    And the text message from the kids
    was just as effective
  • 10:45 - 10:48
    as 20 percent plus loss aversion,
  • 10:48 - 10:49
    which is amazing, right?
  • 10:49 - 10:53
    It's amazing how motivating
    messages from kids work.
  • 10:53 - 10:56
    And one conclusion is
    we don't use kids enough.
  • 10:56 - 10:58
    (Laughter)
  • 10:58 - 11:00
    And of course I don't mean
    in a child labor sense.
  • 11:00 - 11:07
    But if you think about
    parents and their kids,
  • 11:07 - 11:09
    we are the best that we can for our kids,
  • 11:09 - 11:12
    and we think about the future,
    and I think we should think
  • 11:12 - 11:15
    about how to use that amazing
    source of motivation
  • 11:15 - 11:19
    to get parents to behave in a better way.
  • 11:19 - 11:22
    But the big surprise
    of this study was the coin.
  • 11:22 - 11:26
    The coin basically doubled savings
    compared to everything else.
  • 11:26 - 11:29
    And now the question is why.
    What was it about the coin?
  • 11:29 - 11:33
    So I'll tell you how I started
    thinking about the coin,
  • 11:33 - 11:37
    and then we'll come back to it.
  • 11:37 - 11:38
    So you know, when I do research
    on, let's say, buying coffee,
  • 11:38 - 11:38
    I don't need to go anywhere.
  • 11:38 - 11:40
    I can sit in my office.
    I bought enough coffee.
  • 11:40 - 11:41
    I know how it works.
  • 11:41 - 11:43
    The details I'm familiar with.
  • 11:43 - 11:46
    When you do research in some of
    the poorest places in the world,
  • 11:46 - 11:49
    you have to go and visit and see
    what's going on and get some insight
  • 11:49 - 11:51
    about how the system works.
  • 11:52 - 11:54
    And on that particular day,
  • 11:54 - 11:56
    I'm in a place called Soweto
    in South Africa,
  • 11:56 - 12:00
    and I'm sitting in a place
    that sells funeral insurance.
  • 12:00 - 12:04
    You know, in the US people spend
    crazy amounts of money on weddings?
  • 12:04 - 12:06
    In South Africa, it's funerals.
  • 12:07 - 12:12
    People spend up to a year
    or two years of income on funerals.
  • 12:12 - 12:14
    And I sit in this place --
  • 12:14 - 12:17
    oh, by the way, before you judge
    the South Africans
  • 12:17 - 12:20
    as being irrational with this,
  • 12:20 - 12:22
    I just want to remind you that spending
  • 12:22 - 12:25
    a lot of money on funerals
    compared to weddings,
  • 12:25 - 12:28
    at least you know for sure
    you only have one.
  • 12:28 - 12:31
    (Laughter)
  • 12:31 - 12:41
    OK, so I sit in this place
    that sells funeral insurance,
  • 12:41 - 12:45
    and this guy comes in with his son,
    his son is about 12,
  • 12:45 - 12:48
    and he buys funeral insurance for a week.
  • 12:49 - 12:52
    It will cover 90 percent
    of his funeral expense
  • 12:52 - 12:54
    only if he dies in the next seven days.
  • 12:55 - 12:58
    Right? These are very poor people,
    they buy small amounts of insurance
  • 12:58 - 12:59
    and small amount of ??
  • 12:59 - 13:02
    And he gets that certificate,
  • 13:02 - 13:04
    and in a very ceremonious way
    he gives it to his son,
  • 13:05 - 13:08
    and as he gives it to his son,
    I think to myself, why the ceremony?
  • 13:08 - 13:09
    What is this father doing?
  • 13:09 - 13:12
    Now think about the breadwinner
  • 13:12 - 13:15
    that decides on that particular day
    to direct some money
  • 13:15 - 13:17
    into insurance or savings.
  • 13:17 - 13:20
    What is the family going to see tonight?
  • 13:20 - 13:22
    They're going to see less.
  • 13:22 - 13:25
    Right? At that level of poverty, there'll
    be less food, less kerosene, less water,
  • 13:25 - 13:28
    some less tonight.
  • 13:28 - 13:31
    And what his father was doing
    and what our coin was trying to do
  • 13:31 - 13:34
    is to say, yes, there's less
    food on the table,
  • 13:34 - 13:36
    but there's another activity.
  • 13:36 - 13:40
    You see what happened is there are
    many good, important economic activities,
  • 13:40 - 13:43
    like savings and insurance,
    that are invisible.
  • 13:43 - 13:47
    And now the question is,
    how do we make them visible?
  • 13:47 - 13:51
    So let's go back to our rocket model.
  • 13:51 - 13:53
    We have to first of all look at the system
  • 13:53 - 13:55
    and see where there's
    little things we can fix.
  • 13:55 - 13:59
    With friction, where is there
    can we remove friction?
  • 13:59 - 14:03
    And then the next thing we want to do
  • 14:03 - 14:04
    is we want to think
    broadly about the system,
  • 14:04 - 14:06
    and say what other motivations
    can we bring in?
  • 14:06 - 14:08
    And that's a much more difficult exercise,
  • 14:08 - 14:11
    and we don't always know
    what would work best.
  • 14:11 - 14:13
    Is it going to be money?
    Is it going to be loss aversion?
  • 14:13 - 14:15
    Is it going to be
    something that is visible?
  • 14:15 - 14:18
    We don't know, and we have
    to try different things.
  • 14:18 - 14:22
    We also have to realize that
    our intuition sometimes misleads us.
  • 14:22 - 14:25
    We don't always necessarily know
    what would work the best.
  • 14:25 - 14:27
    So if we think about this gap
  • 14:27 - 14:29
    between where we could be
    and where we are,
  • 14:29 - 14:32
    it's a really sad thing to see this gap
    and to think about it,
  • 14:32 - 14:36
    but the good news is
    there's lots we can do.
  • 14:36 - 14:39
    Some of the changes are easy,
    some of the changes are more complex,
  • 14:39 - 14:42
    but if we'll attack each problem directly,
  • 14:42 - 14:46
    not by just providing
    more information to people
  • 14:46 - 14:48
    but trying to change the friction,
  • 14:48 - 14:49
    add motivation,
  • 14:49 - 14:50
    I think we can,
  • 14:50 - 14:52
    can we close the gap? No.
  • 14:52 - 14:55
    But can we get much better?
    Absolutely yes.
  • 14:55 - 14:56
    Thank you very much.
  • 14:56 - 14:59
    (Applause)
Title:
How to change your behavior for the better
Speaker:
Dan Ariely
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
15:13

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions