< Return to Video

Animation basics: The optical illusion of motion - TED-Ed

  • 0:15 - 0:18
    Take a series of still, sequential images.
  • 0:18 - 0:20
    Let's look at them one by one.
  • 0:24 - 0:25
    Faster.
  • 0:28 - 0:30
    Now, let's remove the gaps,
  • 0:30 - 0:31
    go faster still.
  • 0:33 - 0:34
    Wait for it ...
  • 0:36 - 0:38
    Bam!
  • 0:38 - 0:39
    Motion!
  • 0:40 - 0:41
    Why is that?
  • 0:41 - 0:45
    Intellectually, we know we're just looking
    at a series of still images,
  • 0:45 - 0:46
    but when we see them change fast enough,
  • 0:47 - 0:50
    they produce the optical illusion
    of appearing as a single, persistent image
  • 0:50 - 0:52
    that's gradually changing
    form and position.
  • 0:53 - 0:56
    This effect is the basis
    for all motion picture technology,
  • 0:56 - 0:58
    from our LED screens of today
  • 0:58 - 1:01
    to their 20th-century
    cathode ray forebearers,
  • 1:01 - 1:04
    from cinematic film projection
    to the novelty toy,
  • 1:04 - 1:07
    even, it's been suggested,
    all the way back to the Stone Age
  • 1:07 - 1:09
    when humans began painting on cave walls.
  • 1:10 - 1:13
    This phenomenon of perceiving
    apparent motion in successive images
  • 1:13 - 1:15
    is due to a characteristic
    of human perception
  • 1:15 - 1:19
    historically referred
    to as "persistence of vision."
  • 1:19 - 1:20
    The term is attributed
  • 1:20 - 1:23
    to the English-Swiss physicist
    Peter Mark Roget,
  • 1:23 - 1:24
    who, in the early 19th century,
  • 1:24 - 1:27
    used it to describe
    a particular defect of the eye
  • 1:27 - 1:29
    that resulted in a moving object
  • 1:29 - 1:32
    appearing to be still
    when it reached a certain speed.
  • 1:32 - 1:36
    Not long after, the term
    was applied to describe the opposite,
  • 1:36 - 1:38
    the apparent motion of still images,
  • 1:38 - 1:42
    by Belgian physicist Joseph Plateau,
    inventor of the phenakistoscope.
  • 1:42 - 1:46
    He defined persistence of vision
    as the result of successive afterimages,
  • 1:46 - 1:49
    which were retained
    and then combined in the retina,
  • 1:49 - 1:53
    making us believe that what we were seeing
    is a single object in motion.
  • 1:53 - 1:56
    This explanation was widely accepted
    in the decades to follow
  • 1:56 - 1:58
    and up through the turn
    of the 20th century,
  • 1:58 - 2:01
    when some began to question
    what was physiologically going on.
  • 2:01 - 2:04
    In 1912, German psychologist
    Max Wertheimer
  • 2:04 - 2:07
    outlined the basic primary stages
    of apparent motion
  • 2:07 - 2:09
    using simple optical illusions.
  • 2:09 - 2:11
    These experiments led him to conclude
  • 2:11 - 2:15
    the phenomenon was due to processes
    which lie behind the retina.
  • 2:16 - 2:18
    In 1915, Hugo Münsterberg,
  • 2:18 - 2:20
    a German-American pioneer
    in applied psychology,
  • 2:20 - 2:23
    also suggested that the apparent motion
    of successive images
  • 2:23 - 2:25
    is not due to their being
    retained in the eye,
  • 2:26 - 2:28
    but is superadded
    by the action of the mind.
  • 2:30 - 2:32
    In the century to follow,
    experiments by physiologists
  • 2:32 - 2:34
    have pretty much confirmed
    their conclusions.
  • 2:35 - 2:37
    As it relates to the illusion
    of motion pictures,
  • 2:37 - 2:40
    persistence of vision
    has less to do with vision itself
  • 2:40 - 2:42
    than how it's interpreted in the brain.
  • 2:42 - 2:45
    Research has shown that different aspects
    of what the eye sees,
  • 2:45 - 2:49
    like form, color, depth, and motion,
  • 2:49 - 2:51
    are transmitted to different areas
