< Return to Video

CRITICAL THINKING - Cognitive Biases: Anchoring [HD]

  • 0:00 - 0:06
    (intro music)
  • 0:06 - 0:07
    My name is Laurie Santos.
  • 0:07 - 0:10
    I teach psychology at Yale
    University, and today
  • 0:10 - 0:13
    I want to talk to you about anchoring.
  • 0:13 - 0:16
    This lecture is part of a
    series on cognitive biases.
  • 0:16 - 0:19
    Let's do a math problem.
    really quickly, and you've
  • 0:19 - 0:21
    gotta do it in your head
  • 0:21 - 0:22
    Ready?
  • 0:22 - 0:28
    First, multiply the following numbers:
    eight times seven times six
  • 0:28 - 0:32
    times five times four times three times
    two times one.
  • 0:32 - 0:35
    OK, that's it.
  • 0:35 - 0:37
    What's your guess?
  • 0:37 - 0:38
    A thousand?
  • 0:38 - 0:40
    Two thousand?
  • 0:40 - 0:43
    When the psychologists Danny Kahneman
    and Amos Tversky tried this with
  • 0:43 - 0:45
    human subjects, subjects on average
  • 0:45 - 0:48
    guessed about two thousand
    two hundred and fifty.
  • 0:48 - 0:49
    Seems like an OK guess.
  • 0:49 - 0:53
    But now, let's suppose I gave you
    a different math problem.
  • 0:53 - 0:55
    What if I gave you this one?
  • 0:55 - 0:56
    Ready?
  • 0:56 - 1:00
    One times two times three times four
  • 1:00 - 1:05
    times five times six times
    seven times eight.
  • 1:05 - 1:06
    What's your answer?
  • 1:06 - 1:08
    If you're like Kahneman and Tversky's
  • 1:08 - 1:11
    subjects, your answer might
    be a bit different here.
  • 1:11 - 1:14
    For this question, their subjects
    guessed a lot lower.
  • 1:14 - 1:17
    On average they said the answer
    was about five hundred and twelve.
  • 1:17 - 1:20
    The first amazing thing
    about these similar
  • 1:20 - 1:24
    mathematical estimates is that people get
    the answers really, really wrong.
  • 1:24 - 1:25
    In fact, the real answer?
  • 1:25 - 1:29
    Well, for both, its forty thousand
    three hundred and twenty.
  • 1:29 - 1:32
    People are off by an order of magnitude.
  • 1:32 - 1:35
    But the second, even more amazing
    thing is that people give
  • 1:35 - 1:40
    different answers to the two problems,
    even though they're just different ways
  • 1:40 - 1:42
    of asking exactly the same question.
  • 1:42 - 1:44
    Why do we give completely different answers,
  • 1:44 - 1:47
    when the same math problem
    is presented differently?
  • 1:47 - 1:50
    The answer lies in how we make estimates.
  • 1:50 - 1:52
    When you have lots of time to do a math
  • 1:52 - 1:56
    problem, like eight times seven times six
    times five times four times three times
  • 1:56 - 1:59
    two times one, you can multiply all of
  • 1:59 - 2:01
    the numbers together and get an exact product.
  • 2:01 - 2:03
    But when you have to do the problem
  • 2:03 - 2:05
    quickly, you don't really have time to finish.
  • 2:05 - 2:07
    So you start with the first numbers.
  • 2:07 - 2:10
    You multiply eight times
    seven, and get fifty-six.
  • 2:10 - 2:13
    And then you've gotta
    multiply that by six,
  • 2:13 - 2:17
    and, well, you're guessing the final
    number's gotta be pretty big, bigger than
  • 2:17 - 2:20
    fifty-six, like maybe two thousand or so.
  • 2:20 - 2:22
    But when you do the second problem, you start
  • 2:22 - 2:27
    with one times two, and, well, that's only
    two, and two times three's only six.
  • 2:27 - 2:29
    Your answer's gonna be pretty small,
  • 2:29 - 2:31
    maybe only like five hundred or so.
  • 2:31 - 2:34
    This process of guessing based on the first
  • 2:34 - 2:36
    number you see is what's
    known as "anchoring."
  • 2:36 - 2:38
    The first number we think of
  • 2:38 - 2:40
    when we do our estimate is the anchor.
  • 2:40 - 2:42
    And once we have an anchor in our head,
  • 2:42 - 2:45
    well, we sort of adjust
    as needed from there.
  • 2:45 - 2:49
    The problem is that our minds are biased
    not to adjust as much as we need to.
  • 2:49 - 2:52
    The anchors are cognitively really strong.
  • 2:52 - 2:55
    In the first, problem you probably
    started with fifty-six, and
  • 2:55 - 2:58
    then adjusted to an even
    bigger number from there.
  • 2:58 - 3:01
    And in the second problem, you started
    with six, and then adjusted from there.
  • 3:01 - 3:06
    The problem is that starting at different
    points leads to different final guesses.
  • 3:06 - 3:11
    Like real anchors, our estimated anchors
    kinda get us stuck in one spot.
  • 3:11 - 3:15
    We often fail to drag the anchor far
    enough to get to a correct answer.
  • 3:15 - 3:18
    Kahneman and Tversky discovered that this
  • 3:18 - 3:20
    sort of anchoring bias happens all the time,
  • 3:20 - 3:22
    even for anchors that are totally arbitrary.
  • 3:22 - 3:25
    For example, they asked
    people to spin a wheel with
  • 3:25 - 3:28
    numbers from one to a hundred,
    and then asked them to estimate
  • 3:28 - 3:32
    what percentage of countries in
    the United Nations are African.
  • 3:32 - 3:35
    People who spun a ten on
    the wheel estimated that
  • 3:35 - 3:37
    the number was about twenty-five percent.
  • 3:37 - 3:40
    But people who spun a
    sixty-five estimated that
  • 3:40 - 3:42
    the number was forty-five percent.
  • 3:42 - 3:46
    In another experiment, Dan Ariely
    and his colleagues had people
  • 3:46 - 3:49
    write down the last two digits
    of their social security number.
  • 3:49 - 3:51
    They were then asked whether they would
  • 3:51 - 3:54
    pay that amount in dollars
    for a nice bottle of wine.
  • 3:54 - 3:58
    Ariely and colleagues found that people
    in the highest quintile of social security
  • 3:58 - 4:03
    numbers would pay three to four times
    as much for the exact same good.
  • 4:03 - 4:05
    Just setting up a larger anchor can make a
  • 4:05 - 4:07
    person who would pay eight
    dollars for the bottle
  • 4:07 - 4:11
    of wine be willing to spend
    twenty-seven dollars instead.
  • 4:11 - 4:15
    Sadly for us, sales people use
    anchors against us all the time.
  • 4:15 - 4:18
    How many times have you noticed
    a salesperson or an advertisement
  • 4:18 - 4:21
    anchoring you to a particular price, or
  • 4:21 - 4:24
    even to how much of a particular
    product you should buy?
  • 4:24 - 4:26
    Whether it's buying a car, or a sweater,
  • 4:26 - 4:30
    or even renting a hotel room, our
    intuitions about what prices
  • 4:30 - 4:35
    are reasonable to pay often come
    from some arbitrary anchor.
  • 4:35 - 4:38
    So, the next time you're given an
    anchor, take a minute to think.
  • 4:38 - 4:40
    Remember what happens when you
  • 4:40 - 4:42
    drop your anger too high, and then
  • 4:42 - 4:45
    consider thinking of a
    very different number.
  • 4:45 - 4:49
    It might affect your final estimate
    more than you expect.
Title:
CRITICAL THINKING - Cognitive Biases: Anchoring [HD]
Description:

In this video, the cognitive scientist Laurie Santos (Yale University) explains the phenomenon of anchoring. She shows how arbitrary information sometimes can sometimes act as an anchor that affects our judgments in unexpected ways.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Duration:
04:54

English subtitles

Revisions