< Return to Video

The scientific method is crap | Teman Cooke | TEDxLancaster

  • 0:22 - 0:25
    The scientific method sucks.
  • 0:25 - 0:26
    (Laughter)
  • 0:26 - 0:29
    Now, by that I don't mean
    that science sucks.
  • 0:29 - 0:34
    As a physicist, I believe that science
    has had countless benefits for humanity.
  • 0:34 - 0:36
    But when I was in eigth grade,
  • 0:36 - 0:38
    I learned about this thing
    called the scientific method.
  • 0:38 - 0:42
    And since then, I have done science,
    I have worked with other scientists
  • 0:42 - 0:44
    and I've taught science
  • 0:44 - 0:45
    to college students,
  • 0:45 - 0:47
    to K-12 teachers,
  • 0:47 - 0:51
    and to seventh and eighth graders -
    middle schoolers.
  • 0:51 - 0:54
    And from these experiences,
    I've come to the conclusion
  • 0:54 - 0:57
    that the scientific method ... sucks.
  • 0:57 - 0:59
    If it's been a while for you,
  • 0:59 - 1:01
    as it has been for me,
    since middle school,
  • 1:01 - 1:04
    let me run through it real quick for you.
  • 1:05 - 1:08
    First of all, Step 1: Identify a problem.
  • 1:08 - 1:11
    Step 2: Do some research.
  • 1:11 - 1:13
    Step 3: Form a hypothesis.
  • 1:13 - 1:16
    Step 4: Do an experiment with some
    independent and dependent variables
  • 1:16 - 1:18
    to test your hypothesis.
  • 1:18 - 1:20
    Step 5: Analyze data.
  • 1:20 - 1:22
    Step 6: Draw a conclusion.
  • 1:22 - 1:24
    It sounds good, right?
  • 1:24 - 1:28
    It certainly matches
    what we see on TV and in movies,
  • 1:28 - 1:30
    and what we see on the news.
  • 1:30 - 1:33
    Unfortunately, it completely misrepresents
  • 1:33 - 1:37
    what scientists do
    and what science is all about.
  • 1:37 - 1:41
    And this manifests in four main ways:
  • 1:41 - 1:44
    First of all, it's incredibly linear.
  • 1:44 - 1:47
    We start with a problem statement,
    and we end with a conclusion.
  • 1:47 - 1:51
    This implies that science
    is something that's once and done,
  • 1:51 - 1:54
    fixed for all time.
  • 1:54 - 1:57
    Secondly, it's not very intuitive.
  • 1:57 - 1:59
    How do I choose a problem?
  • 1:59 - 2:02
    What do I do with
    my conclusion when I'm done?
  • 2:03 - 2:08
    If I have no idea what should
    or even could happen,
  • 2:08 - 2:10
    how do I come up with a hypothesis?
  • 2:11 - 2:14
    This makes the process intimidating
  • 2:14 - 2:15
    and dangerous.
  • 2:17 - 2:19
    Three, there's a focus on conclusion -
  • 2:19 - 2:22
    reaching the conclusion,
    on getting a fact.
  • 2:22 - 2:25
    Well, what happens
    when the information changes?
  • 2:25 - 2:27
    What happens when facts change?
  • 2:27 - 2:30
    How does this process capture that?
  • 2:31 - 2:33
    And finally, the focus on experimentation.
  • 2:33 - 2:37
    Now, don't get me wrong,
    I'm a big fan of teaching students
  • 2:37 - 2:41
    to control for whatever
    random variables they can,
  • 2:41 - 2:44
    but there's been a lot
    of really good science that's been done
  • 2:44 - 2:48
    without being able to control
    for, well, anything.
  • 2:48 - 2:51
    For a moment, I'd like for you to imagine
  • 2:51 - 2:55
    what budget it would take
    to create a laboratory here on Earth
  • 2:55 - 2:57
    to study the interior of Jupiter.
  • 2:59 - 3:02
    Or to form a star from scratch.
  • 3:03 - 3:06
    Or to study a galaxy's development.
  • 3:08 - 3:11
    Do you want your tax dollars
    to go towards that?
  • 3:12 - 3:16
    Now, you may be thinking,
    well, it's probably not that bad.
  • 3:16 - 3:20
    I mean, we've trained generations
    of scientists using the scientific method,
  • 3:20 - 3:21
    myself included,
  • 3:21 - 3:24
    surely, I'm exaggerating.
  • 3:25 - 3:28
    Well, let me walk through
    a few statistics with you.
  • 3:29 - 3:32
    65 percent of Americans
    have little to no idea
  • 3:32 - 3:35
    what scientists do on a day-to-day basis.
  • 3:35 - 3:36
    (Laughter)
  • 3:36 - 3:39
    50 percent of them, however,
    have considered it,
  • 3:39 - 3:41
    whatever it is, dangerous.
  • 3:41 - 3:43
    (Laughter)
  • 3:43 - 3:46
    32 percent of middle schoolers,
    of eighth graders,
  • 3:46 - 3:51
    score at or above proficiency
    in science, in 2011.
  • 3:51 - 3:55
    The good news is,
    that's up from 30 percent in 2009.
  • 3:56 - 4:01
    The bad new is, that implies 68 percent,
    two thirds, middle schoolers,
  • 4:01 - 4:04
    score below proficiency.
  • 4:05 - 4:10
    30 percent of elementary school teachers
    feel well prepared to teach science.
  • 4:10 - 4:12
    Now that's compared to 52 percent
  • 4:12 - 4:14
    who feel well prepared
    to teach social studies,
  • 4:14 - 4:18
    66 percent who feel
    well prepared to teach mathematics,
  • 4:18 - 4:23
    and 77 percent who feel well prepared
    to teach reading and language arts.
  • 4:24 - 4:28
    But here's my favorite statistic of all,
    the one I find most telling.
  • 4:29 - 4:32
    Of 2,000 parents surveyed in the UK,
  • 4:32 - 4:35
    50 percent said that they feared
  • 4:35 - 4:38
    answering their children's
    questions about science -
  • 4:38 - 4:41
    questions like, why is the sky blue?
  • 4:41 - 4:44
    And why is the moon out
    during the daytime?
  • 4:44 - 4:50
    20 percent, one in five,
    said that in response to such questions,
  • 4:50 - 4:55
    they said, "No one knows"
    or made somethig up.
  • 4:55 - 4:57
    (Laughter)
  • 4:57 - 4:59
    This concerns me.
  • 5:00 - 5:07
    As a culture, as a civilization,
    we fear science.
  • 5:07 - 5:08
    Why?
  • 5:09 - 5:12
    Well, I believe there are
    three main sources of that.
  • 5:12 - 5:15
    First of all, we're obsessed
    with right answers,
  • 5:15 - 5:17
    we're obsessed with conclusions.
  • 5:17 - 5:21
    And as we go further and further,
    we learn more and more stuff,
  • 5:21 - 5:23
    we can't keep track of it all.
  • 5:24 - 5:29
    In addition, the facts,
    those conclusions, change.
  • 5:29 - 5:31
    Let me give you an example.
  • 5:31 - 5:33
    If you're like me,
    when you were growing up,
  • 5:33 - 5:36
    there were nine planets
    in the solar system.
  • 5:36 - 5:38
    In 2006,
  • 5:38 - 5:41
    one of these things was eliminated.
  • 5:41 - 5:43
    Which brings me to my third point:
  • 5:43 - 5:45
    Why?!
  • 5:45 - 5:46
    (Laughter)
  • 5:46 - 5:49
    We don't understand
    how do these decisions get made.
  • 5:49 - 5:52
    We don't understand
    the thought process behind it.
  • 5:53 - 5:58
    Now, all of these problems possibly don't
    lie at the feet of the scientific method,
  • 5:58 - 6:00
    but it's not helping.
  • 6:02 - 6:04
    There is, however, a better way.
  • 6:04 - 6:08
    Let me introduce you
    to the cycle of scientific thinking.
  • 6:08 - 6:12
    This starts with interesting observations.
  • 6:12 - 6:14
    What counts as interesting?
  • 6:15 - 6:18
    Well, if you find yourself
    asking the question:
  • 6:19 - 6:20
    What happened there?
  • 6:21 - 6:23
    Why did that occur?
  • 6:23 - 6:25
    What's going on?
  • 6:25 - 6:27
    It's probably interesting.
  • 6:28 - 6:33
    In the face of such questions,
    the human brain does an amazing thing:
  • 6:33 - 6:38
    It tries to come up with an answer,
    a story, an explanation
  • 6:38 - 6:39
    of what's going on.
  • 6:40 - 6:44
    Now, a lot of people
    are perfectly happy to have an answer.
  • 6:44 - 6:48
    But scientists - and this is what makes
    scientists different from other people -
  • 6:48 - 6:51
    scientists want to know
    if their answer is right.
  • 6:51 - 6:54
    And the way they do that is by saying,
  • 6:54 - 6:58
    "Well, if my explanation
    is true, is correct,
  • 6:58 - 7:02
    then I should also see this ..."
    They make a prediction.
  • 7:02 - 7:05
    And once you have a prediction,
    the only thing left to do
  • 7:05 - 7:08
    is to go make an observation,
    is to see if you were right.
  • 7:08 - 7:11
    And if you are, yay!
  • 7:11 - 7:16
    But if you're not, if you're truly lucky,
    and you got it wrong,
  • 7:16 - 7:19
    then that's going
    to bring up more questions -
  • 7:19 - 7:23
    questions that require more explanation.
  • 7:23 - 7:25
    Which leads to new predictions.
  • 7:25 - 7:29
    And so on, and so on, and so on.
  • 7:31 - 7:33
    Now, why is this better?
  • 7:33 - 7:38
    Well, first of all, it actually
    represents what scientists do.
  • 7:38 - 7:40
    In fact, post-graduate
    education in science
  • 7:40 - 7:44
    is all about teaching people
    how to take interesting observations,
  • 7:44 - 7:46
    ask pertinent questions,
  • 7:46 - 7:51
    and then develop explanations
    that lead to observable predictions.
  • 7:51 - 7:53
    This is science.
  • 7:53 - 7:56
    But secondly, and more importantly,
    it's much more intuitive
  • 7:56 - 7:58
    and much more engaging.
  • 7:58 - 8:00
    The power is in your hands.
  • 8:00 - 8:04
    Once you have an interesting question,
    do you want to go to the library
  • 8:04 - 8:06
    and read up on other people's answers?
  • 8:06 - 8:10
    Or do you want to skip all that
    and come up with an answer of your own,
  • 8:10 - 8:12
    and see if you can make
    a prediction and test it?
  • 8:12 - 8:14
    It's up to you.
  • 8:14 - 8:17
    That makes it much less intimidating.
  • 8:17 - 8:20
    From this, as well,
    it becomes much more obvious
  • 8:20 - 8:24
    how examples, how explanations,
    change over time.
  • 8:24 - 8:26
    In fact, there are only
    three possible things
  • 8:26 - 8:29
    that can happen to a scientific model:
  • 8:29 - 8:31
    It makes the right predictions,
  • 8:31 - 8:35
    in which case it becomes
    stronger over time;
  • 8:35 - 8:37
    it makes a few wrong predictions,
  • 8:37 - 8:41
    in which case it is modified over time;
  • 8:41 - 8:44
    or it makes completely wrong predictions,
  • 8:44 - 8:47
    in which case it will be
    abandoned over time.
  • 8:48 - 8:51
    The last great thing about this model
  • 8:51 - 8:54
    is that it also illuminates something
    that's very dear to my heart:
  • 8:54 - 9:01
    the idea that any explanation
    must be able to be proven false.
  • 9:01 - 9:03
    And to explain this, I want to start with:
  • 9:03 - 9:06
    What would it take for me to show
  • 9:06 - 9:11
    that my explanation of a phenomenon
    is True, with a capital 'T'?
  • 9:11 - 9:14
    Well if it's true,
    then every prediction it makes
  • 9:14 - 9:16
    should match the observations.
  • 9:16 - 9:18
    Well how do I check that?
  • 9:18 - 9:21
    I have to check "every" observation.
  • 9:21 - 9:26
    That's not "every observation
    that I can make with my current budget."
  • 9:26 - 9:29
    That's not "every observation
    that I can make here
  • 9:29 - 9:31
    in this amount of time that I have."
  • 9:31 - 9:34
    It's "every" observation.
  • 9:34 - 9:37
    Everywhere. Everywhen.
  • 9:38 - 9:40
    It's not possible.
  • 9:40 - 9:44
    In order to prove
    an explanation false, however,
  • 9:44 - 9:48
    all I have to do is find out
    that it makes the wrong predictions,
  • 9:48 - 9:51
    and then make sure
    that I didn't make a mistake.
  • 9:51 - 9:54
    The way I think of this is to say:
  • 9:54 - 9:58
    If you give me a model that consistently
    predicts the wrong thing,
  • 9:58 - 10:01
    I can say with certainty,
    your model is wrong.
  • 10:01 - 10:05
    If you give me a model that consistently
    predicts the right thing,
  • 10:05 - 10:09
    I can say with certainty
    that your model is not wrong ...
  • 10:09 - 10:11
    yet.
  • 10:11 - 10:13
    Let me give you an example.
  • 10:13 - 10:17
    In 1781, we found the planet Uranus.
  • 10:18 - 10:20
    But it did this really weird thing.
  • 10:20 - 10:25
    At certain points in its orbit,
    it was further along than we expected
  • 10:25 - 10:28
    based upon our models,
    our understanding of gravity.
  • 10:28 - 10:32
    And at other points in its orbits,
    it hadn't traveled far enough.
  • 10:32 - 10:37
    It was almost as if it was traveling
    too fast at some points
  • 10:37 - 10:40
    and not fast enough at other points.
  • 10:40 - 10:42
    Astronomers looked at this, and they said,
  • 10:42 - 10:46
    "You know, it looks a lot
    like something is pulling it -
  • 10:46 - 10:49
    pulling it a little bit faster
    or a little bit slower
  • 10:49 - 10:52
    depending on where it is in its orbit.
  • 10:53 - 10:56
    Maybe there's a planet out there.
  • 10:56 - 10:59
    Maybe there's something
    interacting with it gravitationally."
  • 10:59 - 11:04
    And so they did the calculations,
    they found out where the planet should be,
  • 11:04 - 11:07
    and they pointed their telescopes
    in the sky at that location.
  • 11:08 - 11:11
    The planet they found
    we call Neptune, today.
  • 11:12 - 11:15
    Now, this was awesome
    to be able to do this,
  • 11:15 - 11:18
    and there were still discrepancies
    in Uranus's orbit.
  • 11:18 - 11:19
    So they did calculations,
  • 11:19 - 11:22
    they said maybe there's
    another planet out there, planet X.
  • 11:22 - 11:24
    They did the calculations,
  • 11:24 - 11:26
    they figured out
    where that planet should be,
  • 11:26 - 11:29
    and they pointed their telescopes
    into the sky at that point,
  • 11:29 - 11:32
    and they looked and they found ...
  • 11:32 - 11:34
    nothing.
  • 11:35 - 11:36
    And this was a problem.
  • 11:36 - 11:40
    And they said, "Okay,"
    but they continued to look.
  • 11:40 - 11:43
    And eventually they saw something.
  • 11:43 - 11:44
    And it wasn't where they expected,
  • 11:44 - 11:47
    and it wasn't really
    the size they expected,
  • 11:47 - 11:49
    but they said, "We found something,
    and it's a planet,
  • 11:49 - 11:52
    and we're going to call that thing Pluto."
  • 11:53 - 11:55
    Fast forward 60 years.
  • 11:56 - 12:01
    Astronomers continued looking,
    continued taking observations.
  • 12:02 - 12:05
    And another one popped up.
  • 12:05 - 12:10
    Same size, same composition,
    same location.
  • 12:10 - 12:12
    And then another one,
  • 12:12 - 12:14
    and another one,
  • 12:14 - 12:15
    and another one,
  • 12:16 - 12:17
    and another one.
  • 12:18 - 12:20
    And astronomers said, "Uh oh.
  • 12:21 - 12:24
    This is not looking so much
    like the other planets,
  • 12:24 - 12:26
    this is looking like the asteroid belt -
  • 12:26 - 12:30
    a collection of objects
    that all share the same orbit
  • 12:30 - 12:34
    but aren't really planets,
    not what we think of as planets."
  • 12:35 - 12:40
    And as the evidence,
    as the observations continued to build,
  • 12:40 - 12:43
    astronomers ended up having to abandon
  • 12:43 - 12:46
    this explanation of Pluto
    as being a planet -
  • 12:46 - 12:48
    it didn't fit.
  • 12:49 - 12:54
    In the same way, we need to abandon
    the scientific model.
  • 12:54 - 12:56
    It doesn't fit.
  • 12:56 - 13:00
    Instead, embrace
    the cycle of scientific thinking.
  • 13:01 - 13:05
    You don't have to be
    an expert to do science.
  • 13:05 - 13:07
    You don't have to know everything
  • 13:07 - 13:10
    to answer your kid's
    questions about science.
  • 13:10 - 13:13
    Even if the answer is: I don't know.
  • 13:13 - 13:15
    What do you think?
  • 13:15 - 13:17
    Observe.
  • 13:17 - 13:18
    Explain.
  • 13:18 - 13:20
    Predict.
  • 13:20 - 13:23
    That's enough; it's good enough.
  • 13:23 - 13:28
    Together, as parents,
    as educators, and as scientists,
  • 13:28 - 13:32
    we can prepare our kids, our students,
    our schools, and our country
  • 13:32 - 13:35
    for the challenges
    of the 21st century and beyond,
  • 13:35 - 13:40
    if we can learn to think differently
    about science over time.
  • 13:40 - 13:41
    Thank you very much.
  • 13:41 - 13:43
    (Applause)
Title:
The scientific method is crap | Teman Cooke | TEDxLancaster
Description:

Teman Cooke hold a Ph.D. in theoretical physics but has no love for the scientific method. He explains an interesting alternative that will challenge your thinking.

This talk was given at a TEDx event using the TED conference format but independently organized by a local community. Learn more at http://ted.com/tedx

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDxTalks
Duration:
13:52

English subtitles

Revisions