    of the visual cortex
  • 2:51 - 2:53
    via different pathways from the retina.
  • 2:53 - 2:55
    It's the continuous interaction
  • 2:55 - 2:57
    of various computations
    in the visual cortex
  • 2:57 - 2:59
    that stitch those different
    aspects together
  • 2:59 - 3:01
    and culminate in the perception.
  • 3:01 - 3:03
    Our brains are constantly working,
  • 3:03 - 3:06
    synchronizing what we see,
    hear, smell, and touch
  • 3:06 - 3:07
    into meaningful experience
  • 3:07 - 3:10
    in the moment-to-moment
    flow of the present.
  • 3:10 - 3:13
    So, in order to create the illusion
    of motion in successive images,
  • 3:13 - 3:15
    we need to get the timing of our intervals
  • 3:15 - 3:18
    close to the speed at which our brains
    process the present.
  • 3:19 - 3:22
    So, how fast is the present
    happening according to our brains?
  • 3:22 - 3:23
    Well, we can get an idea
  • 3:23 - 3:26
    by measuring how fast
    the images need to be changing
  • 3:26 - 3:27
    for the illusion to work.
  • 3:27 - 3:30
    Let's see if we can figure it out
    by repeating our experiment.
  • 3:30 - 3:34
    Here's the sequence presented
    at a rate of one frame per two seconds
  • 3:34 - 3:36
    with one second of black in between.
  • 3:36 - 3:38
    At this rate of change,
  • 3:38 - 3:40
    with the blank space
    separating the images,
  • 3:40 - 3:42
    there's no real motion perceptible.
  • 3:42 - 3:44
    As we lessen the duration of blank space,
  • 3:44 - 3:47
    a slight change in position
    becomes more apparent,
  • 3:47 - 3:49
    and you start to get an inkling
    of a sense of motion
  • 3:50 - 3:51
    between the disparate frames.
  • 3:51 - 3:53
    One frame per second.
  • 3:56 - 3:57
    Two frames per second.
  • 3:59 - 4:01
    Four frames per second.
  • 4:02 - 4:05
    Now we're starting
    to get a feeling of motion,
  • 4:05 - 4:07
    but it's really not very smooth.
  • 4:07 - 4:10
    We're still aware of the fact
    that we're looking at separate images.
  • 4:10 - 4:12
    Let's speed up. Eight frames per second.
  • 4:14 - 4:16
    12 frames per second.
  • 4:17 - 4:18
    It looks like we're about there.
  • 4:21 - 4:25
    At 24 frames per second,
    the motion looks even smoother.
  • 4:25 - 4:27
    This is standard full speed.
  • 4:28 - 4:31
    So, the point at which we lose
    awareness of the intervals
  • 4:31 - 4:32
    and begin to see apparent motion
  • 4:32 - 4:35
    seems to kick in at around
    eight to 12 frames per second.
  • 4:36 - 4:39
    This is in the neighborhood
    of what science has determined
  • 4:39 - 4:41
    to be the general threshold
    of our awareness
  • 4:41 - 4:42
    of seeing separate images.
  • 4:42 - 4:45
    Generally speaking,
    we being to lose that awareness
  • 4:45 - 4:47
    at intervals of around
    100 milliseconds per image,
  • 4:47 - 4:50
    which is equal to a frame rate
    of around ten frames per second.
  • 4:50 - 4:52
    As the frame rate increases,
  • 4:52 - 4:54
    we lose awareness
    of the intervals completely
  • 4:54 - 4:57
    and are all the more convinced
    of the reality of the illusion.
Title:
Animation basics: The optical illusion of motion - TED-Ed
Description:

View full lesson: http://ed.ted.com/lessons/animation-basics-the-optical-illusion-of-motion-ted-ed

How do animators make still images come to life? Are the images really moving, or are they merely an optical illusion? TED-Ed takes you behind the scenes to reveal the secret of motion in movies.

Lesson and animation by TED-Ed.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TED-Ed
Duration:
05:12

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions